Deductive Reasoning and Working Memory Skills in Individuals with Blindness
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants and Ethics
2.2. Materials
2.2.1. Deductive Reasoning Tests
- −
- Most people need to prifa several times a day,
- −
- Most people are very careful about their prifa.
- Her mother.
- Her daughter.
- Her grandmother.
- Her granddaughter.
2.2.2. Working Memory Tests
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Reasoning Tasks
- (1)
- Word context task. In the word context test, no significant effect was found between the groups (F(3,59) = 1.12, p = 0.348, ηp2 = 0.05, H(3) = 3.16, p = 0.367; see Figure 1).
- (2)
- Deductive reasoning argument task. Comparing the four groups in the deductive reasoning argument task yielded a significant result (F(3,59) = 8.18, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.3, H(3) = 18.39, p < 0.001), showing that the sighted participants performed better than the CB group and the blindfolded group, with LB performing better than the blindfolded group.
3.2. Memory Tasks
3.3. Relationship between the Variables
4. Discussion
4.1. Reasoning Ability
4.2. Working Memory
4.3. Relationship between Visual Experience/Working Memory and Deductive Reasoning Skills
4.4. Future Considerations
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Diamond, A. Executive Functions. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Friedman, N.P.; Miyake, A. Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex 2017, 86, 186–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bacon, A.M.; Handley, S.J.; Dennis, I.; Newstead, S.E. Reasoning strategies: The role of working memory and verbal-spatial ability. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2008, 20, 1065–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ahmed, F.S.; Miller, L.S. Executive Function Mechanisms of Theory of Mind. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2011, 41, 667–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chuderski, A.; Necka, E. The contribution of working memory to fluid reasoning: Capacity, control, or both? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2012, 38, 1689–1710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ariës, R.J.; Groot, W.; Van Den Brink, H.M. Improving reasoning skills in secondary history education by working memory training. Br. Educ. Res. J. 2015, 41, 210–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A. Understanding Executive Functions: What Helps or Hinders Them and How Executive Functions and Language Development Mutually Support One Another. Perspect. Lang. Lit. 2014, 40, 7–11. [Google Scholar]
- Kyllonen, P.C.; Christal, R.E. Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelligence 1990, 14, 389–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olesen, P.J.; Westerberg, H.; Klingberg, T. Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2004, 7, 75–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simms, N.K.; Frausel, R.R.; Richland, L.E. Working memory predicts children’s analogical reasoning. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2018, 166, 160–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fine, I.; Park, J.M. Blindness and human brain plasticity. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 2018, 4, 337–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bell, L.; Wagels, L.; Neuschaefer-Rube, C.; Fels, J.; Gur, R.E.; Konrad, K. The Cross-Modal Effects of Sensory Deprivation on Spatial and Temporal Processes in Vision and Audition: A Systematic Review on Behavioral and Neuroimaging Research since 2000. Neural Plast. 2019, 2019, 9603469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Merabet, L.B.; Pascual-Leone, A. Neural reorganization following sensory loss: The opportunity of change. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 11, 44–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chebat, D.R.; Heimler, B.; Hofsetter, S.; Amedi, A. The implications of brain plasticity and task selectivity for visual rehabilitation of blind and visually impaired individuals. In The Neuroimaging of Brain Diseases; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2018; pp. 295–321. [Google Scholar]
- Dormal, V.; Crollen, V.; Baumans, C.; Lepore, F.; Collignon, O. Early but not late blindness leads to enhanced arithmetic and working memory abilities. Cortex 2016, 83, 212–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heled, E.; Oshri, O. Validation of the Tactual Span in individuals with congenital and acquired blindness. Br. J. Vis. Impair. 2021, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pigeon, C.; Marin-Lamellet, C. Evaluation of the attentional capacities and working memory of early and late blind persons. Acta Psychol. 2015, 155, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Withagen, A.; Kappers, A.M.L.; Vervloed, M.P.J.; Knoors, H.; Verhoeven, L. Short term memory and working memory in blind versus sighted children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2013, 34, 2161–2172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, H.J.; Chun, J.W.; Park, B.; Park, H.; Kim, J.I.; Lee, J.D.; Kim, J.J. Activation of the occipital cortex and deactivation of the default mode network during working memory in the early blind. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 407–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rokem, A.; Ahissar, M. Interactions of cognitive and auditory abilities in congenitally blind individuals. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 843–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vecchi, T.; Tinti, C.; Cornoldi, C. Spatial memory and integration processes in congenital blindness. Neuroreport 2004, 15, 2787–2790. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, H.; Voss, P.; Lepore, F.; Scherzer, P. The Nature of Working Memory for Braille. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e10833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Occelli, V.; Lacey, S.; Stephens, C.; Merabet, L.B.; Sathian, K. Enhanced verbal abilities in the congenitally blind. Exp. Brain Res. 2017, 235, 1709–1718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aleman, A.; van Lee, L.; Mantione, M.H.; Verkoijen, I.G.; de Haan, E.H.F. Visual imagery without visual experience: Evidence from congenitally totally blind people. Neuroreport 2001, 12, 2601–2604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cornoldi, C.; Cortesi, A.; Preti, D. Individual differences in the capacity limitations of visuospatial short-term memory: Research on sighted and totally congenitally blind people. Mem. Cognit. 1991, 19, 459–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vecchi, T. Visuo-spatial Imagery in Congenitally Totally Blind People. Memory 1998, 6, 91–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knauff, M.; May, E. Mental imagery, reasoning, and blindness. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2006, 59, 161–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edmonds, C.J.; Pring, L. Generating inferences from written and spoken language: A comparison of children with visual impairment and children with sight. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2006, 24, 337–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knauff, M.; May, E. Visual Imagery in Deductive Reasoning: Results from experiments with sighted, blindfolded, and congenitally totally blind persons. In Proceedings of the Twenty-sixth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2004; pp. 708–713. [Google Scholar]
- Bliss, I.; Kujala, T.; Hämäläinen, H. Comparison of blind and sighted participants’ performance in a letter recognition working memory task. Cogn. Brain Res. 2004, 18, 273–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bonino, D.; Ricciardi, E.; Sani, L.; Gentili, C.; Vanello, N.; Guazzelli, M.; Vecchi, T.; Pietrini, P. Tactile spatial working memory activates the dorsal extrastriate cortical pathway in congenitally blind individuals. Arch. Ital. Biol. 2008, 146, 133–146. [Google Scholar]
- Bottini, R.; Mattioni, S.; Collignon, O. Early blindness alters the spatial organization of verbal working memory. Cortex 2016, 83, 271–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raz, N.; Striem, E.; Pundak, G.; Orlov, T. Report Superior Serial Memory in the Blind: A Case of Cognitive Compensatory Adjustment. Curr. Biol. 2007, 17, 1129–1133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ruggiero, G.; Iachini, T. The Role of Vision in the Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Evidence From Blind and Sighted People. Neuropsychology 2010, 24, 674–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattaneo, Z.; Vecchi, T. Supramodality Effects in Visual and Haptic Spatial Processes. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2008, 34, 631–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rindermann, H.; Ackermann, A.L.; te Nijenhuis, J. Does Blindness Boost Working Memory? A Natural Experiment and Cross-Cultural Study. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carpenter, P.A.; Eisenberg, P. Mental rotation and the frame of reference in blind and sighted individuals. Perception, Psychophys. 1978, 23, 117–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- MacCluskie, K.C.; Tunick, R.H.; Dial, J.G.; Paul, D.S. The role of vision in the development of abstraction ability. J. Vis. Impair. Blind. 1998, 92, 189–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thinus-Blanc, C.; Gaunet, F. Representation of space in blind persons: Vision as a spatial sense? Psychol. Bull. 1997, 121, 20–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delis, D.C.; Kaplan, E.; Kramer, J. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: Examiners Manual; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd ed.; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Snyder, H.R.; Miyake, A.; Hankin, B.L. Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: Bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Knauff, M.; Mulack, T.; Kassubek, J.; Salih, H.R.; Greenlee, M.W. Spatial imagery in deductive reasoning: A functional MRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 2002, 13, 203–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Salillas, E.; Graná, A.; El-Yagoubi, R.; Semenza, C. Numbers in the blind’s eye. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, 3–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wan, C.Y.; Wood, A.G.; Reutens, D.C.; Wilson, S.J. Early but not late-blindness leads to enhanced auditory perception. Neuropsychologia 2010, 48, 344–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruce, S.M.; Luckner, J.L.; Ferrell, K.A. Assessment of Students With Sensory Disabilities: Evidence-Based Practices. Assess. Eff. Interv. 2018, 43, 79–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Congenitally Blind | Late Blind | Blindfolded Controls | Sighted Controls | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 34.4 (5.76) | 29.93 (4.36) | 26.33 (4.36) | 26.33 (4.45) |
Education (years) | 15.33 (2.09) | 13.93 (1.33) | 14.93 (2.65) | 13.86 (1.55) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Heled, E.; Elul, N.; Ptito, M.; Chebat, D.-R. Deductive Reasoning and Working Memory Skills in Individuals with Blindness. Sensors 2022, 22, 2062. https://doi.org/10.3390/s22052062
Heled E, Elul N, Ptito M, Chebat D-R. Deductive Reasoning and Working Memory Skills in Individuals with Blindness. Sensors. 2022; 22(5):2062. https://doi.org/10.3390/s22052062
Chicago/Turabian StyleHeled, Eyal, Noa Elul, Maurice Ptito, and Daniel-Robert Chebat. 2022. "Deductive Reasoning and Working Memory Skills in Individuals with Blindness" Sensors 22, no. 5: 2062. https://doi.org/10.3390/s22052062
APA StyleHeled, E., Elul, N., Ptito, M., & Chebat, D. -R. (2022). Deductive Reasoning and Working Memory Skills in Individuals with Blindness. Sensors, 22(5), 2062. https://doi.org/10.3390/s22052062