1. Introduction
Ever since Young [
1] presented
Internet Addiction: the Emergence of a New Disorder at the Congress of the American Psychological Association in Toronto, Internet addiction has been a widely discussed disorder in the media and in scientific literature [
2]. The interest in the possible addiction to the Internet, video games, online role-playing games, television, and mobile phones has given rise to a new field of study: technological addictions [
3]. In fact, the DSM-5 [
4] included addiction to video games in the list of disorders that should receive further research. The negative consequences of this problem include the possible increase of stress, anxiety, and/or the “paradox” of a lack of communication, despite being more connected, especially among young people and adolescents [
5,
6,
7].
Internet use and Internet access habits have recently evolved. For example, in 2015, in Spain, there was a clear preference for smartphones (88.2%) over computers (78.2%) for accessing the Internet, especially in 14–19 year olds. This evolution of preference for the phone over the computer was also observed for the accessing of leisure activities, which decreases the dominance of the computer in the professional and educational spheres [
8]. Because of its popularity and because it is a relatively new device (compared to the “classical” mobile phone), the smartphone has raised concerns about its potential to be addictive [
9,
10,
11,
12] as happens with other possible behavioral addictions, such as those to the Internet or to social networking sites [
13].
The prevalence of smartphone addiction has been established using different self-report measures. Among the most frequently used, and actually one of the first self-report questionnaires, there was the Mobile Phone Problem Usage Scale (MPPUS) [
9,
14], later translated and adapted to the Spanish population [
15]. The Problematic Mobile Phone Use Questionnaire (PMPUQ) [
16] has also been employed in several studies [
17]. Another questionnaire used in the Spanish speaking context is the
Cuestionario de Experiencias Relacionadas con el Móvil [Questionnaire for mobile phone-related experiences] (CERM) [
18], used to collect data from young people [
19] and university students [
20,
21]. Recently, several other questionnaires were adapted to asses smartphone addiction, like the Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS) [
22], the Smartphone Addiction Inventory (SPAI) [
23], or the one used in Saudi Arabia [
24]. All of them have shown their usefulness and good validity and reliability, but it is difficult to assess if problematic mobile phone use has increased over the years since researchers use different measurements and instruments.
The term ‘Internet addiction’ or ‘smartphone addiction’ is not used consistently in the literature. A review shows the use of different expressions; there is, for instance, ‘digital addiction’ [
25], ‘problematic Internet use’ [
26], or ‘Internet-use disorder’ [
27]. Even the same authors use different expressions in different papers [
27,
28]. It is not the purpose of the present article to discuss the convenience of one term or another, nor to discuss if the negative consequences can be interpreted as an addiction or not; however, we will use the term ‘addiction’ for the studied phenomenon, because it was one of the first terms used [
1] and because in the studies that we will compare, this term has also been used from the beginning.
Added to the scientific relevance of this issue, the media tend to echo and spread negative information about the use of mobile phones. Results of these alerts are concepts such as the so-called technostress [
29], smombie (a combination of “smartphone” and “zombie”) [
30], fear of missing out (“FoMO”) [
31], and nomophobia (“no-mobile-phone phobia”) [
32]. However, studies on the addictive consequences of both the ‘old’ mobile phone [
9,
33,
34,
35,
36], the current smartphone [
22,
24,
37,
38,
39,
40], and the Internet are cross-sectional, and therefore the temporal evolution of their addictive impact on the population is still unknown. On the basis of the aforementioned research, the objectives of the current study were, first, to explore the perception of problematic Internet and smartphone use in young people in 2017 and, second, to compare these results with former those of studies that used the same measurement instruments, in order to analyze the evolution of the negative effects of Internet and mobile phone use over a period of ten years. As Internet and smartphone use has increased considerably during the past decade, we also expect negative effects to increase over the years (H1). As shown in the previous literature, also in our study women reported a greater use of social networking sites, whereas men used more videogames and adult pages (H2). We also expect women to experiment stronger negative consequences than males (H3).
4. Discussion
The objective of this study was to evaluate the perception of problematic Internet and mobile phone use and compare these results with those of similar cohorts from up to a decade ago. One of the problems presented by studies evaluating technological addictions to the Internet, social networks, mobile phones, and video games is the absence of longitudinal studies. It is difficult for these studies to monitor a cohort in the medium or long term because the questionnaires are administered in person or online to: (i) cohorts obtained in the general population through social networks or the like; (ii) samples of high school or university students; (iii) video gamers identified in forums. Other factors that make longitudinal studies difficult are the need for respecting the participants’ anonymity and the existence of time limitations (i.e., on funding for the research projects and doctoral theses). To overcome these difficulties, this study compared data from different cohorts of university students assessed in different moments.
Our hypotheses were partially confirmed. Taken globally, the results of the study support the idea that the perception of a problematic use of the Internet and mobile phones exists and has increased over the last decade (H1). This perception seems to go along with the growth of Internet use and all kinds of electronic devices with a screen, with which our samples became familiar during their adolescence [
44]. This problematic use of the Internet is specific and not general; that is, it depends on the concrete activity that is carried out [
45,
46,
47]. This can also be affirmed for mobile phones [
17,
48,
49]. Given that the Internet applications most widely used by university students are e-mail and messaging, participating in social networks, and listening to music, we infer that the increase in the perception of problematic use is associated with the use of online social networks. The activities in which the university students invest the least time are betting games and adult pages, like in other similar samples [
17,
45,
46] and as is expected for university students. However, despite the existence of problematic use, it seems that the term ‘addiction’ is an inadequate construct when used as simply “Internet addiction”, because: (i) the problematic use does not depend on the mobile phone or the Internet itself, but on the activities accessed on them; (ii) the problematic use can be the symptom of other disorders, not a primary disorder in itself [
10,
17]; (iii) there is a risk that labeling this problematic use an addiction means pathologizing the daily life [
50]. The questionnaires used make it possible to detect a concern about certain technology-based behaviors but in no case to issue a clinical diagnosis. The term ‘addiction’ is probably adequate when related to specific types of use, such as addiction to gaming via the Internet or to pornography via the Internet.
It is difficult to compare our data on problematic mobile use with those of other prevalence studies because of the use of different measuring instruments. A preliminary comparison shows that the range of values for problematic users or addicts ranges between 0% and 35%, with 10–20% being the most frequent values [
7,
10,
12], although there have been reports of 48% in university students [
24]. In a recent research, the percentages ranged from 3.9% in Belgium to 1% in Poland, with 1.7% addicts identified in the Spanish sample [
17].
It is also not surprising that the Internet uses of young men and women are quite different from each other as there are differences in behavior and attitudes between them in the real world which are perpetuated in the network (H2). Our results show that women use social networks and academic applications more and listen to more music than men. Men play more videogames and betting games and use more adult pages than women. In any case, women’s problematic use is greater (H3), probably as a consequence of their using social networks more than males and of the role that those social networks play in communication and in creating and maintaining connections [
17,
40,
51]. Some studies suggest that there may be different thresholds for males and females with respect to these negative effects [
52].
The average scores in the CERI were higher than in the CERM, as has been the case with these instruments in other studies [
18,
20,
53]. No differences were found between men and women in the problematic use of the mobile phone despite this being a frequent result in other investigations [
15,
17,
40,
53]. Although the Internet activities of men and women were different, there were no differences found in the problematic use of the Internet [
53,
54]. However, the perception of our students was that women are more addicted to the Internet than men probably because of the fact that using social networks is more common than using video games. Another possible explanation is that women are more vulnerable to this type of problem because it is related to communication practices such as establishing and actively maintaining relationships, which women engage in more than men [
38,
40].
Social network use is the only Internet use that moderately correlated with CERI and CERM. The other correlations, in line with other studies, were low or nonexistent [
17]. In fact, online social networking is considered, along with video games, to be the use with the highest risk of becoming problematic [
55,
56] even though there is a lack of empirical confirmation [
13]. Facebook and WhatsApp (as WhatsApp could be considered a social media) could be used as a key component to understand how young people socialize through these applications [
57]. A large-scale tracking of online behavior showed that the use of WhatApp over the smartphone accounted for nearly 20% of all smartphone behavior, above Facebook use [
58]. The low correlation with the different Internet uses can be explained because we are talking about a population that, as a whole, bets little and consumes little pornography, which leads us to think that we would obtain higher correlations if the CERI and the CERM were applied to cohorts of people extracted from the general population, who would be more likely to bet online, consume pornography, and/or be intensive video gamers.
The correlation between the CERI and CERM was high. In fact, we wonder if it is still convenient to use both the CERI and the CERM since, at the current time, both measures may be considered equivalent in the context of young people’s technology use; young people use the mobile phone more and more frequently to access the Internet [
8,
17,
40] and tend not to distinguish between the platforms (mobile vs. computer) and the program/application. The current study confirms how strong smartphone and Internet addiction and social media overlap [
47,
59] and advises that the evolution of technology forces us to change and update certain research questions. For example, when we designed the CERM, the mobile phones used by the participants did not have access to the Internet, whereas at present, there is not really a distinction between mobile phones and smartphones because they are considered synonymous. We will draw upon an anecdote to illustrate this situation. When, over a decade ago, the first papers expressing concern about mobile phone addiction were published [
9], they were about mobile phones without Internet access. However, last year, when we showed a fourth-year psychology student a picture of an old Nokia phone from that era, one of them asked: “But... was it possible to be addicted to that?” This question reveals to what extent the diagnosis of technological addictions is influenced by time and social and cultural change.
From our point of view, the concern over mobile phone addiction came in two distinct waves. The first was focused on the non-smart mobile phone and was mainly due to two factors: the amount of phone bills that the use led to and the high use of text messages. Phone bills were a point of concern because a flat rate did not exist, and users needed a certain learning period to understand how to manage their use so that it remained within reasonable limits. Another point of concern was the number of text messages (i.e., for women: 11 or more calls or text messages per day, see Thomee, Dellve, Harenstam, Hagberg [
6]). Several studies can serve as examples [
6,
9,
34,
36]. When, thanks to new billing structures, users managed to control their phone expenses and these worries seemed to dissipate, smartphones emerged on the market. At that point, a new wave of concern started in regard to this new device, because it allowed access to the Internet and Internet-based applications such as social networks and messaging services. Several studies can serve as examples [
22,
23,
24,
60,
61]. Here again, the influence of context and culture is crucial.
Something similar is occurring with Internet addiction. In the last decade, we have learned that it is convenient to distinguish between behavioral addictions that take place on the Internet (for example, pathological gambling), the specific uses of the Internet that can become problematic (for example, videogames and social networks), and a possible generalized Internet addiction [
45,
46,
49]. Davis [
62] offered one of the first theoretical models, which differentiates between a generalized and a specific type of Internet addiction. Later, more sophisticated models to explain the different levels of the addiction process have been developed, such as the I-PACE model [
27], which is useful for understanding the development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders. The students who responded to the CERI 10 years ago did so thinking primarily about their connection to the Internet via their computer, whereas now they access the Internet indiscriminately from their mobile phones, other handheld devices, and home or university computer. One example of how Internet use has changed in recent years is the application WhatsApp. WhatsApp is a telephone messaging service but it shares many features with social networks and, as of 2016, it can be accessed from the computer. Therefore, students can use WhatsApp on their mobile phones or from their laptop when they are in the classroom and they create class groups on both WhatsApp and Facebook indiscriminately. Another example of the merging of lines between phone applications and Internet applications is the difficulty in distinguishing between how much time is dedicated to each application or program, since it is so common to work in multi-screen mode. Students can write an academic paper, answer emails, and have a conversation on WhatsApp, all at the same time.
The results of the comparison between the present and former studies with the CERM and the CERI show that the negative effects of Internet and mobile phone use are considerably stronger now that at the first survey in 2006. However, this is due mainly to the first period, between 2006 and 2013. Apparently, there is a downward tendency in the perception of negative consequences in the recent years, which may correspond to a progressive normalization and integration of these new technologies into our daily life.
Although we have already mentioned the limitations of the CERI and CERM (they were created in the cell phone era before the existence of the smartphone and they are self-report measures), they are easy to use and to score, thereby inviting us to continue using them when possible in order to study the evolution of the perception of the problematic use of the Internet and mobile phones. They indicate, not so much the prevalence of an addiction, but the perception of a problem by the respondents. Although the correlation of the CERI and CERM with the statement “I am addicted to the internet” was moderate, the participants who were “very much in agreement” and “agreed” with this statement obtained significantly higher results than the rest in both the CERI and the CERM. This indicates that it might be possible to use this single question to detect the perception of problematic mobile phone use of individuals, as has already been suggested [
46,
63]. It is highlighted that our students seem to have a perception about their Internet addiction more in line with the data obtained through the questionnaires than the participants in the study done by Pontes, Szabo, and Griffiths [
46], in which 51.9% of the participants identified themselves as Internet addicts.
This study is not without limitations. Firstly, university students have a higher than average level of academic development and their use of the Internet and mobile phones is not necessarily representative of the use that other young people engage in. Secondly, a problematic use of these technologies does not correspond to any diagnostic entity and may be a reflection of their social impact. Thirdly, both the CERI and the CERM should be updated, like other mobile addiction instruments (see, Kuss, Harkin, Kanjo and Billieux [
64]), because a general use of the Internet is no longer conceivable; rather, a specific one is, and more so because of the new and expanded uses of smartphones. The terms “internet addiction” and “smartphone addiction” could be ‘misnomers’ [
49] which we might not want to use anymore. Finally, it is possible that some differences found in this study were more influenced by the cultural differences between the samples than by the temporal differences.