Next Article in Journal
Non-Predictive Model-Free Control of Nonlinear Systems with Unknown Input Time Delay
Next Article in Special Issue
Probing Intrinsic Neural Timescales in EEG with an Information-Theory Inspired Approach: Permutation Entropy Time Delay Estimation (PE-TD)
Previous Article in Journal
Young’s Experiment with Entangled Bipartite Systems: The Role of Underlying Quantum Velocity Fields
Previous Article in Special Issue
Entropy and Cross-Level Orderliness in Light of the Interconnection between the Neural System and Consciousness
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Temporospatial Nestedness in Consciousness: An Updated Perspective on the Temporospatial Theory of Consciousness

1
Department of Anesthesiology, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
2
Center for Consciousness Science, University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, USA
Entropy 2023, 25(7), 1074; https://doi.org/10.3390/e25071074
Submission received: 24 May 2023 / Revised: 20 June 2023 / Accepted: 27 June 2023 / Published: 17 July 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Temporo-Spatial Theory of Consciousness (TTC))

Abstract

:
Time and space are fundamental elements that permeate the fabric of nature, and their significance in relation to neural activity and consciousness remains a compelling yet unexplored area of research. The Temporospatial Theory of Consciousness (TTC) provides a framework that links time, space, neural activity, and consciousness, shedding light on the intricate relationships among these dimensions. In this review, I revisit the fundamental concepts and mechanisms proposed by the TTC, with a particular focus on the central concept of temporospatial nestedness. I propose an extension of temporospatial nestedness by incorporating the nested relationship between the temporal circuit and functional geometry of the brain. To further unravel the complexities of temporospatial nestedness, future research directions should emphasize the characterization of functional geometry and the temporal circuit across multiple spatial and temporal scales. Investigating the links between these scales will yield a more comprehensive understanding of how spatial organization and temporal dynamics contribute to conscious states. This integrative approach holds the potential to uncover novel insights into the neural basis of consciousness and reshape our understanding of the world–brain dynamic.

1. Time and Space in the Brain

Consciousness, a multifaceted and enigmatic phenomenon, has been the subject of exploration from various perspectives. Unraveling the neurobiological underpinnings of consciousness holds great significance for multiple medical disciplines such as neurology, psychiatry, and anesthesiology. The field has witnessed substantial advancements in the past two decades, thanks to various theoretical breakthroughs that have propelled our understanding forward [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16].
Time and space serve as fundamental elements in the fabric of nature. Similarly, within the realm of the brain’s neural activity, these elements manifest and propagate in their intrinsic forms [17]. Despite extensive investigations of time and space in the field of physics, their significance in relation to neural activity and consciousness remains largely unexplored. Drawing upon various lines of empirical evidence, Dr. Northoff and I have formulated the Temporospatial Theory of Consciousness (TTC) [18]. By elucidating the intricate relationships among time, space, neural activity, and consciousness, the TTC offers a framework that links various types of neural activity with different aspects of consciousness [18,19,20]. In this article, I will revisit the fundamental concepts and mechanisms proposed by the TTC and expand on its central concept of temporospatial nestedness. To achieve this, I will update the definition of consciousness from a multidimensional perspective and incorporate recent research on the spatial rules of brain activity origination and the temporal regularities of brain activity.
When we refer to space and time in the brain, we are talking about the extension of neural activity across different hierarchical spatial scales, ranging from neuronal layers and regions to functional networks, as well as the duration of neural activity embedded in hierarchical timescales [17]. In the visual system, for example, there are fine-grained projections from the retina to the cortex, and different neurons have their own preference for object orientation [21,22,23]. This information is integrated through different spatial scales. Likewise, in the context of receiving sensory stimuli across time, such as listening to a story, our brain processes the information by employing distinct temporal receptive windows [24]. For instance, words are processed within the span of hundreds of milliseconds, sentences within a few seconds, and paragraphs within tens of seconds [25,26]. Brain regions characterized by higher-frequency fluctuations play a role in processing swift sensory information, whereas regions exhibiting dominance in low frequencies are involved in integrating perceptual and cognitive events that unfold over longer durations [24,25,26,27]. By exploring these temporal and spatial aspects of the brain, the TTC provides a unique perspective on the nature of consciousness and its neural underpinnings. Four mechanisms were proposed to account for different aspects of consciousness involving the temporospatial nestedness in spontaneous brain activity, the temporospatial alignment during the interactions between pre- and post-stimulus-induced activity, the temporospatial expansion of early stimulus-induced activity, and the temporospatial globalization of late stimulus-induced activity.

2. Four Mechanisms in the TTC

Mechanism 1, temporospatial nestedness (Figure 1), refers to the systematic cross-frequency coupling [28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38] and hierarchical intrinsic timescales in the brain’s temporal domain [20,27,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55], as well as the hierarchical organization across brain regions in the spatial domain [49,56,57,58,59,60]. These dynamic, complex, and flexible temporospatial configurations are crucial in generating our rich phenomenal experiences [17]. Certain changes in the cross-frequency relationship [61,62,63,64], intrinsic timescales [65,66], or functional network hierarchy [67,68] may lead to a loss of consciousness.
The first instance of Mechanism 2, temporospatial alignment (Figure 2), is single-stimulus alignment. The concept behind this is that a stimulus needs to be presented at the appropriate moment for it to interact with the spontaneous activity of the brain, enabling us to perceive it. For example, if sub-threshold sounds are presented, sometimes we can hear them, and sometimes we cannot. This is because the spontaneous activity has an impact on our perception. If the stimulus arrives during the preferred phases of spontaneous activity, we are able to detect it, but not during other phases [69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82]. Another example is sequential temporal alignment in spatially distributed functional areas (see also a three-layer temporal model of alignment in [83]), which we previously discussed regarding temporal receptive windows. A third example is long-term alignment across the lifespan, and the key takeaway is that the alignment between environment-related signals and organism-related signals enables the organism to model the relationship between itself and the world [16,18,83]. The Integrated World Modeling Theory (IWMT) [84,85] has provided a sophisticated elaboration of this notion, proposing that consciousness arises from generative processes that integrate information into coherent models encompassing space, time, and causal relationships between systems and their environments. The IWMT shows promise in providing a mechanistic understanding of temporospatial alignment through its thorough integration with computational models.
Mechanism 3, temporospatial expansion (Figure 3), proposes that the brain’s neural activity needs to expand both in time and space for conscious experience to occur. The idea is that when we are presented with a sensory stimulus, such as a picture of a face, the neural activity initially activates low-level sensory areas, like the primary visual cortex, but we are not yet able to perceive anything. As the activity expands in time and space to a certain level, we begin to have conscious experience or qualia. Studies using techniques like transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and electroencephalography (EEG) support the notion that the conscious brain propagates the stimulus-evoked activity both in time and space [86], while the unconscious brain may lose either the spatial expansion or temporal expansion or both. Another example of temporospatial expansion is seen in the fact that the duration and extension of a stimulus in our conscious experience usually last longer and extend beyond its physical duration and extension [87]. For instance, when we watch a movie, there are temporal gaps between frames and spatial gaps between pixels, yet we still have a unified experience. The temporospatial expansion mechanism may provide a “glue” between the stimulus and the brain, yielding the continuity of our phenomenal experience. The idea of temporospatial expansion is also compatible with the posterior hot zone hypothesis [6,88], which suggests that when the expansion passes a threshold both in time and space in the posterior part of our brain, we become capable of having phenomenal experiences.
Mechanism 4, temporospatial globalization (Figure 4), is the process by which the temporal and spatial aspects of neural activity become globally available for report, resulting in access consciousness [3,89,90]. In other words, once the temporospatial expansion is globalized, involving the lateral frontoparietal loops, conscious content can be accessed and reported. This mechanism is predicted by the Global Neuronal Workspace Theory [15,91], which suggests that conscious information is made globally available when it is broadcast to a large network of neurons in the brain. This broadcasting is thought to be facilitated by neural signatures such as the P3b and non-linear ignition [91,92,93]. The concept of temporospatial globalization complements the Global Neuronal Workspace Theory by adding the dimension of temporal globalization on top of cognitive and neuronal space.
To this end, I have reviewed the four mechanisms of consciousness proposed by TTC, and I argue that Mechanism 1 serves as the foundational building blocks of consciousness that enable the other mechanisms to operate (for a similar view, see [17]). Moving forward, the focus will be on expanding upon the central concept of the TTC, temporospatial nestedness, which is key to understanding the state of consciousness. This will involve updating the definition of consciousness from a multidimensional perspective and incorporating recent studies on the spatial organization and temporal regularities of brain activity. The goal is to gain a deeper understanding of the relationship between consciousness and the brain’s temporospatial patterns, and how alterations in these patterns relate to changes in consciousness.

3. Dimensions of Consciousness

For the past two decades, scientists have emphasized the need to identify key “dimensions” of consciousness. In the field of anesthesiology, consciousness is typically viewed as a variable that changes along a single dimension that is often equated with the level of arousal [95]. In neurology, consciousness is typically viewed as consisting of two dimensions: (1) awareness of the environment and self (also known as the content of consciousness); and (2) wakefulness (also known as the level of consciousness) [96]. While these two dimensions typically coexist, they can become dissociated in everyday situations (e.g., sleep) or under the influence of pharmacological or pathological conditions [97]. For example, patients with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome (vegetative state) have their eyes wide open but are presumably unaware of themselves and their surroundings. Therefore, their condition is considered wakefulness without awareness. This awareness versus wakefulness scheme was later expanded by adding a behavioral dimension (i.e., the ability to produce motor response) [98], which allows for a cognitive–motor dissociation [99] or covert consciousness [100] that occurs in some neuropathological patients who are behaviorally non-responsive. Although these approaches to defining consciousness along multiple cognitive–behavioral dimensions are useful for the clinical diagnosis of neuropathological patients, they controversially assume that normal conscious wakefulness is associated with the highest level of awareness, wakefulness, and responsiveness. Altered states of consciousness, such as those induced by psychedelic drugs or psychiatric causes (e.g., schizophrenia), are difficult to fit into such a scheme, as these patients can be fully aware, awake, and responsive but with even richer conscious experiences than in a drug-free state [101].
In recent years, additional dimensions of consciousness have been proposed, including cognitive dimensions like attention control, sensory processing, executive function, and meta-awareness [102,103,104,105,106]. Despite their theoretical appeal, these dimensions have yet to be precisely linked to neural activity and function in the brain. Although research has extensively examined the neural activity associated with wakefulness and awareness, it has not been performed within the context of mapping a single conceptual dimension of consciousness to a measurable neural variable. One factor contributing to this lack of understanding is the failure of traditional neuroimaging localization approaches to connect specific brain regions with exclusive functions. For instance, the prefrontal cortex serves several functions, such as working memory, decision making, attention, and task control [107]. Additionally, a single brain network can be implicated in multiple cognitive processes at the system level [108]. Therefore, brain regions and networks do not correspond to unique neurofunctional dimensions. In the quest for the neural correlates of consciousness [6], researchers have generally examined distinct brain regions instead of continua reflecting the brain’s intrinsic functional geometry.

4. Functional Geometry—Spatial Organization Rules

Recent advances in neuroimaging offer a new approach that can be applied to investigate the neural dimensions of consciousness by representing the brain’s functional geometry in terms of its cortical gradients [56,109]. These gradients encompass various functions and networks along a continuum that ranges from perception and action to abstract cognitive functions [56,110,111,112]. This functional spectrum is represented by the unimodal to transmodal gradient (Gradient-1), which facilitates the integration of sensory signals with non-sensory information and transforms them into abstract contents. The visual to somatomotor gradient (Gradient-2) represents the functional specialization of different sensory modalities. The task-negative to task-positive gradient (Gradient-3) represents a functional differentiation extending from areas that are often deactivated during task performance to those activated during tasks in frontoparietal and attention networks [113,114]. The cortical gradient mapping approach allows for a more comprehensive understanding of the neural dimensions of consciousness by assessing the role of the topographical continuum, which is analogous to describing a region along several topographical axes related to the slope of elevation or spatial change in vegetation types (Figure 5).
Using cortical gradient mapping, recent research has shown that the three cortical gradients (Gradient-1, Gradient-2, and Gradient-3) may represent the neurofunctional dimensions of consciousness, such as awareness, sensory organization, and cortical arousability [68]. The study found that these gradients selectively changed in various pharmacologically altered states [68]. For example, the degradation of Gradient-1 was observed in deep sedation or general anesthesia induced by propofol, indicating a loss of awareness. However, the degradation of Gradient-1 was less pronounced during ketamine anesthesia, suggesting partial preservation of inner awareness, such as dream-like experiences. In contrast, ketamine anesthesia resulted in a significant degradation of Gradient-2, indicating increased visual-somatosensory crosstalk, which is a common sensory disorganization effect of psychoactive drugs. Finally, Gradient-3 degraded when subjects were no longer arousable by painful glabellum stimulation during propofol infusion at a dose sufficient for general anesthesia. Taken together, these findings provide a brain-based understanding of consciousness as a multidimensional phenomenon (Figure 6) and offer a theoretical and empirical framework for unraveling the complex brain mechanisms underlying consciousness.

5. Temporal Circuit—Regularities in Time

The brain is in a constant state of flux, continually altering its functional connections and evolving over time. In a recent study, researchers employed an unsupervised machine learning technique called k-means clustering to examine moment-to-moment brain activity, identifying eight distinct co-activation patterns (CAPs) [115]. Two of these CAPs correspond to the default mode and dorsal attention networks, while the remaining six are associated with other well-known networks involved in various brain functions, including the frontoparietal, sensory and motor, visual, and ventral attention networks, as well as two networks representing cross-brain states of activation and deactivation. Interestingly, these eight CAPs form four pairs of “mirror” motifs, with a strong negative spatial similarity between each pair (Figure 7).
Furthermore, the work reveals how the push–pull relationship between the default mode and dorsal attention networks may differ between conscious and unconscious individuals [115]. In the conscious brain, the dynamic switching of networks including the default mode and dorsal attention networks occurs along a set of structured transition trajectories, which can be considered a “temporal circuit”. Disruption of this circuit, resulting in limited access to these networks, is a common characteristic of unconsciousness resulting from various causes. In a subsequent study [116], it was found that the ventral attention network plays a crucial role in modulating the transitions between the default mode and dorsal attention networks, which aligns with the triple network hypothesis [117,118]. Furthermore, the ventral attention network operates at short timescales [49,119], facilitating rapid detection and orientation toward salient stimuli [120]. The dorsal attention network, on the other hand, operates at medium timescales [49,119], enabling sustained attention and cognitive control [120,121]. Lastly, the default mode network exhibits temporal dynamics in long timescales that are involved in long-term memory, imagination, and self-referential processes [49,119,122,123]. By considering these nested temporal scales of the network interactions within the triple network hypothesis, we gain a better understanding of the distinct temporal characteristics and functions of each network.
Why are the default mode and dorsal attention networks important to consciousness? They play a crucial role as they correspond to our inward focus on ourselves and outward focus on the environment, respectively [120,121,123,124,125,126,127]. In the conscious brain, these two systems are in a dynamic balance, sliding back and forth, but both are present to some extent. The temporal circuit hypothesis posits that consciousness emerges from the reciprocal balance between these two opposing cortical systems, embedded in the spatiotemporal dynamics of neural activity. To be conscious, an agent must accurately represent its environment and also represent itself in relation to aspects of the world, either external or internal. This is why the self is crucial in the emergence of consciousness, as it is the subject that is aware of the environment, and without a subject that is aware, there is no consciousness [84,85,128,129,130,131]. The dynamic relation between the self and the environment, mediated by the default mode and dorsal attention networks, may enable consciousness to emerge.

6. Temporal Circuit Nested within Functional Geometry—Temporospatial Nestedness

Recent research suggests that the spatial and temporal properties of brain activity are interdependent and cannot be fully understood in isolation. Studies show that transient fMRI co-activations propagate as waves along cortical gradients [132,133,134,135], indicating that temporal dynamics are likely constrained by cortical gradients. Therefore, understanding the covariation between spatial and temporal dimensions can provide greater insight into the neural basis of consciousness. The aforementioned three cortical gradients represent major neurofunctional dimensions of consciousness, and together create a three-dimensional gradient space. Functional brain networks occupy distinct positions in this space, and their functional geometry, such as distance, can be quantified. The network’s functional geometry and the temporal occurrence rates of corresponding co-activation patterns were found to covary with the state of consciousness [68]. For example, the degradation of functional gradients is linked to the disruption of the temporal circuit in depressed states of consciousness (Figure 8). These findings provide insight into the interplay between the brain’s functional geometry and its temporal dynamics, highlighting their potential nested relationship. This notion of nestedness, which is a core concept in the TTC, can be extended by including both spatial (i.e., functional geometry) and temporal (e.g., temporal circuit) dimensions of consciousness.

7. Concluding Remarks

The TTC offers a unique perspective on the nature of consciousness and its neural correlates by exploring the temporal and spatial dimensions of the brain’s neural activity. With its framework, the TTC provides insights into various aspects of consciousness. One of its key concepts, temporospatial nestedness, can be extended by including a nested relationship between the functional geometry and the temporal circuit of the brain. To gain a better understanding of this relationship, future research should focus on characterizing functional geometry and the temporal circuit across multiple spatial and temporal scales, as well as investigating their links across scales. By doing so, we can achieve a more comprehensive understanding of temporospatial nestedness and possibly uncover new insights into the neural basis of consciousness. Together, the TTC opens up new avenues of research, paving the way for innovative investigations into the interplay between the brain and consciousness and fostering a deeper appreciation for the world–brain dynamic.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health grants R01-GM103894 (principal investigator: Anthony G. Hudetz) and T32-GM103730 (principal investigator: George A. Mashour). The content is solely the responsibility of the author and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

No new data were created.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Aru, J.; Suzuki, M.; Larkum, M.E. Cellular Mechanisms of Conscious Processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020, 24, 814–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Alkire, M.T.; Hudetz, A.G.; Tononi, G. Consciousness and Anesthesia. Science 2008, 322, 876–880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Baars, B.J. The Conscious Access Hypothesis: Origins and Recent Evidence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002, 6, 47–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Seth, A.K.; Bayne, T. Theories of Consciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2022, 23, 439–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bayne, T.; Seth, A.K.; Massimini, M. From Complexity to Consciousness. Trends Neurosci. 2020, 43, 546–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Koch, C.; Massimini, M.; Boly, M.; Tononi, G. Neural Correlates of Consciousness: Progress and Problems. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016, 17, 307–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Rees, G.; Kreiman, G.; Koch, C. Neural Correlates of Consciousness in Humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 3, 261–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Tononi, G.; Boly, M.; Massimini, M.; Koch, C. Integrated Information Theory: From Consciousness to Its Physical Substrate. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016, 17, 450–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Tononi, G.; Koch, C. The Neural Correlates of Consciousness. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1124, 239–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Tsuchiya, N.; Wilke, M.; Frässle, S.; Lamme, V.A.F. No-Report Paradigms: Extracting the True Neural Correlates of Consciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015, 19, 757–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Mashour, G.A.; Hudetz, A.G. Neural Correlates of Unconsciousness in Large-Scale Brain Networks. Trends Neurosci. 2018, 41, 150–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Brown, R.; Lau, H.; LeDoux, J.E. Understanding the Higher-Order Approach to Consciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019, 23, 754–768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lau, H.; Rosenthal, D. Empirical Support for Higher-Order Theories of Conscious Awareness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 365–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Lamme, V.A.F. Towards a True Neural Stance on Consciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006, 10, 494–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Dehaene, S.; Changeux, J.-P. Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing. Neuron 2011, 70, 200–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Northoff, G.; Wainio-Theberge, S.; Evers, K. Is Temporo-Spatial Dynamics the “Common Currency” of Brain and Mind? In Quest of “Spatiotemporal Neuroscience”. Phys. Life Rev. 2020, 33, 34–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Fingelkurts, A.A.; Fingelkurts, A.A.; Neves, C.F.H. Natural World Physical, Brain Operational, and Mind Phenomenal Space–Time. Phys. Life Rev. 2010, 7, 195–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Northoff, G.; Huang, Z. How Do the Brain’s Time and Space Mediate Consciousness and Its Different Dimensions? Temporo-Spatial Theory of Consciousness (TTC). Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 80, 630–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Northoff, G.; Zilio, F. Temporo-Spatial Theory of Consciousness (TTC)—Bridging the Gap of Neuronal Activity and Phenomenal States. Behav. Brain Res. 2022, 424, 113788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Northoff, G.; Zilio, F. From Shorter to Longer Timescales: Converging Integrated Information Theory (IIT) with the Temporo-Spatial Theory of Consciousness (TTC). Entropy 2022, 24, 270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hubel, D.H.; Wiesel, T.N. Receptive Fields, Binocular Interaction and Functional Architecture in the Cat’s Visual Cortex. J. Physiol. 1962, 160, 106–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Greene, C.A.; Dumoulin, S.O.; Harvey, B.M.; Ress, D. Measurement of Population Receptive Fields in Human Early Visual Cortex Using Back-Projection Tomography. J. Vis. 2014, 14, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Gupta, P.; Markan, C.M. An Adaptable Neuromorphic Model of Orientation Selectivity Based on Floating Gate Dynamics. Front. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Hasson, U.; Yang, E.; Vallines, I.; Heeger, D.J.; Rubin, N. A Hierarchy of Temporal Receptive Windows in Human Cortex. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 2539–2550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Hasson, U.; Chen, J.; Honey, C.J. Hierarchical Process Memory: Memory as an Integral Component of Information Processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015, 19, 304–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Lerner, Y.; Honey, C.J.; Silbert, L.J.; Hasson, U. Topographic Mapping of a Hierarchy of Temporal Receptive Windows Using a Narrated Story. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 2906–2915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Honey, C.J.; Thesen, T.; Donner, T.H.; Silbert, L.J.; Carlson, C.E.; Devinsky, O.; Doyle, W.K.; Rubin, N.; Heeger, D.J.; Hasson, U. Slow Cortical Dynamics and the Accumulation of Information over Long Timescales. Neuron 2012, 76, 423–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Jirsa, V.; Müller, V. Cross-Frequency Coupling in Real and Virtual Brain Networks. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Weaver, K.E.; Wander, J.D.; Ko, A.L.; Casimo, K.; Grabowski, T.J.; Ojemann, J.G.; Darvas, F. Directional Patterns of Cross Frequency Phase and Amplitude Coupling within the Resting State Mimic Patterns of FMRI Functional Connectivity. Neuroimage 2016, 128, 238–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Wang, L.; Saalmann, Y.B.; Pinsk, M.A.; Arcaro, M.J.; Kastner, S. Electrophysiological Low-Frequency Coherence and Cross-Frequency Coupling Contribute to BOLD Connectivity. Neuron 2012, 76, 1010–1020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Hyafil, A.; Giraud, A.-L.; Fontolan, L.; Gutkin, B. Neural Cross-Frequency Coupling: Connecting Architectures, Mechanisms, and Functions. Trends Neurosci. 2015, 38, 725–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Qin, Y.; Menara, T.; Bassett, D.S.; Pasqualetti, F. Phase-Amplitude Coupling in Neuronal Oscillator Networks. Phys. Rev. Res. 2021, 3, 023218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Akao, A.; Shirasaka, S.; Jimbo, Y.; Ermentrout, B.; Kotani, K. Theta-Gamma Cross-Frequency Coupling Enables Covariance between Distant Brain Regions. arXiv 2019, arXiv:1903.12155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Sotero, R.C. Topology, Cross-Frequency, and Same-Frequency Band Interactions Shape the Generation of Phase-Amplitude Coupling in a Neural Mass Model of a Cortical Column. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2016, 12, e1005180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. He, B.J.; Zempel, J.M.; Snyder, A.Z.; Raichle, M.E. The Temporal Structures and Functional Significance of Scale-Free Brain Activity. Neuron 2010, 66, 353–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Siebenhühner, F.; Wang, S.H.; Arnulfo, G.; Lampinen, A.; Nobili, L.; Palva, J.M.; Palva, S. Genuine Cross-Frequency Coupling Networks in Human Resting-State Electrophysiological Recordings. PLoS Biol. 2020, 18, e3000685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Tort, A.B.L.; Kramer, M.A.; Thorn, C.; Gibson, D.J.; Kubota, Y.; Graybiel, A.M.; Kopell, N.J. Dynamic Cross-Frequency Couplings of Local Field Potential Oscillations in Rat Striatum and Hippocampus during Performance of a T-Maze Task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 20517–20522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Engel, A.K.; Gerloff, C.; Hilgetag, C.C.; Nolte, G. Intrinsic Coupling Modes: Multiscale Interactions in Ongoing Brain Activity. Neuron 2013, 80, 867–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Chaudhuri, R.; Knoblauch, K.; Gariel, M.-A.; Kennedy, H.; Wang, X.-J. A Large-Scale Circuit Mechanism for Hierarchical Dynamical Processing in the Primate Cortex. Neuron 2015, 88, 419–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Chialvo, D.R. Emergent Complex Neural Dynamics. Nat. Phys. 2010, 6, 744–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Farbood, M.M.; Heeger, D.J.; Marcus, G.; Hasson, U.; Lerner, Y. The Neural Processing of Hierarchical Structure in Music and Speech at Different Timescales. Front. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Gao, R.; van den Brink, R.L.; Pfeffer, T.; Voytek, B. Neuronal Timescales Are Functionally Dynamic and Shaped by Cortical Microarchitecture. Elife 2020, 9, e61277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Golesorkhi, M.; Gomez-Pilar, J.; Zilio, F.; Berberian, N.; Wolff, A.; Yagoub, M.C.E.; Northoff, G. The Brain and Its Time: Intrinsic Neural Timescales Are Key for Input Processing. Commun. Biol. 2021, 4, 970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Kiebel, S.J.; Daunizeau, J.; Friston, K.J. A Hierarchy of Time-Scales and the Brain. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2008, 4, e1000209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Klar, P.; Çatal, Y.; Langner, R.; Huang, Z.; Northoff, G. Scale-free Dynamics of Core-periphery Topography. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2023, 44, 1997–2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Manea, A.M.; Zilverstand, A.; Ugurbil, K.; Heilbronner, S.R.; Zimmermann, J. Intrinsic Timescales as an Organizational Principle of Neural Processing across the Whole Rhesus Macaque Brain. Elife 2022, 11, e75540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Murray, J.D.; Bernacchia, A.; Freedman, D.J.; Romo, R.; Wallis, J.D.; Cai, X.; Padoa-Schioppa, C.; Pasternak, T.; Seo, H.; Lee, D.; et al. A Hierarchy of Intrinsic Timescales across Primate Cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2014, 17, 1661–1663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Nanda, A.; Johnson, G.W.; Mu, Y.; Ahrens, M.B.; Chang, C.; Englot, D.J.; Breakspear, M.; Rubinov, M. Time-Resolved Correlation of Distributed Brain Activity Tracks E-I Balance and Accounts for Diverse Scale-Free Phenomena. Cell. Rep. 2023, 42, 112254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Raut, R.V.; Snyder, A.Z.; Raichle, M.E. Hierarchical Dynamics as a Macroscopic Organizing Principle of the Human Brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 20890–20897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Runyan, C.A.; Piasini, E.; Panzeri, S.; Harvey, C.D. Distinct Timescales of Population Coding across Cortex. Nature 2017, 548, 92–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Varela, F.; Lachaux, J.-P.; Rodriguez, E.; Martinerie, J. The Brainweb: Phase Synchronization and Large-Scale Integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 2, 229–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Wang, X.-J. Macroscopic Gradients of Synaptic Excitation and Inhibition in the Neocortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2020, 21, 169–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Wolff, A.; Berberian, N.; Golesorkhi, M.; Gomez-Pilar, J.; Zilio, F.; Northoff, G. Intrinsic Neural Timescales: Temporal Integration and Segregation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2022, 26, 159–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Yeshurun, Y.; Nguyen, M.; Hasson, U. Amplification of Local Changes along the Timescale Processing Hierarchy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, 9475–9480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Zilio, F.; Gomez-Pilar, J.; Cao, S.; Zhang, J.; Zang, D.; Qi, Z.; Tan, J.; Hiromi, T.; Wu, X.; Fogel, S.; et al. Are Intrinsic Neural Timescales Related to Sensory Processing? Evidence from Abnormal Behavioral States. Neuroimage 2021, 226, 117579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Margulies, D.S.; Ghosh, S.S.; Goulas, A.; Falkiewicz, M.; Huntenburg, J.M.; Langs, G.; Bezgin, G.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Castellanos, F.X.; Petrides, M.; et al. Situating the Default-Mode Network along a Principal Gradient of Macroscale Cortical Organization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, 12574–12579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Burt, J.B.; Demirtaş, M.; Eckner, W.J.; Navejar, N.M.; Ji, J.L.; Martin, W.J.; Bernacchia, A.; Anticevic, A.; Murray, J.D. Hierarchy of Transcriptomic Specialization across Human Cortex Captured by Structural Neuroimaging Topography. Nat. Neurosci. 2018, 21, 1251–1259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Deco, G.; Vidaurre, D.; Kringelbach, M.L. Revisiting the Global Workspace Orchestrating the Hierarchical Organization of the Human Brain. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021, 5, 497–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Demirtaş, M.; Burt, J.B.; Helmer, M.; Ji, J.L.; Adkinson, B.D.; Glasser, M.F.; Essen, D.C.V.; Sotiropoulos, S.N.; Anticevic, A.; Murray, J.D. Hierarchical Heterogeneity across Human Cortex Shapes Large-Scale Neural Dynamics. Neuron 2019, 101, 1181–1194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Wang, P.; Kong, R.; Kong, X.; Liégeois, R.; Orban, C.; Deco, G.; van den Heuvel, M.P.; Yeo, B.T.T. Inversion of a Large-Scale Circuit Model Reveals a Cortical Hierarchy in the Dynamic Resting Human Brain. Sci. Adv. 2019, 5, eaat7854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Mukamel, E.A.; Pirondini, E.; Babadi, B.; Wong, K.F.K.; Pierce, E.T.; Harrell, P.G.; Walsh, J.L.; Salazar-Gomez, A.F.; Cash, S.S.; Eskandar, E.N.; et al. A Transition in Brain State during Propofol-Induced Unconsciousness. J. Neurosci. 2014, 34, 839–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Li, D.; Li, X.; Hagihira, S.; Sleigh, J.W. Cross-Frequency Coupling during Isoflurane Anaesthesia as Revealed by Electroencephalographic Harmonic Wavelet Bicoherence. Bja Br. J. Anaesth. 2013, 110, 409–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Chamadia, S.; Pedemonte, J.C.; Hahm, E.Y.; Mekonnen, J.; Ibala, R.; Gitlin, J.; Ethridge, B.R.; Qu, J.; Vazquez, R.; Rhee, J.; et al. Delta Oscillations Phase Limit Neural Activity during Sevoflurane Anesthesia. Commun. Biol. 2019, 2, 415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Jiang, X.; Wen, X.; Ou, G.; Li, S.; Chen, Y.; Zhang, J.; Liang, Z. Propofol Modulates Neural Dynamics of Thalamo-Cortical System Associated with Anesthetic Levels in Rats. Cogn. Neurodyn. 2022, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Huang, Z.; Tarnal, V.; Vlisides, P.E.; Janke, E.L.; McKinney, A.M.; Picton, P.; Mashour, G.A.; Hudetz, A.G. Asymmetric Neural Dynamics Characterize Loss and Recovery of Consciousness. Neuroimage 2021, 236, 118042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Huang, Z.; Liu, X.; Mashour, G.A.; Hudetz, A.G. Timescales of Intrinsic BOLD Signal Dynamics and Functional Connectivity in Pharmacologic and Neuropathologic States of Unconsciousness. J. Neurosci. 2018, 38, 2304–2317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Signorelli, C.M.; Uhrig, L.; Kringelbach, M.; Jarraya, B.; Deco, G. Hierarchical Disruption in the Cortex of Anesthetized Monkeys as a New Signature of Consciousness Loss. Neuroimage 2021, 227, 117618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Huang, Z.; Mashour, G.A.; Hudetz, A.G. Functional Geometry of the Cortex Encodes Dimensions of Consciousness. Nat. Commun. 2023, 14, 72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Podvalny, E.; Flounders, M.W.; King, L.E.; Holroyd, T.; He, B.J. A Dual Role of Prestimulus Spontaneous Neural Activity in Visual Object Recognition. Nat. Commun. 2019, 10, 3910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Ergenoglu, T.; Demiralp, T.; Bayraktaroglu, Z.; Ergen, M.; Beydagi, H.; Uresin, Y. Alpha Rhythm of the EEG Modulates Visual Detection Performance in Humans. Cogn. Brain Res. 2004, 20, 376–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Boly, M.; Balteau, E.; Schnakers, C.; Degueldre, C.; Moonen, G.; Luxen, A.; Phillips, C.; Peigneux, P.; Maquet, P.; Laureys, S. Baseline Brain Activity Fluctuations Predict Somatosensory Perception in Humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 12187–12192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Sadaghiani, S.; Hesselmann, G.; Kleinschmidt, A. Distributed and Antagonistic Contributions of Ongoing Activity Fluctuations to Auditory Stimulus Detection. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 13410–13417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  73. Al, E.; Iliopoulos, F.; Forschack, N.; Nierhaus, T.; Grund, M.; Motyka, P.; Gaebler, M.; Nikulin, V.V.; Villringer, A. Heart–Brain Interactions Shape Somatosensory Perception and Evoked Potentials. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 10575–10584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Sadaghiani, S.; Scheeringa, R.; Lehongre, K.; Morillon, B.; Giraud, A.-L.; Kleinschmidt, A. Intrinsic Connectivity Networks, Alpha Oscillations, and Tonic Alertness: A Simultaneous Electroencephalography/Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 10243–10250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. Hesselmann, G.; Kell, C.A.; Kleinschmidt, A. Ongoing Activity Fluctuations in HMT+ Bias the Perception of Coherent Visual Motion. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 14481–14485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Sadaghiani, S.; Poline, J.-B.; Kleinschmidt, A.; D’Esposito, M. Ongoing Dynamics in Large-Scale Functional Connectivity Predict Perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 8463–8468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Benwell, C.S.Y.; Tagliabue, C.F.; Veniero, D.; Cecere, R.; Savazzi, S.; Thut, G. Prestimulus EEG Power Predicts Conscious Awareness But Not Objective Visual Performance. Eneuro 2017, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Ploner, M.; Lee, M.C.; Wiech, K.; Bingel, U.; Tracey, I. Prestimulus Functional Connectivity Determines Pain Perception in Humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 355–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Van Dijk, H.; Schoffelen, J.-M.; Oostenveld, R.; Jensen, O. Prestimulus Oscillatory Activity in the Alpha Band Predicts Visual Discrimination Ability. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 1816–1823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Romei, V.; Brodbeck, V.; Michel, C.; Amedi, A.; Pascual-Leone, A.; Thut, G. Spontaneous Fluctuations in Posterior α-Band EEG Activity Reflect Variability in Excitability of Human Visual Areas. Cereb. Cortex 2008, 18, 2010–2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Iemi, L.; Chaumon, M.; Crouzet, S.M.; Busch, N.A. Spontaneous Neural Oscillations Bias Perception by Modulating Baseline Excitability. J. Neurosci. 2017, 37, 807–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  82. Van Vugt, B.; Dagnino, B.; Vartak, D.; Safaai, H.; Panzeri, S.; Dehaene, S.; Roelfsema, P.R. The Threshold for Conscious Report: Signal Loss and Response Bias in Visual and Frontal Cortex. Science 2018, 360, 537–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  83. Northoff, G.; Klar, P.; Bein, M.; Safron, A. As without, so within: How the Brain’s Temporo-Spatial Alignment to the Environment Shapes Consciousness. Interface Focus 2023, 13, 20220076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Safron, A. An Integrated World Modeling Theory (IWMT) of Consciousness: Combining Integrated Information and Global Neuronal Workspace Theories With the Free Energy Principle and Active Inference Framework; Toward Solving the Hard Problem and Characterizing Agentic Causation. Front. Artif. Intell. 2020, 3, 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Safron, A. Integrated World Modeling Theory Expanded: Implications for the Future of Consciousness. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 642397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Casali, A.G.; Gosseries, O.; Rosanova, M.; Boly, M.; Sarasso, S.; Casali, K.R.; Casarotto, S.; Bruno, M.-A.; Laureys, S.; Tononi, G.; et al. A Theoretically Based Index of Consciousness Independent of Sensory Processing and Behavior. Sci. Transl. Med. 2013, 5, 198ra105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Fingelkurts, A.A.; Fingelkurts, A.A. Present Moment, Past, and Future: Mental Kaleidoscope. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Siclari, F.; Baird, B.; Perogamvros, L.; Bernardi, G.; LaRocque, J.J.; Riedner, B.; Boly, M.; Postle, B.R.; Tononi, G. The Neural Correlates of Dreaming. Nat. Neurosci. 2017, 20, 872–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Cul, A.D.; Dehaene, S.; Reyes, P.; Bravo, E.; Slachevsky, A. Causal Role of Prefrontal Cortex in the Threshold for Access to Consciousness. Brain 2009, 132, 2531–2540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. Block, N. Two Neural Correlates of Consciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 46–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Mashour, G.A.; Roelfsema, P.; Changeux, J.-P.; Dehaene, S. Conscious Processing and the Global Neuronal Workspace Hypothesis. Neuron 2020, 105, 776–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Moutard, C.; Dehaene, S.; Malach, R. Spontaneous Fluctuations and Non-Linear Ignitions: Two Dynamic Faces of Cortical Recurrent Loops. Neuron 2015, 88, 194–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  93. Dehaene, S.; Charles, L.; King, J.-R.; Marti, S. Toward a Computational Theory of Conscious Processing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2014, 25, 76–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  94. Dehaene, S.; Kerszberg, M.; Changeux, J.-P. A Neuronal Model of a Global Workspace in Effortful Cognitive Tasks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1998, 95, 14529–14534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Brown, E.N.; Purdon, P.L.; Dort, C.J.V. General Anesthesia and Altered States of Arousal: A Systems Neuroscience Analysis. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2011, 34, 601–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  96. Laureys, S. The Neural Correlate of (Un)Awareness: Lessons from the Vegetative State. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 556–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Fingelkurts, A.A.; Fingelkurts, A.A.; Bagnato, S.; Boccagni, C.; Galardi, G. Do We Need a Theory-Based Assessment of Consciousness in the Field of Disorders of Consciousness? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  98. Monti, M.M. Cognition in the Vegetative State. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2012, 8, 431–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  99. Schiff, N.D. Cognitive Motor Dissociation Following Severe Brain Injuries. Jama Neurol. 2015, 72, 1413–1415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Owen, A.M.; Coleman, M.R.; Boly, M.; Davis, M.H.; Laureys, S.; Pickard, J.D. Detecting Awareness in the Vegetative State. Science 2006, 313, 1402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Timmermann, C.; Bauer, P.R.; Gosseries, O.; Vanhaudenhuyse, A.; Vollenweider, F.; Laureys, S.; Singer, T.; Group, M. and L.E. (MLE) E.R.; Antonova, E.; Lutz, A. A Neurophenomenological Approach to Non-Ordinary States of Consciousness: Hypnosis, Meditation, and Psychedelics. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2023, 27, 139–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Bayne, T.; Hohwy, J.; Owen, A.M. Are There Levels of Consciousness? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016, 20, 405–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Bayne, T.; Carter, O. Dimensions of Consciousness and the Psychedelic State. Neurosci. Conscious. 2018, 2018, niy008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Fortier-Davy, M.; Millière, R. The Multi-Dimensional Approach to Drug-Induced States a Commentary on Bayne and Carter’s “Dimensions of Consciousness and the Psychedelic State”. Neurosci. Conscious. 2020, 2020, niaa004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Birch, J.; Schnell, A.K.; Clayton, N.S. Dimensions of Animal Consciousness. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020, 24, 789–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Walter, J. Consciousness as a Multidimensional Phenomenon: Implications for the Assessment of Disorders of Consciousness. Neurosci. Conscious. 2021, 2021, niab047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Carlén, M. What Constitutes the Prefrontal Cortex? Science 2017, 358, 478–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  108. Park, H.-J.; Friston, K. Structural and Functional Brain Networks: From Connections to Cognition. Science 2013, 342, 1238411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  109. Bernhardt, B.C.; Smallwood, J.; Keilholz, S.; Margulies, D.S. Gradients in Brain Organization. Neuroimage 2022, 251, 118987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Huntenburg, J.M.; Bazin, P.-L.; Margulies, D.S. Large-Scale Gradients in Human Cortical Organization. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018, 22, 21–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  111. Keller, A.S.; Sydnor, V.J.; Pines, A.; Fair, D.A.; Bassett, D.S.; Satterthwaite, T.D. Hierarchical Functional System Development Supports Executive Function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2023, 27, 160–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Murphy, C.; Jefferies, E.; Rueschemeyer, S.-A.; Sormaz, M.; Wang, H.; Margulies, D.S.; Smallwood, J. Distant from Input: Evidence of Regions within the Default Mode Network Supporting Perceptually-Decoupled and Conceptually-Guided Cognition. Neuroimage 2018, 171, 393–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Smallwood, J.; Turnbull, A.; Wang, H.; Ho, N.S.P.; Poerio, G.L.; Karapanagiotidis, T.; Konu, D.; Mckeown, B.; Zhang, M.; Murphy, C.; et al. The Neural Correlates of Ongoing Conscious Thought. Iscience 2021, 24, 102132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Cross, N.; Paquola, C.; Pomares, F.B.; Perrault, A.A.; Jegou, A.; Nguyen, A.; Aydin, U.; Bernhardt, B.C.; Grova, C.; Dang-Vu, T.T. Cortical Gradients of Functional Connectivity Are Robust to State-Dependent Changes Following Sleep Deprivation. Neuroimage 2021, 226, 117547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Huang, Z.; Zhang, J.; Wu, J.; Mashour, G.A.; Hudetz, A.G. Temporal Circuit of Macroscale Dynamic Brain Activity Supports Human Consciousness. Sci. Adv. 2020, 6, eaaz0087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  116. Huang, Z.; Tarnal, V.; Vlisides, P.E.; Janke, E.L.; McKinney, A.M.; Picton, P.; Mashour, G.A.; Hudetz, A.G. Anterior Insula Regulates Brain Network Transitions That Gate Conscious Access. Cell Rep. 2021, 35, 109081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Menon, V. Large-Scale Brain Networks and Psychopathology: A Unifying Triple Network Model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 483–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Uddin, L.Q. Salience Processing and Insular Cortical Function and Dysfunction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015, 16, 55–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Klar, P.; Çatal, Y.; Langner, R.; Huang, Z.; Northoff, G. Scale-Free Dynamics in the Core-Periphery Topography and Task Alignment Decline from Conscious to Unconscious States. Commun. Biol. 2023, 6, 499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Vossel, S.; Geng, J.J.; Fink, G.R. Dorsal and Ventral Attention Systems. Neurosci. 2014, 20, 150–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Corbetta, M.; Shulman, G.L. Control of Goal-Directed and Stimulus-Driven Attention in the Brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002, 3, 201–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Smallwood, J.; Bernhardt, B.C.; Leech, R.; Bzdok, D.; Jefferies, E.; Margulies, D.S. The Default Mode Network in Cognition: A Topographical Perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2021, 22, 503–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Raichle, M.E.; MacLeod, A.M.; Snyder, A.Z.; Powers, W.J.; Gusnard, D.A.; Shulman, G.L. A Default Mode of Brain Function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 676–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Demertzi, A.; Kucyi, A.; Ponce-Alvarez, A.; Keliris, G.A.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S.; Deco, G. Functional Network Antagonism and Consciousness. Netw. Neurosci. 2022, 6, 998–1009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Schooler, J.W.; Smallwood, J.; Christoff, K.; Handy, T.C.; Reichle, E.D.; Sayette, M.A. Meta-Awareness, Perceptual Decoupling and the Wandering Mind. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011, 15, 319–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Raichle, M.E. The Brain’s Default Mode Network. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2013, 38, 433–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  127. Fornito, A.; Harrison, B.J.; Zalesky, A.; Simons, J.S. Competitive and Cooperative Dynamics of Large-Scale Brain Functional Networks Supporting Recollection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 12788–12793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Critchley, H.D.; Wiens, S.; Rotshtein, P.; Öhman, A.; Dolan, R.J. Neural Systems Supporting Interoceptive Awareness. Nat. Neurosci. 2004, 7, 189–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  129. Faivre, N.; Salomon, R.; Blanke, O. Visual Consciousness and Bodily Self-Consciousness. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2015, 28, 23–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  130. Fingelkurts, A.A.; Fingelkurts, A.A.; Kallio-Tamminen, T. Selfhood Triumvirate: From Phenomenology to Brain Activity and Back Again. Conscious. Cogn. 2020, 86, 103031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Northoff, G. Immanuel Kant’s Mind and the Brain’s Resting State. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 356–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Gu, Y.; Sainburg, L.E.; Kuang, S.; Han, F.; Williams, J.W.; Liu, Y.; Zhang, N.; Zhang, X.; Leopold, D.A.; Liu, X. Brain Activity Fluctuations Propagate as Waves Traversing the Cortical Hierarchy. Cereb. Cortex 2021, 31, bhab064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Raut, R.V.; Snyder, A.Z.; Mitra, A.; Yellin, D.; Fujii, N.; Malach, R.; Raichle, M.E. Global Waves Synchronize the Brain’s Functional Systems with Fluctuating Arousal. Sci. Adv. 2021, 7, eabf2709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  134. Yousefi, B.; Keilholz, S. Propagating Patterns of Intrinsic Activity along Macroscale Gradients Coordinate Functional Connections across the Whole Brain. Neuroimage 2021, 231, 117827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  135. Bolt, T.; Nomi, J.S.; Bzdok, D.; Salas, J.A.; Chang, C.; Yeo, B.T.T.; Uddin, L.Q.; Keilholz, S.D. A Parsimonious Description of Global Functional Brain Organization in Three Spatiotemporal Patterns. Nat. Neurosci. 2022, 25, 1093–1103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Temporospatial nestedness. (A) Various types of cross-frequency coupling, including power-to-power, phase-to-phase, phase-to-power, phase-to-frequency, power-to-frequency, and frequency-to-frequency. The depiction is adapted from the work of Jirsa and Müller (2013) [28]. (B) Interaction between envelope and phase for intrinsic coupling modes (ICMs). Envelope ICMs may serve to regulate the coactivation of brain regions participating in a functional network. The illustration is adapted from the work of Engel et al. (2013) [38]. (C) Hierarchical topography of temporal receptive windows (TRWs) mapped using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in response to audio narratives. The fMRI map demonstrates the gradual transition from short to long TRWs along the temporal–parietal axis. The illustration is adapted from the work of Lerner et al. (2011) [26]. (D) Hierarchy of timescales in response to visual input. A pulse of input to area V1 is propagated along the hierarchy, displaying increasing decay times as it progresses. The autocorrelation of area activity in response to white noise input to V1 shows a functional hierarchy ranging from area V1 at the bottom to prefrontal areas at the top. The illustration is adapted from the work of Chaudhuri et al. (2015) [39].
Figure 1. Temporospatial nestedness. (A) Various types of cross-frequency coupling, including power-to-power, phase-to-phase, phase-to-power, phase-to-frequency, power-to-frequency, and frequency-to-frequency. The depiction is adapted from the work of Jirsa and Müller (2013) [28]. (B) Interaction between envelope and phase for intrinsic coupling modes (ICMs). Envelope ICMs may serve to regulate the coactivation of brain regions participating in a functional network. The illustration is adapted from the work of Engel et al. (2013) [38]. (C) Hierarchical topography of temporal receptive windows (TRWs) mapped using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in response to audio narratives. The fMRI map demonstrates the gradual transition from short to long TRWs along the temporal–parietal axis. The illustration is adapted from the work of Lerner et al. (2011) [26]. (D) Hierarchy of timescales in response to visual input. A pulse of input to area V1 is propagated along the hierarchy, displaying increasing decay times as it progresses. The autocorrelation of area activity in response to white noise input to V1 shows a functional hierarchy ranging from area V1 at the bottom to prefrontal areas at the top. The illustration is adapted from the work of Chaudhuri et al. (2015) [39].
Entropy 25 01074 g001
Figure 2. Temporospatial alignment. (A) Pre-stimulus functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) time courses from the bilateral auditory cortex. It shows significantly (as indicated by *) higher activity preceding hits compared to misses at the stimulus onset (t = 0; as indicated by a dash line). The illustration is adapted from the work of Sadaghiani et al. (2009) [72]. (B) How the brain processes information by employing distinct temporal receptive windows (TRWs). Words are processed within the span of hundreds of milliseconds, sentences within a few seconds, and paragraphs within tens of seconds. Brain regions characterized by higher frequency fluctuations contribute to the processing of swift sensory information, while regions exhibiting dominance in low frequencies are involved in integrating perceptual and cognitive events that unfold over longer durations [24,25,26,27].
Figure 2. Temporospatial alignment. (A) Pre-stimulus functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) time courses from the bilateral auditory cortex. It shows significantly (as indicated by *) higher activity preceding hits compared to misses at the stimulus onset (t = 0; as indicated by a dash line). The illustration is adapted from the work of Sadaghiani et al. (2009) [72]. (B) How the brain processes information by employing distinct temporal receptive windows (TRWs). Words are processed within the span of hundreds of milliseconds, sentences within a few seconds, and paragraphs within tens of seconds. Brain regions characterized by higher frequency fluctuations contribute to the processing of swift sensory information, while regions exhibiting dominance in low frequencies are involved in integrating perceptual and cognitive events that unfold over longer durations [24,25,26,27].
Entropy 25 01074 g002
Figure 3. Temporospatial expansion. (A) The content of external inputs, such as a face, remains unconscious during low-level sensory registration, with limited temporospatial expansion in the brain. Subjective and phenomenal experience occurs when the temporospatial expansion in brain activity reaches a certain extent, which is associated with the state of “no-report” consciousness. (B) A conscious brain can propagate the TMS -evoked activity both in time and space. In contrast, an unconscious brain may lose either the spatial expansion or temporal expansion or both. The illustration is inspired by the works of Casali et al. (2013) [86] and Koch et al. (2016) [6].
Figure 3. Temporospatial expansion. (A) The content of external inputs, such as a face, remains unconscious during low-level sensory registration, with limited temporospatial expansion in the brain. Subjective and phenomenal experience occurs when the temporospatial expansion in brain activity reaches a certain extent, which is associated with the state of “no-report” consciousness. (B) A conscious brain can propagate the TMS -evoked activity both in time and space. In contrast, an unconscious brain may lose either the spatial expansion or temporal expansion or both. The illustration is inspired by the works of Casali et al. (2013) [86] and Koch et al. (2016) [6].
Entropy 25 01074 g003
Figure 4. Temporospatial globalization. (A) The original depiction of the Dehaene–Changeux model of a Global Neuronal Workspace (GNW) (adapted from [93,94]). The GNW model suggests that subjective experience corresponds to the global availability of information. According to this model, conscious access occurs when a piece of information enters a distributed network of cortical areas that are tightly interconnected by long-distance axons, known as the GNW. This network enables the flexible broadcasting of information to various specialized processors within the brain. (B) Subjects are able to “report” the content of external inputs when the temporospatial expansion is globalized. This globalized expansion involves the engagement of lateral frontoparietal networks. The phenomenon of “temporospatial globalization” is concurrent with complex cognitive processing.
Figure 4. Temporospatial globalization. (A) The original depiction of the Dehaene–Changeux model of a Global Neuronal Workspace (GNW) (adapted from [93,94]). The GNW model suggests that subjective experience corresponds to the global availability of information. According to this model, conscious access occurs when a piece of information enters a distributed network of cortical areas that are tightly interconnected by long-distance axons, known as the GNW. This network enables the flexible broadcasting of information to various specialized processors within the brain. (B) Subjects are able to “report” the content of external inputs when the temporospatial expansion is globalized. This globalized expansion involves the engagement of lateral frontoparietal networks. The phenomenon of “temporospatial globalization” is concurrent with complex cognitive processing.
Entropy 25 01074 g004
Figure 5. Analogy between land spatial geometry and cortical gradients. Huntenburg et al. (2018) [110] proposed an intrinsic coordinate system based on the distance along the cortex. The dimension from unimodal to transmodal processing is represented by the distance along the cortical surface. The illustration is adapted from the works of Margulies et al. (2016) and Huntenburg et al. (2018) [56,110].
Figure 5. Analogy between land spatial geometry and cortical gradients. Huntenburg et al. (2018) [110] proposed an intrinsic coordinate system based on the distance along the cortex. The dimension from unimodal to transmodal processing is represented by the distance along the cortical surface. The illustration is adapted from the works of Margulies et al. (2016) and Huntenburg et al. (2018) [56,110].
Entropy 25 01074 g005
Figure 6. Links between cortical gradients and neurofunctional dimensions of consciousness. Consciousness is often compared to solving a scrambled Rubik’s cube, where examining a single surface may lead to confusion about its organization. To fully understand consciousness, it is necessary to consider multiple dimensions. These dimensions encompass (1) awareness, or what we actually experience, like the redness of a rose; (2) sensory organization, or how sights and sounds and feelings become woven together to create our seamless conscious experience; and (3) arousability, that is, the ability of the brain to be awake. Recent research has identified three cortical gradients that appear to align with these dimensions of consciousness. The illustration is adapted from the work of Huang et al. (2023) [68].
Figure 6. Links between cortical gradients and neurofunctional dimensions of consciousness. Consciousness is often compared to solving a scrambled Rubik’s cube, where examining a single surface may lead to confusion about its organization. To fully understand consciousness, it is necessary to consider multiple dimensions. These dimensions encompass (1) awareness, or what we actually experience, like the redness of a rose; (2) sensory organization, or how sights and sounds and feelings become woven together to create our seamless conscious experience; and (3) arousability, that is, the ability of the brain to be awake. Recent research has identified three cortical gradients that appear to align with these dimensions of consciousness. The illustration is adapted from the work of Huang et al. (2023) [68].
Entropy 25 01074 g006
Figure 7. Dynamic brain states and the temporal circuit. (A) Illustration of the k-means clustering algorithm used to partition fMRI image volumes into k co-activation patterns (CAPs). (B) Eight CAPs were classified as follows: default mode network (DMN+), dorsal attention network (DAT+), frontoparietal network (FPN+), sensory and motor network (SMN+), visual network (VIS+), ventral attention network (VAT+), and global network of activation and deactivation (GN+ and GN−). These eight CAPs can be grouped into four pairs of “mirror” motifs (represented by four color classes). For example, the DMN+ is accompanied by co-deactivation of DAT (DAT−) and vice versa for DAT+ (DMN−). The illustration is adapted from the work of Huang et al. (2020) [115].
Figure 7. Dynamic brain states and the temporal circuit. (A) Illustration of the k-means clustering algorithm used to partition fMRI image volumes into k co-activation patterns (CAPs). (B) Eight CAPs were classified as follows: default mode network (DMN+), dorsal attention network (DAT+), frontoparietal network (FPN+), sensory and motor network (SMN+), visual network (VIS+), ventral attention network (VAT+), and global network of activation and deactivation (GN+ and GN−). These eight CAPs can be grouped into four pairs of “mirror” motifs (represented by four color classes). For example, the DMN+ is accompanied by co-deactivation of DAT (DAT−) and vice versa for DAT+ (DMN−). The illustration is adapted from the work of Huang et al. (2020) [115].
Entropy 25 01074 g007
Figure 8. Covariation of the temporal circuit and functional geometry with changes in conscious state. (A) The temporal circuit is characterized by trajectories along which dynamic brain states occur. Transitions between the default mode network (DMN+) and dorsal attention network (DAT+) are embedded in this temporal circuit, with balanced reciprocal accessibility of brain states being characteristic of consciousness. Unconsciousness is associated with isolation (i.e., reduced accessibility) of DMN+ and DAT+ from the trajectory space, which is dominated by a few giant attractors such as the visual network (VIS+) and ventral attention network (VAT+). (B) The brain’s functional geometry, such as network distance in the gradient space, and temporal dynamics, such as occurrence rates of co-activation patterns, covary with the state of consciousness. Co-activation patterns of DMN+, DAT+, and VAT+ can be considered snapshots of global propagation waves. During unconscious states, the propagation of co-activations across the DMN and DAT (DMN+ and DAT+) becomes less frequent (e.g., low occurrence rates), while those involving the VAT (VAT+) become more frequent. These effects are associated with a reduced functional distance between DMN and VAT, as well as a reduced functional distance between DMN and DAT, in the 3D gradient space. The illustration is adapted from the works of Huang et al. (2020, 2023) [68,115].
Figure 8. Covariation of the temporal circuit and functional geometry with changes in conscious state. (A) The temporal circuit is characterized by trajectories along which dynamic brain states occur. Transitions between the default mode network (DMN+) and dorsal attention network (DAT+) are embedded in this temporal circuit, with balanced reciprocal accessibility of brain states being characteristic of consciousness. Unconsciousness is associated with isolation (i.e., reduced accessibility) of DMN+ and DAT+ from the trajectory space, which is dominated by a few giant attractors such as the visual network (VIS+) and ventral attention network (VAT+). (B) The brain’s functional geometry, such as network distance in the gradient space, and temporal dynamics, such as occurrence rates of co-activation patterns, covary with the state of consciousness. Co-activation patterns of DMN+, DAT+, and VAT+ can be considered snapshots of global propagation waves. During unconscious states, the propagation of co-activations across the DMN and DAT (DMN+ and DAT+) becomes less frequent (e.g., low occurrence rates), while those involving the VAT (VAT+) become more frequent. These effects are associated with a reduced functional distance between DMN and VAT, as well as a reduced functional distance between DMN and DAT, in the 3D gradient space. The illustration is adapted from the works of Huang et al. (2020, 2023) [68,115].
Entropy 25 01074 g008
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Huang, Z. Temporospatial Nestedness in Consciousness: An Updated Perspective on the Temporospatial Theory of Consciousness. Entropy 2023, 25, 1074. https://doi.org/10.3390/e25071074

AMA Style

Huang Z. Temporospatial Nestedness in Consciousness: An Updated Perspective on the Temporospatial Theory of Consciousness. Entropy. 2023; 25(7):1074. https://doi.org/10.3390/e25071074

Chicago/Turabian Style

Huang, Zirui. 2023. "Temporospatial Nestedness in Consciousness: An Updated Perspective on the Temporospatial Theory of Consciousness" Entropy 25, no. 7: 1074. https://doi.org/10.3390/e25071074

APA Style

Huang, Z. (2023). Temporospatial Nestedness in Consciousness: An Updated Perspective on the Temporospatial Theory of Consciousness. Entropy, 25(7), 1074. https://doi.org/10.3390/e25071074

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop