1. Introduction
Over several decades, the “activity perspective” [
1] has gained footing in social gerontology and political communities. The “activity perspective” represents new standards of ageing “well” by rejecting that old age is inevitably associated with deficits, decline, and disengagement; rather, the “activity perspective” holds that inactivity in old age is usually involuntary. Two strands of research in social gerontology support the “activity perspective”: “successful ageing” and “active ageing.” These two perspectives have different roots and meanings [
2,
3,
4,
5]. The “successful ageing” concept, which is predominant in US ageing discourses, focuses on e.g., appropriate lifestyles enabling wellbeing and the participation of older adults [
6]. In contrast, “active ageing”—the subject of this article—is primarily a concept [
1,
5] widely used in Europe over the last 15–20 years.
Still, there is no consensus on the actual meaning of “active ageing” [
1]. It is a polysemic concept, subject to different theoretical approaches, analytical perspectives, and empirical observations. Some have used the WHO [
7] definitions of active ageing as a test case and found that psychological variables are important contributors to the construct (e.g., [
8]). Others have developed a model of active ageing that focuses on the individual level, analysing the impact of active ageing on life satisfaction and how coping styles may predict active ageing [
9], while Barrio et al. [
10] claim that the discourse of active ageing has paved the way towards active citizenship in later life. It has also been analysed how the social policies of active ageing are not in agreement with older adults’ wishes and aspirations [
11]. Thus, a multiplicity of micro and macro level approaches offers different theoretical and analytical guidance as to how to study active ageing.
In this article, our point of departure is the blueprint of active ageing that has been developed in the active ageing index (AAI) project, defining active ageing as a “situation where people continue to participate in the formal labour market as well as engage in other unpaid productive activities (such as care provision to family members and volunteering) and live healthy, independent, and secure lives as they age” [
12]. The purpose of the AAI project is to measure and promote the realisation of older adults’ active ageing potential, and the project is financed jointly by the European Commission (EC) and the United Nations Economic Commission for Europe (UNECE) [
13,
14]. The AAI project occupies a prominent position in discourses about active ageing. It is highly influential politically, and it has become a central reference point in academic reflections about active ageing. Thus, the AAI project is considered (by some) to be a practical “manifestation” of active ageing ideation [
15].
As the AAI project occupies a central position in discussions about active ageing, the aim of this article is to critically to scrutinise the conceptual and empirical framework of the AAI project, meaning that we are challenging the AAI. Our critical analysis of the AAI project is divided into two parts:
First, we assess how the AAI project has conceptualised the construct of active ageing. Thus the aim of the first part of this article is to present the theoretical framework of the AAI-project, and then to develop a theoretical critique and show analytical shortcomings in the construct of the AAI-active ageing concept.
Second, the developers of the AAI-project have not yet addressed whether inactivity among older adults is actually involuntary [
11,
16], which means that social inequalities in active ageing prospects and the structuring mechanisms behind such inequalities have so far been under-researched. In effect, the second part of this article is an
empirical analysis guided by the following research question: is AAI-active ageing realistic—and for whom?
When we test the premises embedded in the AAI-project empirically, we make use of the conceptual framework developed in the AAI-project as our point of departure. Hence, we reconstruct the AAI-project in that we make use of the same concepts (the four domains: (1) employment, (2) participation in society, (3) independent living, and (4) capability to age actively), variables, operationalisation, and indicators of active ageing as the AAI-project.
In analysing empirically whether AAI-active ageing is realistic, we draw primarily on two hypotheses: first, we hypothesise that active ageing is dependent on individual resources, given that numerous studies have shown, for instance, that older workers’ labour market activities (the employment domain) are conditioned by factors such as health, education, and age. Second, given that health (belonging to AAI-domain 4: capability to age actively) may precondition labour market participation (AAI-domain 1: employment), we furthermore hypothesise that there is a spillover between the different domains or that a Matthew effect of accumulated advantages exists, indicating that older adults who are blessed in one domain or sphere of life are also blessed in others—and vice versa. These hypotheses indicate that active ageing is only for the blessed and most privileged older adults.
Our analytical endeavour differs from the AAI-project. The AAI-project seeks to identity active ageing achievements and room for improvements, while we analyse active ageing inequalities. We therefore make use of a different metric than the AAI-project. We have thus chosen to apply the multiple correspondence analysis (MCA), as this metric is especially suited to identifying patterns of social inequalities, as well as the main factors structuring such inequalities.
Unfortunately, we are not able fully to replicate the variables of the AAI project. We draw on the Danish Senior Data Base (DSDB), comprising 9965 interviews with older adults 52–87 years of age. Regrettably, survey questionnaires used in the AAI and DSDB projects do not match one-to-one, meaning that the data used in this article are an approximation of the AAI data.
As indicated, the article begins with a critical assessment of the AAI-active ageing approach. Then we move on to the empirical analysis, and the article ends with a discussion and conclusion.
2. The AAI-Active Ageing Framework and Its Shortcomings
Like the notion of active ageing in general, the AAI-active ageing concept is founded on the idea that active ageing is beneficial for all older adults and the wider community alike [
12,
17]. The wider community benefits because active ageing reduces the financial burden of demographic ageing on society. Individuals benefit because participation in the formal labour market, as well as engagement in other unpaid productive activities (such as caring for family members and volunteering), is expected to empower older persons and allow them fully to realise their potential [
12,
18].
More specifically, the AAI project is designed to monitor developments and progress in the realisation of active ageing within the EU area. The project is based on the assumption that active ageing occurs within four domains in society: (1) employment, i.e., participation in the labour market; (2) participation in society, i.e., participating in voluntary activities, caring for children and grandchildren etc.; (3) independent living, i.e., doing physical exercise, access to health and dental care, independent living, etc., and (4) capability to age actively, i.e., share of healthy life expectancy at 55, mental well-being, use of ICT (internet use), etc. [
5].
Within each domain, countries are ranked on a scale of 0–100, indicating country-specific achievements of active ageing. By crosscutting the four domains, an overall or aggregated index has been constructed that summarises all domains together. The overall ranking of countries in the ‘Active Ageing Index 2012’ showed that Sweden was the highest-ranking country (44.0 points) and Poland the lowest-ranking country (27.3 points) in Europe [
14]. The developers of the AAI further argue that the distance from a maximum of 100 points indicates untapped potential [
18] or room for improvements. This would mean that the index construction is based on the normative ideal that all countries score 100 in all four domains, which is unrealistic as Sweden currently reaches only 44 points.
This raises an index construction issue. Critics of the AAI have argued that social inequalities (e.g., gender, ethnicity, class, age) most likely condition opportunities for active ageing [
13,
14,
15,
17,
19,
20,
21], and that inactivity may mirror the fact that some segments of older adults are unable to comply with the norms of active ageing due to structural, social, physical, or mental limitations. As such, the question becomes whether the AAI index measures (a) structural properties of different societies that may be hard to overcome as an act of will or (b) achievements of individuals at the micro level—or “room for improvement” due to involuntary inactivity—of the countries.
Another index construction issue is that on the micro level individuals cannot be fully active in all four domains due to the limitations of time (cf. [
20]). Thus, to comply with the logic of the AAI project, older adults are expected to participate in the labour market; care for grandchildren and frail, elderly relatives; do volunteer work; engage in political activities and lifelong learning; perform physical exercise; etc. However, the AAI index disregards the limitations of cross-domain activities, indicating that “room for improvement” may also be limited.
Furthermore, a basic question is whether active ageing is actually in accordance with older adults’ wishes, dispositions, and aspirations; that is, are older adults really
willing to become active according to the ideas of active ageing? This is an empirical question not addressed by the AAI project. Sufficient financial security and good mental and physical health are most likely central AAI-indicators that would be widely supported by older adults. However, in relation to the employment domain, for example, it is safe to say that older workers’ orientations towards the labour marked differ markedly across Europe. The ISSP 2015 Work Orientations module [
22] reveals that the proportion of older adults 56–65 years of age that “strongly agree” or “agree” with the statement that they would “Enjoy a paid job even if I did not need the money” was e.g., 67 per cent in Denmark and 34 per cent in Finland. With the Finnish figures in the back of one’s mind, it becomes hard to claim that all older adults all over Europe will feel themselves ‘empowered’ or that they have been fully allowed to ‘realise their potential’ if they are forced to work longer as an outcome of active ageing policies, such as pension scheme changes [
23]. Of course, the decision to raise state pension ages all over Europe is not necessarily an external force pressuring older workers to continue working. Nonetheless, as state pension ages are rising, most older workers have no choice but to prolong their working lives.
This indicates that some dimensions of active ageing are more beneficial for the welfare state and employers than for older adults, often conceptualised as a productive notion of ‘active ageing’ [
18]. It is therefore no surprise that the AAI project is fostered by the political system and managed and financed jointly by the European Commission (EC) and the United Nations Economic Commission for Europe (UNECE) [
24]. Accordingly, the primary aims of the AAI project become the identification of public policies and programmes that may enhance or function as a prerequisite for active ageing [
12,
25,
26]. Such findings are transported back into the political system, wherein knowledge about factors enhancing active ageing is supposedly used to rethink or reformulate strategies promoting active ageing [
5]. As such, social science researchers working with active ageing are predominantly communicating with the political system, epitomised as a wide range of “stakeholders, such as policy-makers, researchers, students and businesses” [
13].
It is sometimes argued that active ageing, in contrast to the successful ageing concept, represents a “sociological turn,” meaning that active ageing is not preconditioned by (internal) psychological features (as the successful ageing concept prescribes) but rather by external opportunity structures [
5]. However, the AAI perception of active ageing is marked by major theoretical and analytical shortcomings. The AAI project is undertheorised, and the ideas informing the AAI vision are rarely discussed or justified [
2,
16,
20]. From a theoretical point of view, concepts and distinctions seem arbitrarily constructed. For instance, domains—a central dimension of the AAI-active ageing framework—originate from an EU political discourse about “societies for all ages”, wherein the Council of the European Union has reaffirmed in 2012 that active ageing must “be promoted in the three domains of employment, participation in society and independent living” [
12]. This clearly indicates that the choice of domains is politically informed and defined.
Hence, the AAI conceptual framework does not clarify the constituents or determinants of active ageing, which—at the individual level—may be largely dependent on individual resources. Within the “employment” domain, for instance, numerous studies have shown how the timing of retirement is conditioned by factors such as health, education, and age [
27], while studies within the participation-in-society domain have shown that engagement in volunteer work depends on factors such as the participants’ levels of education [
28,
29]. Even Barslund, von Werder and Zaidi [
30] have, within the framework of the AAI project, shown that vertical or hierarchical inequalities in active ageing do exist.
Horizontally, very little is known about the relationships between the different domains; for example, how does the “employment” domain interact with the “participation in society” domain? Such interactions likely do exist, and Asghar Zaidi, who coordinated the AAI project, and colleagues have made a few empirical analyses mapping the spillover from one domain to another. For instance, they have found a strong correlation linking Domain 4 (capability to age actively) to Domain 2 (participation in society) and Domain 3 (independent living) [
25]. This suggests that different activities and individual resources are interdependent [
19], meaning that a high score in one domain is associated with high scores in other domains. However, the structuring mechanisms behind such spillover effects are unknown. At the individual level, such interdependence is most likely intermediated by factors such as, e.g., educational attainment, given that (1) the timing of retirement, as well as (2) the participation in volunteer work as already mentioned, seems to depend on level of education.
When measuring the degree of active ageing within the four domains, it is obviously of utmost importance that there is a minimum of logic in the choice of indicators. However, the choice of indicators is marked by a lack of transparency and by an element of arbitrariness [
15]. The developers of the AAI have chosen indicators that are simple, understandable, and comparable [
12]. Potentially relevant indicators that cannot be compared across countries based on existing statistics are excluded. The choice of indicators was also influenced by comments and feedback from the UNECE Expert Group, which functioned as an advisory board on the AAI project [
24]. Minutes from these expert meetings are publicly accessible (e.g., [
31]). In the minutes, the “opinions” of experts are reported. But the foundation of these opinions and how they have been translated into the indices are not explained.
In the analysis, different domains and indicators have been assigned different weights, meaning that the choice of weights in the measurement is crucial for the results [
21]. However, the developers of the AAI have made clear that the weights are “admittedly arbitrary” and without any “theoretical guidance” [
25]. Rather, the weights “reflect the political relevance as perceived by the Experts” [
24]. Thus the AAI project results represent normative judgements based on expert opinions, leaving us with the old Baconian epistemic problem, meaning that domains, indicators, and weights would have undoubtedly looked rather different if the advisory board had been differently composed, e.g., composed of Scandinavian feminist scholars engaged in ageing research.
Another basic problem of the AAI project is that it neglects differences in value systems and cultural orientation across Europe, i.e., it adheres to a unidimensional cultural model that is insensitive to cultural diversity across European countries [
20]. For instance, in the Participation in Society domain, the score is high if older people function as carers for others, typically their own frail parents (or spouses) and their grandchildren. This, however, is based on norms and values predominant in southern European—and, to some extent, central European countries—and within the framework of the male breadwinner model, whereas in northern European countries like Denmark with its dual-earner family structure, it is the obligation of the welfare state to care for frail older persons [
32]. In effect, Denmark scores much lower on indicators such as ‘care for children, grandchildren’ compared to e.g., Italy, because the Danish and Italian welfare states are founded on different ideals, which means that the developers of the AAI have developed concepts, distinctions, and reflections on goals and means that may be disconnected from the social reality in most European countries. One may therefore ask: what is the basic notion of society from which the AAI project develops its ideals and distinctions?
That the AAI project is disconnected from any coherent theory of society or systematic information about older adults’ dispositions, aspirations, and characteristics has the effect that there is no compulsive logic in how concepts and distinctions are evolved. Conceptual combinations are arbitrary. The AAI conceptual framework is nonetheless organised according to a hidden “logic”; namely, the logic of the dichotomous organisation of concepts, constituting the world as either “in or out”, “before or after”, “good or bad”, etc. The AAI project is thus based on “a positive normative judgement meaning that the higher the value, the better the active ageing outcome” [
12], which has profound effects on researchers’ possible insights and theoretical positions. As researchers, we focus on what is measurable and on registering a position on one side or the other of a boundary. In this endeavour, we observe according to an “objective” criterion (e.g., older or younger than age 60) or by disregarding an “objective” criterion (man or women). This dichotomous approach has obvious advantages; it permits distinction between political intention and scientific observation. Intention is a political domain, whereas the scientific task is to register and deliver knowledge about achievements in active ageing, irrespective of the fact that the researcher’s choice of concepts and approach is informed by politicians, i.e., is normatively and politically motivated.
4. Results of the Empirical Analysis of Active Ageing
The first step in the analysis is to select a relevant number of axes for interpretation. Each of the MCA axes sums up a decreasing part of the total variance of the model. In our model, there are marked reductions in the explained variance after the third axis, indicating that three axes can be interpreted. We present the first three axes summing to an accumulated modified rate of 68%. Axis 1 is related to all of the active-ageing domains, indicating a correlation between indicators from the different domains. The variables contributing the most are ICT (24%), independent living arrangements (16%), and taking care of older adults (13%). The variables contributing the most to Axis 2 are self-rated health (37%), mental wellbeing (24%), and access to health care (18%). Axis 3 primarily relates to physical exercise (26%) and self-rated health (23%) but also to voluntary activities (13%) and independent living arrangements (13%). Categories contributing above average (100/20 = 5%) are retained for the analysis of the main opposition of each of the first three axes. Categories contributing above average to the variance of Axes 1 and 2 are included in
Table 2 and
Table 3 as well as in
Figure 1. Categories in brackets contribute less than average but are included as oppositional categories to aid the interpretation [
33]. The Axis 3 categories will not be displayed, but a summary interpretation will be presented. The axes of MCA are orthogonal to each other [
33,
37], and the axes we interpret are uncorrelated. Axis 1 sums up the most important polarities in the data; Axis 2 the second-most important, etc. [
37]. Hence, the plane constructed by crossing Axis 1 and Axis 2 (
Figure 1) gives the best low-dimensional representation of the patterns in active ageing, given the indicators included in the model.
Interpretation of Axis 1 (λ1 = 0.175): The categories contributing above average to Axis 1 can be read from
Figure 1 (horizontal line) and
Table 2. To the right of Axis 1 are indicators of actively using ICT (use of ICT: use often), not living by oneself (independent living: don’t live alone), taking care of older adults (care for older: yes), doing voluntary activities (voluntary activities: yes), doing physical exercise (physical exercise: active), and having better self-rated health than most (self-rated health: better than most). On the left side are indicators of the opposite. We interpret this as an opposition between being both socially and physically active and well versus being inactive and less well. Turning to the supplementary elements projected in
Figure 2, being well and active is positively associated with high levels of political participation (
Figure 2c), high levels of education (
Figure 2e), having been part of the salariat rather than the unskilled or skilled occupations in the labour market (
Figure 2f), still being in the labour market (
Figure 2b), making ends meet (
Figure 2d), and reporting a good or very good financial situation (
Figure 2g). The pattern along Axis 1 is not significantly related to age or gender.
Summary interpretation: We interpret this axis as a general axis summing up oppositions regarding Health and active living. It separates the socially and physically active and well from the socially and physically inactive and less well. It follows from the supplementary elements that being healthy and active in general is associated with economic and educational privileges, but it is not related to age or gender.
Interpretation of Axis 2 (λ1 = 0.133):
The categories making above-average contributions to Axis 2 can be read from
Figure 1 (vertical line) and
Table 3. On the lower side of Axis 2 are being mentally well (mental wellbeing: high) and in good health (self-rated health: better than most; access to health care: did not visit). Not having caring responsibilities for others only contributes slightly above average to this pattern. Along the top side in
Figure 1 are opposing answers to the same questions. We interpret this pattern as an opposition between being in good mental and physical health versus being less well mentally and physically. Turning to the supplementary variables (
Figure 2), being mentally and physically well is associated with having good finances (
Figure 2g) and is (perhaps somewhat surprisingly) positively associated with age, indicating that older respondents are more likely to evaluate their mental and physical health as being relatively good (
Figure 2h).
Summary interpretation: We interpret this axis as specifically related to health (and to some degree with not having caring responsibilities for elders). It separates the mentally and physically well from the mentally and physically less well, with the former being positively associated with income and age.
Interpretation of Axis 3 (λ1 = 0.126): Axis 3 is not illustrated. On the lower part of Axis 3, we find that indicators of being physically and socially active (physically active: active; voluntary activities: yes) corresponds to indicators of fair health (self-rated health: as good as most; access to health care: did not visit) and with living alone (independent living: live alone). The opposing answers to the same questions are on the top side of Axis 3. We interpret this pattern as an opposition between being physically and socially active versus being less active in these regards. Turning to the supplementary variables, we find that being active is positively associated with being politically active but not with any other enabling or individual characteristics.
Summary interpretation: We interpret this axis as a general axis summing up oppositions related to physical and social activity. It separates the physically and socially active living alone from the physically and socially inactive living with others. The axis does not appear to be influenced by individual characteristics (apart from also being active in politics). We consider this pattern to be a specific variant of the one identified in Axis 1. It shows opposition between the physically and socially active to the less active, but unlike Axis 1, being active is associated with living alone and with health being as good as others (rather than being better than others).
5. Discussion
The notion of active ageing has gained footing in discourses about how to meet the challenges from demographic ageing. The message is that “activity” is beneficial for society, as well as desirable for all older workers, and that structural or individuals’ constraints for the activity paradigm are limited. As such, active ageing is considered (more or less) to be a purely technical question (a technicality) that can be handled top-down through prudence, reason, insights, and declarations of intent.
There is no consensus about how active ageing can be framed theoretically or analysed empirically. However, as the AAI-project occupies a central position in the study of active ageing, we have critically scrutinised the AAI-project to come to grips with some of the basic problems embedded in the notion of active ageing and how it is studied. We have thus made a second order analysis of the AAI-project from two different angles: a theoretical and empirical.
A basic theoretical problem is that the concept of active ageing is nurtured by the political system and that the AAI-project is driven financially by the EU and the UN. This means that the AAI-project is both informed by the political system and designed to report back to the political system by identifying policies that enhance active and healthy ageing [
12]. As such, the AAI-project is guided by political ambitions (AAI may be said to be a political benchmark tool) rather than standards of science (objectivity and transparency). Therefore, it may not come as a big surprise that the AAI-active ageing conceptual framework is undertheorised and lacks coherence and conceptual and analytical clarity.
Of course, researchers making use of the concept of active ageing, including the developers of the AAI-project, are all fully aware of this basic problem. The answer thus far has been that researchers who make use of the concepts have argued for improvements in the methodology, such as rethinking the weighting and showing respect for cultural diversity [
21]. This also points to a limitation of our critique, namely that the critique is based on the implicit assumption that the AAI is a scientific—and less political—contribution. In any case, future research should focus on strengthening the idea of active ageing theoretically.
Empirically, the indicators used in the AAI-project supposedly measure achievements (or room for improvements) in active ageing, and the AAI encourages countries to develop policies that facilitate the participation of older adults. It remains unclear, however, which policies and programmes actually promote participation and engagement among older adults. For instance, the AAI-project does not give an account for which premeditated policies of the past (or type of society) have led to the fact that Sweden has an active ageing score of 44.0 points, while Poland only scores 27.3 points. Future research should therefore be more engaged in identifying policies that are conducive to active ageing and transferable from one country to another.
The active ageing discourse takes for granted that older adults are willing and able to become active. As a result, the active ageing approach tends to neglect frailty and limitations. As shown empirically in this article, inequality in ageing is conditioned by factors such as class and wealth, i.e., factors rooted in the social life biography. Future research should therefore focus more strongly on early-life experiences and cumulative disadvantage over the life course [
39] in the study of older adults’ active ageing prospects.
These findings are based on an empirical study where we replicated the conceptual framework of the AAI-project in order to address the weaknesses of the AAI approach on its own terms or from an AAI-internal position. However, this also reveals a weakness in our empirical study. We have not been able to replicate the AAI approach in a 1:1 relation. We have used data that are not fully compatible with the AAI data. For instance, in the domain of independent living, one serious limitation is that the AAI-project analysis “Percentage of people aged 75 years and older who live in a single household alone or in a couple household”, whereas we based our variable on independent living on the following survey question: “Do you live alone? Live alone/don’t live alone”. Finally, in adopting an exploratory approach to a single nation study, we make no claims that the specific dimensions (axes) of active ageing identified will be identical in other contexts.