Next Article in Journal
Pain Acceptance and Pain-Related Disability Predict Healthcare Utilization and Medication Intake in Patients with Non-Specific Chronic Spinal Pain
Next Article in Special Issue
Time Management by Young People in Social Difficulties: Suggestions for Improving Their Life Trajectories
Previous Article in Journal
Parental Opinion about the Low FODMAP Diet in Dietary Treatment of Children with Functional Abdominal Pain
Previous Article in Special Issue
Examining the Mediating Role of Motivation in the Relationship between Multidimensional Perfectionism and Well- and Ill-Being in Vocational Dancers
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Imbalances in the Study of the Relationship between Leisure and Self-Esteem: A Systematic Review

1
Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, University of Barcelona, 08035 Barcelona, Spain
2
Department of Applied Psychology, School of Psychology, University of Minho, 4710-057 Braga, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(15), 5555; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155555
Submission received: 17 June 2020 / Revised: 9 July 2020 / Accepted: 27 July 2020 / Published: 31 July 2020

Abstract

:
This systematic review offers a comprehensive examination of the relationship between leisure and self-esteem. The different perspectives were analyzed according to a framework that includes the different approaches for defining and measuring leisure, and a similar one was proposed for self-esteem. Articles indexed in the Web of Science (WoS) up to the end of 2018 were reviewed, specifically those that contained the keywords “leisure”, “self-esteem” or “self esteem” anywhere in the manuscript. Articles that did not present the qualitative or quantitative instruments needed to evaluate leisure or self-esteem were excluded. A total of 49 articles included the final quantitative synthesis. The overall findings showed that the prevailing methodology was objective (external). As regards content, the following combinations predominated: the behavioral approach to leisure with the unidimensional approach to self-esteem and the experiential approach to leisure with the unidimensional approach to self-esteem. Less studies were observed with the combination of mixed approaches and more comprehensive analyses: the behavioral-experiential combined with the multidimensional. To conclude, this study shows there is a demand for further empirical studies that explore the relationships between leisure and self-esteem. It also identified which approaches are most desirable to expand our understanding of the relationships between leisure and self-esteem.

1. Introduction

The relationship between leisure and self-esteem does not seem open to doubt, with many specialists insisting on its validity in recent decades (see, among others, [1,2,3]). It is a relationship of recognized importance where leisure is understood to be a creator of opportunities that have a resultant impact on self-esteem [4,5]. Leisure provides opportunities for the person enhance and to engage in freely chosen life events and experiences [6]—and this is related to the wellbeing of the person, since it refers to how “people feel good about themselves and feel that they are lovable and worthwhile people” [7] (p. 181).
However, some research has revealed distinctions in this relationship that invite a review of this field of study. For example, it has been found that an increase in self-esteem is associated with the practice of leisure activities that are significant for the person or activities where social support from others is perceived [8,9,10,11], while the practice of sedentary activities or activities whose difficulty causes stress is associated with a loss of self-esteem [12,13,14]. Furthermore, self-esteem is seen to be nourished by both leisure and non-leisure, to the point that work is more important than leisure for some people [15,16]. Despite these distinctions in the relationship between leisure and self-esteem, there are no articles that discuss them or further explore new findings. At the same time, no studies provide a theoretical reflection and guidance as regards which different aspects of leisure and self-esteem should be evaluated when studying their relationship. In the same vein, research is needed to assess what knowledge can be gathered by adopting different reference points and conceptual and methodological approaches. In other words, there is no research that makes a firm commitment to introducing the study of leisure into the exploration of the dynamics of self-esteem and which thereby facilitates the investigation and comprehensive understanding of this relationship.
Given these observations, this paper evaluates the current studies on leisure and self-esteem by analyzing the contribution made by conceptual and methodological approaches that serve to study both topics. Then, we draw on these approaches to present a systematic review of the scientific publications that empirically analyze the relationship between leisure and self-esteem. With this study, we reveal the possibilities and limitations of the research carried out so far and show what is needed to make progress towards a comprehensive understanding of the relationship between leisure and self-esteem.

1.1. Analyzing Leisure from a Plural Perspective

Although leisure is commonly understood from a lay perspective, among scientific literature a diversity of leisure definitions exists making difficult a unique definition of leisure [17]. Despite this, researchers have been trying to converge characterizing leisure, highlighting relevant dimensions and categories in order to understand the plurality of perspectives based such a multidisciplinary topic. According to this and apart from the different scientific lens, leisure can be characterized as a life time commonly associated to free time, as activities, as experiences, and/or as contexts, underlying the coexistence and relationships with the elements in one’s environment. Based on this plurality of perspectives, authors tend to agree that each one of them alone is insufficient, and thus, an integral/holistic analysis of the phenomenon should integrate all the valued aspects.
The comprehensive understanding of leisure, which counts on a plural, solid, well-grounded body of knowledge, also contemplates different approaches to its study, that is, an epistemological and methodological pluralism as regards the differences of conceptions about what is considered legitimate. Advocating in favor of a comprehensive study of leisure are Kleiber, Walker and Mannell [18,19], who developed a useful research framework based on different research approaches adopted to define and measure leisure. Specifically, their model makes it possible to organize, evaluate and orientate comprehensive research into the leisure phenomenon. The framework distinguishes two approaches to analysis (previously specified, among others, by Neulinger, Ellis and Witt, or Lawton [20,21,22]): objective leisure and subjective leisure. The objective approach focuses on the assessment of activities, settings or time periods; and the subjective approach contemplates certain types of meanings, experiences and satisfied needs, which derive from the significance of the activity and not from the activity itself. In addition to these, they distinguish two perspectives from which the phenomenon is defined and operationalized, based on the researcher’s viewpoint (external definitional) or the participant’s viewpoint (internal definitional). As a result of these different approaches to “what” and “how” leisure is analyzed, the authors plotted a two by-two matrix (Table 1.).
Four sets of data are obtained by combining the two perspectives of the object of study (objective or subjective behavior) and the two types of agent who structure the information (internal or external informant). We now briefly describe each of them.
Behavioral-Observer: The practice of an activity is recorded in a setting or time period. The researchers define leisure and non-leisure.
Experiential-Observer: Leisure is valued in terms of experience, satisfaction or meaning associated with involvement. The researchers define leisure and non-leisure.
Behavioral-Participant: The practice of an activity is recorded in a setting or time period. The participants define leisure and non-leisure.
Experiential-Participant: Leisure is valued in terms of experience, satisfaction or meaning associated with involvement. The participants define leisure and non-leisure.
As pointed out above, research into the phenomenon requires these four complementary approaches if the goal is to try to obtain a full picture or an understanding of leisure in all its complexity. In effect, and as stated by various authors, the divergences—between what researchers and participants consider leisure or between what defines the leisure experience for researchers and participants—enrich the corpus of knowledge because the researchers are obliged to explain and investigate the said divergences [23,24,25,26]. In this respect, Shaw [27] affirms that if leisure is only studied from the researcher’s standpoint, leisure that is significant for people may be neglected; and Godbey and Song [28] observe that the fact of having disregarded laypeople’s perspectives has hampered the growth of knowledge regarding the phenomenon of leisure in non-Western cultures. Consequently, the ideal solution is to combine the different approaches in the same study. However, this is not an easy task. One good alternative is that with respect to a topic, while individually different research projects may adopt one of the four approaches, all four could be taken into consideration, inasmuch as is possible, in the whole set of studies. Ultimately, this model is useful for evaluating to what extent, in the study of the relationship between leisure and self-esteem, leisure is analyzed comprehensively, maintaining a certain balance between different approaches or focusing on certain perspectives and research strategies.

1.2. Analyzing Self-Esteem in Its Dimensions

The above framework guides the study of leisure and we draw on it to propose a similar one for the comprehensive study of self-esteem. In general terms, the scientific literature understands self-esteem as a self-assessment–which generates feelings and emotions towards oneself that may range from approval to disapproval [29,30,31]. The APA [32] (p. 995) defines it as “the degree to which the qualities and characteristics contained in one’s self-concept are perceived to be positive”. But before detailing our proposal, we will briefly consider the approaches adopted to study self-esteem.
The concept of self-esteem has become increasingly popular in psychological and psychosocial studies in recent decades [33,34,35,36]. As pointed out by Habrat [37], this popularity has made it a household word thanks to the literature with limited scientific import that responds to certain specific problems [37,38,39]. On the other hand, self-esteem has also been thoroughly researched thanks to its popularity, which has worked in favor of its theoretical and empirical operationalization and shown that it is a construct of a complex nature with diverse dimensions.
The concept of self-esteem, in a more operative sense, has been conceptualized as both a unidimensional and a multidimensional phenomenon. Initially, self-esteem was conceived as a global construct, referred to in terms of a unidimensional model or global self-esteem. In this case, self-esteem is understood to be a positive or negative general attitude or feeling towards oneself [40]. Subsequently, the multidimensional conception of the self-emerged and gathered impetus thanks to the model proposed by Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton [41] (Figure 1). From this perspective, which also includes global self-esteem, it is understood that self-esteem derives from experiences in different areas of a person’s life, also called domains, dimensions or components, which are integrated by specific competences organized in a hierarchical manner. In other words, although the terms self-concept and self-esteem are sometimes used interchangeably, the latter is strictly limited to the evaluative aspect of self-concept, which derives from the conscious perceptions about oneself in different domains [32].
Consequently, given that a person’s life is affected by different areas of competence and relationships (family, school, work, social, physical, out-of-school, etc.), it is understood that the person has a specific self-image in each one of these areas and that the global self-image derives from the whole set [41,42,43]. Therefore, specific self-esteem is a better predictor of a specific behavior, and global self-esteem, of general wellbeing [33,35,37]. The versatility of the multidimensionality of the self, and above all its potential to explain and intervene, has promoted the development of different multidimensional models, which incorporate specific dimensions and subareas in order to identify what and how certain areas affect self-esteem (for example, the models developed by [44,45,46,47]).
In a more practical sense, the application of multidimensional models has shown, for example, that higher global self-esteem is related to physical self-esteem, which derives from participation in physical activities [48,49]. Thus, it would seem necessary to adopt a multidimensional perspective of the construct to discover the details of the relationship between leisure activities and self-esteem. If the work of Campbell, Eisner and Riggs [50] is adapted to the field of leisure, this premise can be supported by assumptions such as the following: If a person is very good at sport and bad at relationships, we will find this difference if we take a multidimensional approach, but we will not find this difference reflected in a global or one-dimensional approach that includes the dimensions of sport activities and relationships. On the other hand, the multidimensional approach also has its limitations. For example, the dimension of leisure and its associated attributes are not clearly delimited in multidimensional models and working with these models is much more costly in terms of the time required of participants. Furthermore, it is extremely difficult to standardize the instruments to ensure they are valid for participants with different ages and from different countries or cultures [51]. Having ascertained the possibilities and limitations of the multidimensional model, a successful research model to better understand the complexity of self-esteem would be one that combines standardized unidimensional and multidimensional approaches with qualitative methodological approaches or one that opts for a qualitative multidimensional approach [37,39,52,53,54,55].

1.3. Analyzing the Plurality of the Sources of Self-Esteem

Having detailed the conceptual and methodological considerations about self-esteem, we propose a framework that contemplates, on the one hand, the unidimensional and multidimensional analysis of self-esteem and, on the other hand, the analysis of the person’s self-evaluations, which can be carried out according to the content proposed by the person under study (internal) or the researcher (external) (Table 2). Therefore, the framework is structured as follows:
Four sets of data are obtained by combining the two concepts related to the object of study (unidimensional or multidimensional) and the two types of agent who structure the information (internal or external informant).
Unidimensional-observer: Self-esteem is understood to be a global self-evaluation. The researcher determines the specific questions that he/she considers as defining general self-esteem.
Multidimensional-observer: Self-esteem is understood to be the result of specific evaluations in different areas of everyday life. The researcher determines the dimensions and the specific questions that he/she considers as defining each specific dimension and general self-esteem.
Unidimensional-participant: Self-esteem is understood to be a global self-evaluation. The participant evaluates himself/herself on the basis of specific questions that, in his/her opinion, define general self-esteem.
Multidimensional-participant: Self-esteem is understood to be the result of specific evaluations in different areas of everyday life. The participant values himself/herself according to those dimensions that he/she considers relevant.
This framework for the analysis of self-esteem is based in the model proposed by Kleiber, Walker and Mannell [16] and also serves to assess the plurality of approaches to the study of the phenomenon. Then again, as regards the necessary complementarity of perspectives noted in the case of leisure, such complementarity in self-esteem emerges in the participant’s and the researcher’s perspectives, and the differences provide information that generates new knowledge [37,39,56]. On these lines, for example, it has been observed that the results obtained from unstructured and semi-structured interviews question the meaning of certain sections of the recommended scales. In particular, Schwan, Fallonbar and Milne, 2018 [57], using semi-structured interviews, found that what makes the homeless have positive feelings and emotions towards themselves is the creation of a product, the achievement of a goal, the discovery of themselves and so on—aspects that are not included in the recommended scales for research on self-esteem. However, as the authors themselves point out, these interviews would have to be complemented by the observer approach to more constructively enrich the body of knowledge. As to the unidimensional and multidimensional perspectives, their complementarity is relative because one of them, the multidimensional perspective, does not renounce the evaluation of global self-esteem. In this case, new knowledge emerges thanks to the precision with which the sources of self-esteem are evaluated, i.e., activities, contexts, relationships, etc., that affect self-esteem [35,36,37]. However, as pointed out above, the multidimensional approach has its limitations, and one in particular in the case of the study of leisure is the fact of not having worked seriously to ensure leisure is fully incorporated into the study of self-esteem.

1.4. About This Study

After delimiting the two concepts, we went on to explore the approaches used to study the relationship between leisure and self-esteem. The goal was to map the combination of approaches, identify the most and least popular, and discover the possibilities, limitations and challenges that this line of work presents with respect to the goal of developing a comprehensive body of knowledge. With this in mind, we carried out a systematic review of the published works that relate leisure to self-esteem. Specifically, we expected to find that the relationship between leisure and self-esteem had been analyzed and explained by combining the four approaches to analysis available in each case. In other words, as far as leisure is concerned, we expected this to have been studied from both objective and subjective perspectives as well as observational and participant ones, and regarding self-esteem, we expected this to have been studied from the unidimensional and multidimensional perspectives as well as observational and participant ones.

2. Materials and Methods

This review followed the 27 statements of the PRISMA guidelines for systematic reviews.
The search was carried out using the WoS database and concluded on 10 January 2020. In order to review complete years and due to the delay in updating the databases, the period reviewed finished on 31 December 2018 (any articles published and added to this database after that date were not included). The WoS Core Collection database was searched via the electronic interface and the Boolean search command used was “leisure” AND “self-esteem” OR “self-esteem”. Two reviewers judged independently whether the studies met the inclusion criteria. Any disagreements about whether or not to include studies were resolved between the authors. No third party was consulted. The criteria applied when considering studies for possible review were as follows.
The inclusion criteria were met by:
  • Articles published up to the end of December 2018.
  • Articles published in peer review journals indexed in the WoS Core Collection.
  • Articles containing the keywords “leisure” and “self-esteem” or “self esteem” anywhere in the manuscript.
The exclusion criteria were met by:
  • Articles without “leisure” and “self-esteem” or “self esteem” in the title, keywords or abstract.
  • Articles that did not include empirical research.
  • Articles without qualitative or quantitative instruments needed to evaluate self-esteem.
After considering all the search results, we organized the information according to the above criteria. A total of 49 articles were finally considered eligible for review, as shown in Figure 2.

3. Results

According to the WoS records, the total number of studies in which the relationship between leisure and self-esteem assumes some centrality and is empirically analyzed was 49 (Table 3). Regarding variable leisure and in line with the framework developed by Kleiber et al. [18], a total of 18 out of these 49 articles (Table 4) were included in the behavioral-observer category, and 12 in the experiential-observer category, highlighting an external definitional point in both cases. When looking for an internal definitional point, three articles were included in the behavioral-participant category, two articles in the experiential-participant category and one article in the observer-experiential participant category. Thus, and according to the type of phenomena, 21 articles were associated with behavioral phenomena and 14 with experiential phenomena. In addition, 13 articles were identified that form part of three categories (behavioral-observer, experiential-observer and behavioral-experiential participant). On the other hand, as far as the observer-participant approach is concerned, 30 papers valued leisure in terms of an observer approach, i.e., leisure studies where criteria or variables selected by the observer predominate. Finally, a minority consisting of just five articles analyzed leisure from the participant approach or the internal perspective and one combined the modalities of the observer and participant approaches. As regards the variable of self-esteem and its dimensionality, 43 articles conceptualized self-esteem as a unidimensional construct and six, as a multidimensional construct (Table 4). With respect of the observer or participant approach, only one article used the participant unidimensional approach.
As far as the joint study of leisure and self-esteem is concerned, on considering the different approaches to the study of leisure and the unidimensional and multidimensional approaches to the study of self-esteem, we found that in the case of behavioral approaches, whether behavioral-observer or behavioral-participant (or a combination of both), self-esteem is studied as a unidimensional phenomenon (22). This same tendency in the study of self-esteem is observed when the approach to the study of leisure is experiential (14). As reported in Table 4, multidimensional analyzes of self-esteem were found in papers where the study of leisure combines behavioral and experiential approaches. In effect, almost half of the studies (6) in this case analyzed the self from a multidimensional perspective (it was analyzed as unidimensional in the rest). Considering the studies with minority combinations, it is observed that only on one occasion—the sole case identified in this review—was self-esteem studied from an internal perspective, and leisure was analyzed using a behavioral-experiential-internal approach on this occasion.

4. Discussion

Although the scientific literature links leisure to self-esteem, the scientific research that strongly supports this relationship is limited. In our research, despite finding 154 articles that give the terms leisure and self-esteem a certain amount of importance—including them in the title, abstract or keywords, only in 49 cases were efforts made to empirically register both realities, the first in 1992 [102,103]. In other words, the relationship between leisure and self-esteem has limited significance in over a third of these articles: It is not empirically studied, nor is it mentioned in the discussion or conclusions. Consequently, if the number of publications in which these concepts are central is compared with the number of those that empirically investigate them, it can be seen that there are relatively few studies whose goal was to generate new knowledge in this field with a proven foundation. Thus, with respect to the assumption of the unquestionable relationship between the two concepts, a certain amount of liberty appears to have been taken by defending this idea without presenting any evidence, which does not favor the advance of knowledge.
Among those studies where the relationship between leisure and self-esteem is given particular attention, certain approaches take precedence, while others are minority or non-existent. Specifically, leisure is analyzed for the most part by applying observational methodologies in both the behavioral and experiential approaches. The participant approaches are in the minority, especially when conjugated with experiential approaches. As regards the study of self-esteem, the one-dimensional approach combined with observational methodologies predominates. This trend is also the predominant one in the study of self-esteem in other contexts and settings; a trend, to clarify the point, that uses the Rosemberg self-esteem scale as the main reference [37,38,39].
When the diversity of approaches in the investigation of both phenomena is compared, we see that leisure is the more plural field. Nonetheless, and as previously affirmed by Roberts [104], the prevalence of the behavioral-observational approach can be understood as an example of why leisure studies are not making as much progress as would be desirable. A main limitation is the search for a theory that explains leisure while considering that only one of the available theories is going to be correct and ignoring the fact that the different theories are complementary.
In the combination of approaches to the joint study of leisure and self-esteem, the dominant ones are observational (behavioral or experiential), in the case of leisure, and unidimensional-observational, in the case of self-esteem, followed by those that combine the behavioral and experiential observation of leisure with the unidimensional and multidimensional observation of self-esteem. Therefore, there is very limited pluralism regarding the way of collecting information. On the other hand, there is a relative degree of pluralism in the content of the studies; to be more precise, relative in the case of leisure but less so in the case of self-esteem. Consequently, given that the body of knowledge concerning the relationship between leisure and self-esteem is mainly nourished by observational approaches, the behaviors and other aspects experienced by the participants are not sufficiently valued. In effect, in the set of studies reviewed, only a limited number take into account people’s evaluations of their behaviors and leisure experiences and their relationship with global or specific self-esteem–and this is not a trivial matter. Precisely this internal approach can provide very valuable information regarding how leisure and self-esteem influence each other and how these variables affect people’s wellbeing. In this sense, one of the reviewed studies—by Schwan et al. [57] and featuring a behavioral-experiential-unidimensional participant approach—showed that artistic creation is understood by the participants as a survival strategy rather than a hobby. It is positively experienced due to the perception of agency rather than from satisfaction, and it is valued–depending fundamentally on the achievement of goals or targets and learning things about oneself, as a source of self-assessment, self-fulfillment, self-efficacy, happiness and wellbeing. Accordingly, the internal approach provides a valuable strategy for furthering the understanding of the relationships between leisure and self-esteem and the contributions of leisure and self-esteem to people’s wellbeing.
With regard to the aforesaid predominant observational approach, the research combinations reveal the following: The behavioral approach combined with the unidimensional approach associates leisure practices with a global perception of self-esteem, without managing to specify what experiential aspects of leisure are associated with self-esteem or whether other areas of everyday life have an impact on self-esteem. Consequently, there is no contrasted scientific support underpinning this combination of approaches to the relationship between leisure and self-esteem and its contribution to wellbeing. In fact, a leisure or non-leisure activity may be linked to self-esteem or to the perception of wellbeing, not by the activity itself but by the experiences associated with it. Furthermore, neither can this combination of approaches explain the contradictions regarding how leisure or a specific leisure behavior is related to an increase or not in self-esteem. The combination of the experiential and unidimensional approaches contemplates the evaluation of leisure and its relationship with global self-esteem. Therefore, the analysis is more precise in this case; it is the qualities of leisure (satisfaction, perception of competence, etc.) that are analyzed in relation to self-esteem. However, this does not explain what leisure behaviors are related to self-esteem. As for the somewhat more mixed approaches, those that combine the behavioral and experiential approaches with the unidimensional approach can identify which qualities of certain activities are associated with global self-esteem. Moreover, the most mixed methodologies found in this review—those that combine the behavioral and experiential approaches with the multidimensional approach—can serve to identify what activities and what qualities of the activities affect certain dimensions of self-esteem. In short, in the last case, which is the combination of observational approaches and found in the fewest studies, a more complete analysis of this relationship is carried out. It is the approach that best predicts the influence of specific behaviors on self-esteem. Nonetheless, all these approaches suffer from the limitation of starting out with the observer’s analysis and disregarding the experience of the subjects themselves, as explained above. It is also important to note that when the study of leisure combines the behavioral and experiential approaches, then self-esteem is considered from a multidimensional perspective in nearly half of the cases. A point that seems to indicate that when a more comprehensive approach to leisure is taken, the researchers are seeking a more exact understanding of the components that make up self-esteem.
The precedence that some approaches take over others could be justified by research tradition or by the resources available to obtain and/or analyze data. Regarding leisure, the potential of leisure practices as a promoter of change, adaptation and social transformation, for example, has led to the objective study of leisure from the perspective of the observer [105,106]. In the case of self-esteem, the premise of the role played by tradition and the available resources might be confirmed by the fact of the application (in two-thirds of the sample) of the self-esteem scale most widely used in the past and the easiest to apply: the Rosenberg scale [37,107]. On the other hand, the exploration of minority approaches might be inhibited by the limitations that we have highlighted regarding the application of standardized multidimensional models (application costs, standardization, etc.), the low popularity of participant approaches in the study of self-esteem and, as far as leisure studies are concerned, the fact of not having seriously integrated the challenge of studying self-esteem.
In a more practical sense, having applied the proposed frameworks to assess the study of leisure and self-esteem, we note among other limitations in this body of knowledge that there is a lack of information concerning the following: which dimensions of self-esteem are most benefited by leisure practices and experiences; which kinds of leisure not contemplated by the research contribute to self-esteem; the differences in the relationship between leisure and self-esteem for reasons of gender, culture and age and which subjective aspects of leisure are convergent with global self-esteem or with some of its dimensions.
Finally, if we are to deepen our understanding of the relationship between leisure and self-esteem, an important advance, apart from research where the participants structure information and the experiential and multidimensional approaches are valued, would be to develop research from a mixed perspective, i.e., with studies that combine the internal and external approach, as seen in one of the articles in this review [76]. This approach, which could be multi-method or mixed-methods, would constitute an important source of new findings, as shown by the leading role occupied by this methodological movement in recent years [56,108,109].
The systematic review presented in this article began with the analysis of one of the databases most used by researchers, i.e., one of the most important indexes of research trends. However, for a more complete and thorough analysis, it would be necessary to expand the sample with the results obtained from other scientific databases such as Scopus or PsycInfo. The analysis carried out also focused on “how” the relationship between leisure and self-esteem is studied; from which methodological and conceptual approaches. Nonetheless, studies with more applied goals should undoubtedly contemplate, for example, the valuations made about leisure activities in particular, the leisure experiences that most contribute to self-esteem, the differences based on gender, culture or age in the relationships between leisure and self-esteem, and the ages at which most attention is paid to the relationships between leisure and self-esteem. However, despite these limitations, this systematic review of the relationships between leisure and self-esteem reveals a hitherto unknown reality regarding the scientific fundamentals. It tests a series of frameworks to evaluate, locate and plan research work and reveal the dominant and minority approaches. It reflects on the possibilities and limits of different combinations, and it highlights those less explored and more promising approaches in terms of deepening the understanding of the relationships between these constructs.

5. Conclusions

With this systematic review, we bring to light some of the challenges facing research on leisure and self-esteem with regard to the attainment of specific knowledge, which has an impact on intervention strategies and programs in this field. In view of the results, it should be noted that the relationship between leisure and self-esteem research requires further research. In other words, there is a general need for more studies that rigorously assess the relationship between leisure and self-esteem, a relationship that is confirmed in numerous papers but to which only a few (49 since 1992) give any degree of centrality and work on empirically. In particular, more studies are needed to cover the different approaches to the analysis of leisure and self-esteem. In effect, the reviewed articles show that the observational approaches are the most dominant, which, although they provide important information, fail to register the participants’ points of view. Thus, with respect to leisure, there is a need for further investigation of what leisure activities and experiences are relevant to individuals and consequently significant for their self-esteem, there also being a need to record which aspects or areas of activity most affect it. The predominant trends in the study of this relationship lead us to conclude that the relationship between leisure and self-esteem is indeed documented and supported in various scientific works, but since there is very little pluralism, many nuances of this relationship remain unidentified; this is a relationship that is possibly much deeper and wide-ranging than what has been documented. In short, professionals involved in research on leisure and self-esteem face the challenge of revaluing this relationship. To obtain a comprehensive understanding of this topic, they need to take into account the different approaches, while continuing to attend to more practical aspects of the differences in this relationship (gender, age or culture, for example) and the implications of this relationship on individual wellbeing, a circumstance that depends on both leisure and self-esteem. In any case, this study shows, on the one hand, that the relationship between leisure and self-esteem is only partially understood and, on the other hand, it shows that it is necessary and possible to advance in this understanding by using the pluralism of conceptual and methodological approaches that serve to explore this relationship. In this respect, frameworks of reference are provided as a means of organizing, guiding and reflecting on the possibilities and limits of the different approaches. Furthermore, an assessment is made of those approaches that currently make a greater contribution to the understanding of the relationships between leisure and self-esteem.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization and methodology, N.C.; software, N.C.; validation, T.F.; formal analysis, N.C. and T.F.; investigation, N.C.; writing—original draft preparation, N.C.; writing—review and supervision, T.F.; funding acquisition, N.C. Both authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This contribution has received financial support from REDICE16-1320, University of Barcelona; RTI2018-094405-B-100, Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades of Spain, and the PsicoSAO-Research Group in Social, Environmental, and Organizational Psychology (2017 SRG 564 Comissionat per a Universitats i Recerca del DIUE of the Generalitat de Catalunya).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Dattilo, J.; Dattilo, A.M.; Samdahl, D.M.; Kleiber, D.A. Leisure orientations and self-esteem in women with low incomes who are overweight. J. Leis. Res. 1994, 26, 23–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Denovan, A.; Macaskill, A. Building resilience to stress through leisure activities: A qualitative analysis. Ann. Leis. Res. 2017, 20, 446–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Iwasaki, Y. Leisure and quality of life in an international and multicultural context: What are major pathways linking leisure to quality of life? Soc. Indic. Res. 2007, 82, 233–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Coleman, D.; Iso-Ahola, S.E. Leisure and health: The role of social support and self-determination. J. Leis. Res. 1993, 25, 111–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Shaw, S.M.; Kleiber, D.A.; Caldwell, L.L. Leisure and identity formation in male and female adolescents: A preliminary examination. J. Leis. Res. 1995, 27, 245–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Edginton, C.; Chen, P. Leisure Transformation, 2nd ed.; Sagamore: Urbana, IL, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  7. Fiske, S.T. Social Beings. Core Motives in Social Psychology, 3rd ed; Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  8. Costa-Giomi, E. Effects of three years of piano instruction on children’s academic achievement, school performance and self-esteem. Psychol. Music 2004, 32, 139–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Kim, J.; Iwasaki, Y. Role of leisure-generated meanings in adaptation to acculturation stress of Korean immigrants in Canada. Loisir Soc. Soc. Leis. 2016, 39, 177–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Thoits, P.A.; Hewitt, L.N. Volunteer work and well-being. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2001, 42, 115–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Walker, A. Active ageing: Realising its potential. Australas. J. Age. 2015, 34, 2–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Suchert, V.; Hanewinkel, R.; Isensee, B. Sedentary behavior and indicators of mental health in school-aged children and adolescents: A systematic review. Prev. Med. 2015, 76, 48–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Schultz, S. Theory of Occupational Adaptation. In Willard’s & Spackman’s Occupational Therapy, 11th ed.; Crepeau, E.B., Cohn, E.S., Schell, B.A., Eds.; Lippincott Williams & Wilkins: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2009; pp. 537–540. [Google Scholar]
  14. Tremblay, M.S.; LeBlanc, A.G.; Kho, M.E.; Saunders, T.J.; Larouche, R.; Colley, R.C.; Gorber, S. Systematic review of sedentary behaviour and health indicators in school-aged children and youth. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phy. Act. 2011, 8, 98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Kuykendall, L.; Craig, L.; Tay, L. Work-contingent self-esteem: A boon or bane for worker well-being? J. Organ. Behav. 2020, 41, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Reid, D.G.; Mannell, R.C. Future possibilities: The changing patterns of work and leisure. In Leisure and Tourism: Social and Environmental Chang; Veal, A.J., Jonson, P., Cushman, G., Eds.; University of Technology Sydney Press: Sydney, Australia, 1993; pp. 373–378. [Google Scholar]
  17. Purrington, A.; Hickerson, B. Leisure as a cross-cultural concept. World Leis. J. 2013, 55, 125–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kleiber, D.A.; Walker, G.J.; Mannell, R.C. A Social Psychology of Leisure, 2nd ed.; Venture: State College, PA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  19. Mannell, R.; Kleiber, D. A Social Psychology of Leisure; Venture: State College, PA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  20. Neulinger, J. The Psychology of Leisure; Thomas, C.C., Ed.; Springfield: Irvine, CA, USA, 1974. [Google Scholar]
  21. Ellis, G.; Witt, P. Conceptualization and Measurement of Leisure: Making the Abstract Concrete. In Recreation and Leisure: Issues in an Era of Change, 3rd ed.; Goodale, T., Witt, P., Eds.; Venture: State College, PA, USA, 1990; pp. 377–395. [Google Scholar]
  22. Lawton, M.P. Meanings of Activity. In Activity and Aging: Staying Involved in Later Life; Kelly, J.R., Ed.; Sage: Newbury Park, CA, USA, 1993; pp. 25–41. [Google Scholar]
  23. Anić, P.; Roguljić, D.; Švegar, D. What do students do in their free time and why? Pol. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 48, 504–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Ito, E.; Walker, G.J.; Mitas, O.; Liu, H. Cultural similarities and differences in the relationship between types of leisure activity and happiness in Canadian, Chinese, and Japanese university students. World Leis. J. 2019, 61, 30–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Parr, M.G.; Lashua, B.D. What is leisure? The perceptions of recreation practitioners and others. Leis. Sci. 2004, 26, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Walker, G.J.; Wang, X. The Meaning of Leisure for Chinese/Canadians. Leis. Sci. 2009, 31, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Shaw, S.M. The measurement of leisure: A quality of life issue. Loisir Soc. Soc. Leis. 1984, 7, 91–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Godbey, G.; Song, R. Finding Leisure in China; Venture: State College, PA, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  29. Brown, J.D.; Marshall, M.A. The Three Faces of Self-Esteem. In Self-Esteem Issues and Answers: A Sourcebook of Current Perspectives; Kernis, M.H., Ed.; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 4–9. [Google Scholar]
  30. Codina, N. Self-Esteem. In Encyclopedia of Leisure and Outdoor Recreation; Jenkins, J.M., Pilgram, J.J., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2003; p. 451. [Google Scholar]
  31. Hewitt, J.P. Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology; University Press: Oxford, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  32. VanderBos, G.R. APA Dictionary of Psychology, 2nd ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  33. Baumeister, R.F.; Campbell, J.D.; Krueger, J.I.; Vohs, K.D. Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness, or healthier lifestyles? Psychol. Sci. Publ. Int. 2003, 4, 1–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Donnellan, M.B.; Trzesniewski, K.H.; Robins, R.W. Measures of Self-Esteem. In Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Constructs; Boyle, G.J., Saklofske, D.H., Matthews, G., Eds.; Elsevier: San Diego, CA, USA, 2015; pp. 131–157. [Google Scholar]
  35. Rosenberg, M.; Schooler, C.; Schoenbach, C.; Rosenberg, F. Global self-esteem and specific self-esteem: Different concepts and different outcomes. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1995, 60, 141–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Zeigler-Hill, V. Self-Esteem; Psychology Press: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  37. Habrat, A. A Review of Selected Empirical Research on Self-Esteem. In The Role of Self-Esteem in Foreign Language Learning and Teaching Second Language Learning and Teaching; Habrat, A., Ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 47–75. [Google Scholar]
  38. Bednar, R.; Peterson, S. Self-Esteem: Paradoxes and Innovations in Clinical Theory and Practice, 2nd ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  39. Mruk, C.J. Self-Esteem Research, Theory, and Practice: Toward a Positive Psychology of Self-Esteem, 3rd ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  40. Rosenberg, M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1965. [Google Scholar]
  41. Shavelson, R.J.; Hubner, J.J.; Stanton, G.C. Self-concept: Validation of construct interpretations. Rev. Educ. Res. 1976, 46, 407–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Harter, S.; Bukowski, W.M. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  43. Wagner, J.; Hoppmann, C.; Ram, N.; Gerstorf, D. Self-esteem is relatively stable late in life: The role of resources in the health, self-regulation, and social domains. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 51, 136–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Bracken, B.A. Multidimensional Self-Concept Scale; Pro-Ed: Austin, TX, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  45. Fox, K.R. The Physical Self-Perception Profile Manual; North Illinois University Office of Health Promotion: DeKalb, II, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  46. Harter, S. The Construction of the Self; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  47. O’Brien, E.J.; Epstein, S. The Multidimensional Self-Esteem Inventory; Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  48. Esnaola, I.; Zulaika, L.M. Physical activity and physical self-concept in a sample of middle-aged Basque adults. Percept. Motor Skills 2009, 108, 479–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Fox, K.R.; Corbin, C.B. The physical self-perception profile: Development and preliminary validation. J. Sport Exercise Psy. 1989, 11, 408–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Campbell, R.L.; Eisner, S.; Riggs, N. Sources of self-esteem: From theory to measurement and back again. New Ideas Psychol. 2010, 28, 338–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Stewart, P.K.; Roberts, M.C.; Kim, K.L. The psychometric properties of the Harter Self-Perception Profile for children with at-risk African American females. J. Child. Fam. Stud. 2010, 19, 326–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Codina, N. El self y sus pluralidades: Un análisis desde el paradigma de la complejidad. Escr. Psicol. 2005, 7, 24–34. [Google Scholar]
  53. Egberink, I.J.L.; Meijer, R.R. An Item Response Theory Analysis of Harter’s Self-Perception Profile for Children or Why Strong Clinical Scales Should be Distrusted. Assessment 2011, 18, 201–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Devos, T.; Banaji, M.R. Implicit Self and Identity. In Handbook of Self and Identity; Leary, M.R., Tangney, J.P., Eds.; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 153–175. [Google Scholar]
  55. Harter, S. The Development of Self-Representations During Childhood and Adolescence. In Handbook of Self and Identity; Leary, M.R., Tangney, J.P., Eds.; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 610–642. [Google Scholar]
  56. Weindl, D.; Lueger-Schuster, B. Coming to terms with oneself: A mixed methods approach to perceived self-esteem of adult survivors of childhood maltreatment in foster care settings. BMC Psychol. 2018, 6, 47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Schwan, K.J.; Fallon, B.; Milne, B. “The one thing that actually helps”: Art creation as a self-care and health-promoting practice amongst youth experiencing homelessness. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2018, 93, 355–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Freire, T.; Teixeira, A. The influence of leisure attitudes and leisure satisfaction on adolescents’ positive functioning: The role of emotion regulation. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Jordan, K.A.; Gagnon, R.J.; Anderson, D.M.; Pilcher, J.J. Enhancing the college student experience: Outcomes of a leisure education program. J. Exper. Educ. 2018, 41, 90–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Nemcek, D. Self-esteem analyses in people who are deaf or hard of hearing: A comparison between active and inactive individuals. Phys. Act. Rev. 2017, 5, 95–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Kim, J.; Roh, E.Y.; Kim, G.; Irwin, L. Understanding the relationships between leisure satisfaction, self-esteem, family satisfaction, and life satisfaction among Korean individuals with intellectual disability. Ther. Recreat. J. 2016, 50, 265–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Uijtdewilligen, L.; Singh, A.S.; Chinapaw, M.J.M.; Twisk, J.W.R.; van van Mechelen, W. Person-related determinants of TV viewing and computer time in a cohort of young Dutch adults: Who sits the most? Scand. J. Med. Sci. Spor. 2015, 25, 716–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Sheridan, A.J.; Drennan, J.; Coughlan, B.; O’Keeffe, D.; Frazer, K.; Kemple, M.; Alexander, D.; Howlin, F.; Fahy, A.; Kow, V.; et al. Improving social functioning and reducing social isolation and loneliness among people with enduring mental illness: Report of a randomised controlled trial of supported socialisation. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatr. 2015, 61, 241–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Han, K.; Lee, Y.; Gu, J.; Oh, H.; Han, J.; Kim, K. Psychosocial factors for influencing healthy aging in adults in Korea. Health Qual. Life Out. 2015, 3, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Lai, C.K.Y.; Chan, E.A.; Chin, K.C.W. Who are the healthy active seniors? A cluster analysis. BMC Geriatr. 2014, 14, 127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Goltz, H.; Brown, T. Are children’s psychological self-concepts predictive of their self-reported activity preferences and leisure participation? Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 2014, 61, 177–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Mitchell, E.J.; Veitch, C.; Passey, M. Efficacy of leisure intervention groups in rehabilitation of people with an acquired brain injury. Disabil. Rehabil. 2014, 36, 1474–1482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Sangowawa, A.O.; Adebiyi, A.O. Factors associated with sexual abstinence among out-of-school females in a transitional town in Oyo State, South-Western Nigeria. Health Care Women Int. 2013, 34, 917–932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Noack, P.; Kauper, T.; Benbow, A.E.F.; Eckstein, K. Physical self-perceptions and self-esteem in adolescents participating in organized sports and religious groups. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 10, 663–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Jackson, A.C.; Francis, K.L.; Byrne, G.; Christensen, D.R. Leisure substitution and problem gambling: Report of a proof of concept group intervention. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2013, 11, 64–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Basurovic, N.; Svetel, M.; Pekmezovic, T.; Kostic, V.S. Evaluation of the quality of life in patients with segmental dystonia. Vojnosanit Pregled 2012, 69, 759–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Molina-Garcia, J.; Castillo, I.; Queralt, A. Leisure-time physical activity and psychological well-being in university students. Psychol. Rep. 2011, 109, 453–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Ng, S.T.; Tengku-Aizan, H.; Tey, N.P. Perceived health status and daily activity participation of older Malaysians. Asia Pac. J. Public Health 2011, 23, 470–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Casale, S.; Fioravanti, G. Psychosocial correlates of Internet use among Italian students. Int. J. Psychol. 2011, 46, 288–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Lindström, C.; Aman, J.; Norberg, A. Parental burnout in relation to sociodemographic, psychosocial and personality factors as well as disease duration and glycaemic control in children with type 1 diabetes mellitus. Acta Paediatr. 2011, 100, 1011–1017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Lecomte, T.; Corbiere, M.; Theroux, L. Correlates and predictors of optimism in individuals with early psychosis or severe mental illness. Psychosis 2010, 2, 122–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Huurre, T.; Lintonen, T.; Kaprio, J.; Pelkonen, M.; Marttunen, M.; Aro, H. Adolescent risk factors for excessive alcohol use at age 32 years. A 16-year prospective follow-up study. Soc. Psych. Psych. Epid. 2010, 45, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Gerber, M.; Puhse, U. Don’t crack under pressure!—Do leisure time physical activity and self-esteem moderate the relationship between school-based stress and psychosomatic complaints? J. Psychosom. Res. 2008, 65, 363–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Dergance, J.M.; Mouton, C.P.; Lichtenstein, M.J.; Hazuda, H.P. Potential mediators of ethnic differences in physical activity in older Mexican Americans and European Americans: Results from the San Antonio Longitudinal Study of Aging. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005, 53, 1240–1247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Ritsner, M.; Kurs, R.; Gibel, A.; Ratner, Y.; Endicott, J. Validity of an abbreviated quality of life enjoyment and satisfaction questionnaire (Q-LES-Q-18) for schizophrenia, schizoaffective, and mood disorder patients. Qual. Life Res. 2005, 14, 1693–1703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Wilton, R. Putting policy into practice? Poverty and people with serious mental illness. Soc. Sci. Med. 2004, 58, 25–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Gana, K.; Alaphilippe, D.; Bailly, N. Positive illusions and mental and physical health in later life. Aging Ment. Health 2004, 8, 58–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Blairy, S.; Linotte, S.; Souery, D.; Papadimitriou, G.N.; Dikeos, D.; Lerer, B.; Kaneva, R.; Milanova, V.; Serretti, A.; Macciardi, F.; et al. Social adjustment and self-esteem of bipolar patients: A multicentric study. J. Affect. Disord. 2004, 79, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Seidman, E.; Lambert, L.E.; Allen, L.; Aber, J.L. Urban adolescents’ transition to junior high school and protective family transactions. J. Early Adolesc. 2003, 23, 166–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Ritsner, M. Predicting changes in domain-specific quality of life of schizophrenia patients. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2003, 191, 287–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Waters, L.E.; Moore, K.A. Reducing latent deprovation during unemployment: The role of meaningful leisure activity. J. Occup. Organ. Psych. 2002, 75, 15–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Waters, L.E.; Moore, K.A. Self-esteem, appraisal and coping: A comparison of unemployed and reemployed people. J. Occup. Organ. Psych. 2002, 23, 593–604. [Google Scholar]
  88. Petryshen, P.M.; Hawkins, J.D.; Fronchak, T.A. An evaluation of the social recreation component of a community mental health program. Psychiatr. Rehabil. J. 2001, 24, 293–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Dedobbeleer, N.; Desjardins, S. Outcomes of an ecological and participatory approach to prevent alcohol and other drug “abuse” among multiethnic adolescents. Subst. Use Misuse. 2001, 36, 1959–1991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Tiggemann, M. The Impact of Adolescent Girls’ Life Concerns and Leisure Activities on Body Dissatisfaction, Disordered Eating, and Self-Esteem. J. Genet. Psychol. 2001, 162, 133–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Darling, N.; Dowdy, B.B.; Van Horn, M.L.; Caldwell, L.L. Mixed-sex settings and the perception of competence. J. Youth Adolesc. 1999, 28, 461–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Shapira, B.; Zislin, J.; Gelfin, Y.; Osher, Y.; Gorfine, M.; Souery, D.; Mendlewicz, J.; Lerer, B. Social adjustment and self-esteem in remitted patients with unipolar and bipolar affective disorder: A case-control study. Compr. Psychiatry 1999, 40, 24–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Philipp, S.F. Race, gender, and leisure benefits. Leis. Sci. 1997, 19, 191–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Kemmler, G.; Holzner, B.; Neudorfer, C.; Meise, U.; Hinterhuber, H. General life satisfaction and domain-specific quality of life in chronic schizophrenic patients. Qual. Life Research. 1997, 6, 265–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Barnett, M.A.; Vitaglione, G.D.; Harper, K.K.G.; Quackenbush, S.W.; Steadman, L.A.; Valdez, B.S. Late adolescents’ experiences with and attitudes toward videogames. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 27, 1316–1334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Hull, R.B.; Michael, S.E.; Walker, G.J.; Roggenbuck, J.W. Ebb and flow of brief leisure experiences. Leis. Sci. 1996, 18, 299–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Gordon, W.R.; Caltabiano, M.L. Urban–rural differences in adolescent self-esteem, leisure boredom, and sensation-seeking as predictors of leisure-time usage and satisfaction. Adolescence 1996, 31, 883–901. [Google Scholar]
  98. Melamed, S.; Meir, E.I.; Samson, A. The benefits of personality-leisure congruence: Evidence and implications. J. Leis. Res. 1995, 27, 25–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Colwell, J.; Grady, C.; Rhaiti, S. Computer games, self-esteem and gratification of needs in adolescents. J. Community Appl. Soc. 1995, 5, 195–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Coyle, C.P.; Lesnikemas, S.; Kinney, W.B. Predicting life satisfaction among adults with spinal cord injuries. Rehabil. Psychol. 1994, 39, 95–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Raymore, L.A.; Godbey, G.C.; Crawford, D.W. Self-esteem, gender, and socioeconomic status: Their relation to perceptions of constraint on leisure among adolescents. J. Leis. Res. 1994, 26, 99–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Slater, J.; Groves, D.L.; Lengfelder, J. Correctional recreation and its impact: Self-esteem components of prison inmates. Psychol. J. Hum. Behav. 1992, 29, 27–39. [Google Scholar]
  103. Kinney, W.B.; Coyle, C.P. Predicting life satisfaction among adults with physical disabilities. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1992, 73, 863–869. [Google Scholar]
  104. Roberts, K. Writing about leisure. World Leis. J. 2016, 60, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Mair, H.; Reid, D.G. Leisure research and social change: A millennial state of the art. Leis. Loisir 2007, 31, 501–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Rojek, C. The Labour of Leisure. The Culture of Free Time; Sage: London, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  107. Goodson, P.; Buhi, E.R.; Dunsmore, S.C. Self-Esteem and Adolescent Sexual Behaviors, Attitudes, and Intentions: A Systematic Review. J. Adolesc. Health 2006, 38, 310–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Anguera, M.T.; Camerino, O.; Castañer, M.; Sánchez, P. Mixed methods in research into physical activity and sport. Rev. Psicol. Deporte 2014, 23, 123–130. [Google Scholar]
  109. Zedeck, S. APA Dictionary of Statistics and Research Methods; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Representation of a multidimensional, hierarchical model (Adapted from: Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton [41], p. 413; original version copyright is the American Educational Research Association).
Figure 1. Representation of a multidimensional, hierarchical model (Adapted from: Shavelson, Hubner and Stanton [41], p. 413; original version copyright is the American Educational Research Association).
Ijerph 17 05555 g001
Figure 2. Selection of eligible articles.
Figure 2. Selection of eligible articles.
Ijerph 17 05555 g002
Table 1. Research approaches for defining and measuring leisure [16] (p. 58). (adapted from: Kleiber, Walker and Mannell [18]).
Table 1. Research approaches for defining and measuring leisure [16] (p. 58). (adapted from: Kleiber, Walker and Mannell [18]).
ExternalInternal
ObjectiveBehavioral-ObserverBehavioral-Participant
SubjectiveExperiential-ObserverExperiential-Participant
Table 2. Research approaches in the conception and investigation of self-esteem (prepared by the authors).
Table 2. Research approaches in the conception and investigation of self-esteem (prepared by the authors).
ExternalInternal
UnidimensionalUnidimensional-ObserverUnidimensional-Participant
MultidimensionalMultidimensional-ObserverMultidimensional-Participant
Table 3. Articles selected.
Table 3. Articles selected.
ArticleResearch Approaches to Defining and Measuring LeisureLeisure InstrumentResearch Approaches to Defining and Measuring Self-EsteemSelf-Esteem Instrument
[57]Behavioral-experiential-participantInterviewUnidimensional-participantSemi-structured interview
[58]Experiential-observerLeisure Attitude Scale (Short Version), Leisure Satisfaction ScaleUnidimensional-observerRSES *
[59]Behavioral-observerAd hoc questionnaireUnidimensional-observerRSES
[60]Behavioral-observerAd hoc questionnaireUnidimensional-observerRSES
[61]Experiential-observerPanel Survey of Employment
for the Disabled (PSED, 2012)
Unidimensional-observerRSES
[9]Experiential-observerMeaning, Global Leisure Meanings (2016)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[62]Behavioral-observerAd hoc questionnaireUnidimensional-observerDutch Personality Inventory
[63]Behavioral-observerAd hoc questionnaireUnidimensional-observerRSES
[64]Behavioral-observerSeok Questionnaire (2005)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[65]Behavioral-observerLawton Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale (IADL)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[66]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerChildren’s Assessment of Participation and Enjoyment (CAPE) and Preferences for Activities of Children (PAC)Multidimensional-observerPiers-Harris Self Concept Scale (Piers Harris-2)
[67]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerLeisure Participation Survey (SWBIRS) and Leisure Satisfaction Scale (1980)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[68]Behavioral-observerSemi-structured interviewUnidimensional-observerRSES
[69]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerAd hoc: index of sport activity and items measuring the self-efficacy, competence and attractiveness in the physical domainMultidimensional-observerRSES and Facets of the physical self-concept (2004)
[70]Behavioral-observerGambling Activity Measurement Tool (GAMT)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[71]Behavioral-observerLeisure Activities Scale (1978)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[72]Behavioral-observerWHO (1995)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[73]Behavioral-participantDaily activities reported by the respondentsUnidimensional-observerRSES
[74]Behavioral-observerGeneralized Problematic Internet Use Scale (GPIUS, 2002)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[75]Behavioral-participantOpen questionsUnidimensional-observerPerformance-Based Self-Esteem scale (PBSE)
[76]Behavioral-observerClient Assessment of Strengths Interests and Goals (CASIG, 2001)Unidimensional-observerSelf-Esteem Rating Scale-Short-Form (SERS-SF, 2006)
[77]Behavioral-observerQuestions about lifestyle: 1 item: leisure-time daily among friendsUnidimensional-observerRSES
[78]Behavioral-observerAd hoc questionnaireUnidimensional-observerRSES
[79]Behavioral-observer and Behavioral-participantMinnesota Leisure Time Physical Activity Questionnaire and Four open-ended questions to elicit additional activitiesUnidimensional-observerRSES
[80]Experiential-observerQuality of Life Enjoyment and Satisfaction Questionnaire (Q-LES-Q) and Lancashire Quality of Life Profile (LQOLP)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[81]Experiential-observerQuality of Life (QOL)Unidimensional-observerQOL
[82]Experiential-participantAd hoc questionnaireUnidimensional-observerRSES
[83]Experiential-observerSocial adjustment scale (SAS, 1976)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[84]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerAdolescent-perceived Microsystems Scales: Social support, daily hassles and involvement (1995)Multidimensional-observerSelf-Perception Profile for Adolescents (1987)
[85]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerQuality of Life Enjoyment and Satisfaction Questionnaire (Q-LES-Q)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[86]Experiential-observerMeaningful Leisure Activities Questionnaire (1999)Unidimensional-observerGlobal Self-Worth subscale (1986) Adult Self-Perception Profile (ASPP)
[87]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerAd hoc Questionnaire social and solitary leisure activities and Meaningful Leisure Activities Questionnaire (1999)Unidimensional-observerGlobal Self-Worth (1986) Adult Self-Perception Profile (ASPP)
[88]Experiential-observerQuality Life Interview (1988), Satisfaction with leisure activitiesUnidimensional-observerRSES
[89]Behavioral-observerQuestionnaire about different leisure activities and placesUnidimensional-observerRSES
[90]Behavioral-observerUni ad hoc itemUnidimensional-observerRSES
[91]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerParticipation and evaluationMultidimensional-observerSelf-Perception Profile for Adolescents (1987)
[92]Behavioral-observerSocial Adjustment Scale (SAS)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[93]Experiential-observerLeisure Benefit ScaleUnidimensional-observerSelf-esteem = “feeling good about myself,”
[94]Experiential-observerLancashire QOL (Quality of Life) ProfileUnidimensional-observerLancashire Quality of Life Profile
[95]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerQuestions about amount of time and evaluationsUnidimensional-observerRSES
[96]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerItems describing experiences about leisure in natural areasUnidimensional-observerItems for self-esteem adapted
[97]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerAdolescent Leisure-Time Use InventoryMultidimensional-observerSelf-Rating Scale (SRS, 1984)
[98]Behavioral-participantRequest to list non work activitiesUnidimensional-observerInventory used by Meir, Melamed and Abu-Freha (1990).
[99]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerAd hoc instrument: frequency, duration and feelingsMultidimensional-observerBatlle’s Culture Free Self-esteem
[1]Experiential-observerLeisure Orientation Scale (LOS)Unidimensional-observerRSES
[100]Behavioral-observer and Experiential-observerActivity of Daily Living Scale (ADL, 1963) and Five Leisure Satisfaction QuestionsUnidimensional-observerRSES
[101]Experiential-observerMeasure of Perceived constraints on leisureUnidimensional-observerRSES
[102]Behavioral-observerAd hoc checklistUnidimensional-observerCoopersmith Inventory
[103]Experiential-participantLeisure satisfaction interviewUnidimensional-observerRSES
* RSES = Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (1965).
Table 4. Research approaches in the study of the relationship between leisure and self-esteem.
Table 4. Research approaches in the study of the relationship between leisure and self-esteem.
Definitional Vantage PointTotal
LeisureObserverParticipantObserver-Participant
Self-EsteemUD-EUD-IMD-EMD-IUD-EUD-IMD-EMD-IUD-EUD-IMD-EMD-I
Behavioral180003000100022
Experiential120002000000014
Behavioral-experiential60600100000013
Total360605100100049
UD = Unidimensional; MD = Multidimensional; E = External or observer; I = Internal or participant.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Codina, N.; Freire, T. Imbalances in the Study of the Relationship between Leisure and Self-Esteem: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5555. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155555

AMA Style

Codina N, Freire T. Imbalances in the Study of the Relationship between Leisure and Self-Esteem: A Systematic Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(15):5555. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155555

Chicago/Turabian Style

Codina, Nuria, and Teresa Freire. 2020. "Imbalances in the Study of the Relationship between Leisure and Self-Esteem: A Systematic Review" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 15: 5555. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155555

APA Style

Codina, N., & Freire, T. (2020). Imbalances in the Study of the Relationship between Leisure and Self-Esteem: A Systematic Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(15), 5555. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17155555

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop