Searching for the New Behavioral Model in Energy Transition Age: Analyzing the Forward and Reverse Causal Relationships between Belief, Attitude, and Behavior in Nuclear Policy across Countries
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Background
2.1. Belief: Perceived Risk on Nuclear Power
2.2. Attitude: Judgement toward Nuclear Energy Policy
2.3. Behavior: Acceptance of Nuclear Energy Policy
2.4. The Role of Trust in Government and Media
2.5. Conceptual Framework
3. Sample and Measurement
3.1. Sample
3.2. Measurement
4. Analysis
4.1. Discriptive Analysis
4.2. Effect of Trust in Government
4.3. Effect of Trust in Media
5. Summary
6. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Siegrist, M.; Visschers, V. Acceptance of nuclear power: The Fukushima effect. Energy Policy 2013, 59, 112–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodfellow, M.J.; Williams, H.R.; Azapagic, A. Nuclear renaissance, public perception and design criteria: An exploratory review. Energy Policy 2011, 39, 6199–6210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramana, M.V. Nuclear power and the public. Bull. At. Sci. 2011, 67, 43–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bromet, E.J.; Parkinson, D.K.; Dunn, L.O. Long-term Mental Health Consequences of the Accident at Three Mile Island. Int. J. Ment. Health 1990, 19, 48–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drottz-Sjöberg, B.-M.; Sjoberg, L. Risk perception and worries after the Chernobyl accident. J. Environ. Psychol. 1990, 10, 135–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fischhoff, B.; Slovic, P.; Lichtenstein, S.; Read, S.; Combs, B. How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes to-wards technological risks and benefits. Policy Sci. 1978, 9, 127–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slovic, P. Perception of risk. Science 1987, 236, 280–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ryu, Y.J.; Kim, S. The study of the effects of different message sources and direction on receiver’s attitude change about nuclear power Energy. Crisisonomy 2015, 11, 125–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, S.; Wang, J.; Lin, S.; Li, J. Public perceptions and acceptance of nuclear energy in China: The role of public knowledge, perceived benefit, perceived risk and public engagement. Energy Policy 2019, 126, 352–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jang, Y.; Park, E. Social acceptance of nuclear power plants in Korea: The role of public perceptions following the Fukushima accident. Renew. Sustain. Energy Rev. 2020, 128, 109894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Renn, O.; Rohrmann, B. Cross-Cultural Risk Perception: A Survey of Empirical Studies; Kluwer Academic Publishers: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Thomas, K.W. Conflict and conflict management: Reflections and update. J. Organ. Behav. 1992, 13, 265–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ajzen, I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flynn, J.; Burns, W.; Mertz, C.K.; Slovic, P. Trust as a determinant of opposition to a high-level radioactive waste repository: Analysis of a structural model. Risk Anal. 1992, 12, 417–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Visschers, V.; Siegrist, M. Fair play in energy policy decisions: Procedural fairness, outcome fairness and acceptance of the decision to rebuild nuclear power plants. Energy Policy 2012, 46, 292–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron, J.; Herzog, S. Public opinion on nuclear energy and nuclear weapons: The attitudinal nexus in the United States. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 2020, 68, 101567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ansolabehere, S.; Konisky, D.M. Public attitudes toward construction of new power plants. Public Opin. Q. 2009, 73, 566–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- He, Y.; Li, Y.; Xia, D.; Zhang, T.; Wang, Y.; Hu, L.; Gu, J.; Wu, Y. Moderating effect of regulatory focus on public acceptance of nuclear energy. Nucl. Eng. Technol. 2019, 51, 2034–2041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kellstedt, P.M.; Zahran, S.; Vedlitz, A. Personal efficacy, the information environment, and attitudes toward global warming and climate change in the United States. Risk Anal. 2008, 28, 113–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hansen, J.; Holm, L.; Frewer, L.; Robinson, P.; Sandøe, P. Beyond the knowledge deficit: Recent research into lay and expert attitudes to food risks. Appetite 2003, 41, 111–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Camerer, C.F.; Loewenstein, G. Behavioral economics: Past, present, future. In Advances in Behavioral Economics; Camerer, C.F., Loewenstein, G., Matthew, R., Eds.; Princeton University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 3–51. [Google Scholar]
- Muller, D.; Judd, C.M.; Yzerbyt, V.Y. When moderation is mediated and mediation is moderated. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 89, 852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sokolski, H.D. Nuclear Power’s Global Expansion: Weighing Its Costs and Risks; Strategic Studies Institute: Carlisle Barracks, PA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Jeon, Y.; Mok, J.W.; Kim, B.J. The influences of risk perception and trust in government on nuclear energy policy compliance and satisfaction: Applying the expectancy disconfirmation model. Korean J. Policy Anal. Eval. 2016, 26, 85–110. [Google Scholar]
- Mok, J. Moderating effect of knowledge level on the risk and acceptance relationship: The case of Korean nuclear policy. Korea Assoc. Policy Stud. 2017, 26, 419–449. [Google Scholar]
- Davies, S. What now for nuclear? Eng. Technol. 2011, 6, 39–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, E. Positive or negative? Public perceptions of nuclear energy in South Korea: Evidence from Big Data. Nucl. Eng. Technol. 2018, 51, 626–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kleinhesselink, R.R.; Rosa, E.A. Cognitive representation of risk perceptions: A comparison of Japan and the United States. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 1991, 22, 11–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slovic, P.; Fischhoff, B.; Lichtenstein, S. Why study risk perception? Risk Anal. 1982, 2, 83–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boholm, A. Comparative studies of risk perception: A review of twenty years of research. J. Risk Res. 1998, 1, 135–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kanda, R.; Tsuji, S.; Yonehara, H. Perceived risk of nuclear power and other risks during the last 25 years in Japan. Health Phys. 2012, 102, 384–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhai, G.; Suzuki, T. Risk perception in Northeast Asia. Environ. Monit. Assess. 2008, 157, 151–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa-Font, J.; Rudisill, C.; Mossialos, E. Attitudes as an expression of knowledge and “political anchoring”: The case of nuclear power in the United Kingdom. Risk Anal. 2008, 28, 1273–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fishbein, M.; Ajzen, I. Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior; Addison-Wesley: Reading, MA, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Kuklinski, J.H.; Metlay, D.S.; Kay, W.D. Citizen knowledge and choices on the complex issue of nuclear energy. Am. J. Political Sci. 1982, 26, 615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Visschers, V.; Wallquist, L. Nuclear power before and after Fukushima: The relations between acceptance, ambivalence and knowledge. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 36, 77–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allison, W. Public Trust in Nuclear Energy; WNA Personal Perspectives; World Nuclear Association: London, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Katsuya, T. Public Response to the Tokai Nuclear Accident. Risk Anal. 2001, 21, 1039–1046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kitada, A. Public opinion changes after the Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear power plant accident to nuclear power generation as seen in continuous polls over the past 30 years. J. Nucl. Sci. Technol. 2016, 53, 1686–1700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Japan Atomic Energy Relations Organization. Opinion Poll on Nuclear Utilization; Japan Atomic Energy Relations Organization: Tokyo, Japan, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Korea Nuclear Energy Agency. Annual National Polling; Korea Nuclear Energy Agency: Seoul, Korea, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Panina, O.V.; Prokofiev, S.E.; Barmenkova, N.A.; Krasyukova, N.L.; Kushchev, N.P. Prospects of nuclear energy devel-opment in Asia: Comparison with “Green Energy”. Int. J. Energy Econ. Policy 2020, 10, 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayes, S.C.; Strosahl, K.D.; Wilson, K.G. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An Experiential Approach to Behavior Change; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Zaller, J. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Blais, A.; Dobrzynska, A. Turnout in electoral democracies. Eur. J. Political Res. 1998, 33, 239–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindbladh, E.; Lyttkens, C.H. Polarization in the reaction to health-risk information: A question of social position? Risk Anal. Int. J. 2003, 23, 841–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, R.J.; Neuwirth, K.; Dunwoody, S. Using the theory of reasoned action to examine the impact of health risk messages. Ann. Int. Commun. Assoc. 1995, 18, 201–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Connor, R.E.; Bard, R.J.; Fisher, A. Risk perceptions, general environmental beliefs, and willingness to address climate change. Risk Anal. 1999, 19, 461–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sheppard, B.H.; Hartwick, J.; Warshaw, P.R. The theory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past research with recom-mendations for modifications and future research. J. Consum. Res. 1988, 15, 325–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodall, C.E.; Reed, P. Threat and efficacy uncertainty in news coverage about bed bugs as unique predictors of information seeking and avoidance: An extension of the EPPM. Health Commun. 2013, 28, 63–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- National Research Center. Health Risks from Exposure to Low Levels of Ionizing Radiation. BIER VII Phase 2; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Yablokov, A.V.; Nesterenko, V.B.; Nesterenko, A.V. Chernobyl: Consequences of the Catastrophe for People and the Environment; Sherman-Nevinger, J.D., Ed.; New York Academy of Sciences: Malden, MA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Pickett, S.E. Japan’s nuclear energy policy: From firm commitment to difficult dilemma addressing growing stocks of plutonium, program delays, domestic opposition and international pressure. Energy Policy 2002, 30, 1337–1355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, C.; Kim, S. The role of knowledge in acceptance of nuclear power: A focus on objective and subjective knowledge. Korean J. Public Adm. 2015, 54, 117–150. [Google Scholar]
- Tanaka, Y. Major psychological factors determining public acceptance of the siting of nuclear facilities. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 34, 1147–1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, S.; Kim, K. Risk society and the transition of the energy system: An analysis of energy preference and its policy implica-tions. Korean J. Public Adm. 2016, 54, 287–318. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, S.; Ryu, Y. Analyzing Koreans’ risk judgement process in case of Fukushima nuclear accident: The Receiver’s involvement and ability in HSM (Heuristic-Systematic Information Processing Model). J. Gov. Stud. 2014, 20, 315–342. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, S.; Lim, C.; Jeong, J.; Wang, J.; Park, C. Analyzing the risk judgement about Fukushima nuclear accident and nuclear power by integrating the risk-perception paradigm with risk communication model. Korean J. Public Adm. 2014, 23, 113–144. [Google Scholar]
- Roh, S.; Lee, J.W. Differentiated influences of risk perceptions on nuclear power acceptance according to acceptance targets: Evidence from Korea. Nucl. Eng. Technol. 2017, 49, 1090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pijawka, K.D.; Mushkatel, A.H. Public opposition to the siting of the high-level nuclear waste repository: The importance of trust. Rev. Policy Res. 1991, 10, 180–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rousseau, D.M.; Sitkin, S.B.; Burt, R.S.; Camerer, C. Not so different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1998, 23, 393–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Venables, D.; Pidgeon, N.F.; Parkhill, K.A.; Henwood, K.L.; Simmons, P. Living with nuclear power: Sense of place, proximity, and risk perceptions in local host communities. J. Environ. Psychol. 2012, 32, 371–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Visschers, V.H.; Siegrist, M. How a nuclear power plant accident influences acceptance of nuclear power: Results of a longitudinal study before and after the Fukushima disaster. Risk Anal. Int. J. 2013, 33, 333–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Siegrist, M.; Cvetkovich, G. Perception of hazards: The role of social trust and knowledge. Risk Anal. 2000, 20, 713–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Midden, C.J.H.; Huijts, N.M.A. The role of trust in the affective evaluation of novel risks: The case of CO2 storage. Risk Anal. 2009, 29, 743–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Siegrist, M. Trust and Confidence: The difficulties in distinguishing the two concepts in research. Risk Anal. 2010, 30, 1022–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Montijn-Dorgelo, F.N.H.; Midden, C.J.H. The role of negative associations and trust in risk perception of new hydrogen systems. J. Risk Res. 2008, 11, 659–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegrist, M. The Influence of trust and perceptions of risks and benefits on the acceptance of gene technology. Risk Anal. 2000, 20, 195–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitfield, S.C.; Rosa, E.A.; Dan, A.; Dietz, T. The future of nuclear power: Value orientations and risk perception. Risk Anal. 2009, 29, 425–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tumlison, C.; Moyer, R.M.; Song, G. The origin and role of trust in local policy elites’ perceptions of high-voltage power line installations in the state of Arkansas. Risk Anal. 2016, 37, 1018–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heitz, C.; Spaeter, S.; Auzet, A.V.; Glatron, S. Local stakeholders’ perception of muddy flood risk and implications for man-agement approaches: A case study in Alsace (France). Land Use Policy 2009, 26, 443–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bradbury, J.A.; Branch, K.M.; Focht, W. Trust and public participation in risk policy issues. In Social Trust and the Management of Risk; Cvetkovich, G., Loefstedt, R.E., Eds.; Earthscan Publications Ltd.: London, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Tait, M. Trust and the public interest in the Micropolitics of planning practice. J. Plan. Educ. Res. 2011, 31, 157–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hardin, R. Trust; Policy Press: Cambridge, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Blackburn, S. Trust, cooperation, and human psychology. In Trust and Governance; Braithwaite, V., Levi, M., Eds.; Russell Sage Foundation: New York, NY, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Kasperson, R.E.; Golding, D.; Tuler, S. Social distrust as a factor in siting hazardous facilities and communicating risks. J. Soc. Issues 1992, 48, 161–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Misztal, B.A. Trust in Modern Societies; Polity Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Tomes, N. The making of a germ panic, then and now. Am. J. Public Health 2000, 90, 191–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Klandermans, B.; Goslinga, S. Media discourse, movement publicity, and the generation of collective action frames: Theoretical and empirical exercises in meaning construction. In Comparative Perspectives on Social Movements: Political Opportunities, Moblizing Structures, and Cultural Framings; McAdam, D., McCarthy, J.D., Zald, M.N., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1996; pp. 312–337. [Google Scholar]
- Benford, R.D.; Snow, D.A. Framing processes and social movements: An overview and assessment. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2000, 26, 611–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Johnston, H.; Noakes, J.A. Frames of Protest: Social Movements and the Framing Perspective. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers: Lanham, MD, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Yates, J.E.; Stone, E.R. The risk construct. In Risk-Taking Behavior; Yates, J.F., Ed.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 1–25. [Google Scholar]
- Sussman, S.; Dent, C.W.; Flay, B.R.; Burton, D.; Craig, S.; Mestel-Rauch, J.; Holden, S. Media manipulation of adolescents’ personal level judgments regarding consequences of smokeless tobacco use. J. Drug Educ. 1989, 19, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tyler, T.R.; Cook, F.L. The mass media and judgments of risk: Distinguishing impact on personal and societal level judgments. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1984, 47, 693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robinson, M.J. Public affairs television and the growth of political malaise: The case of “the selling of the Pentagon”. Am. Political Sci. Rev. 1976, 70, 409–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rozin, P.; Royzman, E.B. Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2001, 5, 296–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhu, Z.; Liu, Y.; Kapucu, N.; Peng, Z. Online media and trust in government during crisis: The moderating role of sense of security. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2020, 50, 101717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Armitage, C.J.; Reid, J.C.; Spencer, C.P. Changes in cognition and behavior: A causal analysis of single-occupancy car use in a rural community. Transp. A Transp. Sci. 2013, 9, 1–10. [Google Scholar]
- Elliott, M.A.; Thomson, J.A.; Robertson, K.; Stephenson, C.; Wicks, J. Evidence that changes in social cognitions predict changes in self-reported driver behavior: Causal analyses of two-wave panel data. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2013, 50, 905–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liska, A.E. A Critical examination of the causal structure of the Fishbein/Ajzen attitude-behavior model. Soc. Psychol. Q. 1984, 47, 61–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fredricks, A.J.; Dossett, D.L. Attitude-behavior relations: A comparison of the Fishbein-Ajzen and the Bentler-Speckart models. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1983, 45, 501–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tyagi, P.K.; Wotruba, T.R. An exploratory study of reverse causality relationships among sales force turnover variables. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 1993, 21, 143–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroesen, M.; Handy, S.; Chorus, C. Do attitudes cause behavior or vice versa? An alternative conceptualization of the attitude-behavior relationship in travel behavior modeling. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 2017, 101, 190–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sussman, R.; Gifford, R. Causality in the theory of planned behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2019, 45, 920–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- King, G.; Keohane, R.; Verba, S. Designing Social Inquiry: Scientific Inference in Qualitative Research; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Mill, J.S. A system of logic, ratiocinative and inductive, being a connected view of the principles of evidence, and the methods of scientific investigation. N. Am. Rev. 1854, 78, 82–105. [Google Scholar]
- Russell, B. On the notion of cause. Proc. Aristotelean Soc. 1913, 13, 1–26, Reprinted in Mysticism and Logic; Unwin: London, UK, 1917; pp. 171–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cook, T.D.; Campbell, D.T. Causal Inference and the Language of Experimentation. In Quasi-Experimentation: Design & Analysis Issues for Field Settings; Houghton Millin Company: Boston, MA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Baron, R.; Kenny, D. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 1173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edelman Trust Barometer Team. Global Report. 2020, pp. 1–78. Available online: https://www.edelman.com/ (accessed on 3 April 2021).
- Battaglio, R.P., Jr.; Belardinelli, P.; Bellé, N.; Cantarelli, P. Behavioral public administration ad fontes: A synthesis of research on bounded rationality, cognitive biases, and nudging in public organizations. Public Adm. Rev. 2019, 79, 304–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grimmelikhuijsen, S. Linking transparency, knowledge and citizen trust in government: An experiment. Int. Rev. Adm. Sci. 2012, 78, 50–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Guo, Y.; Li, Y.; Chen, L. After Fukushima: How Do News Media Impact Japanese Public’s Risk Perception and Anxiety Re-garding Nuclear Radiation. Environ. Commun. 2020, 14, 97–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mok, J.; Lee, T.; Kim, B. The investigation of the relationship between dimensional and overall satisfaction on nuclear energy policies: Testing the non-compensatory and non-linearity models. Korean J. Politics Anal. Eval. 2015, 25, 29–56. [Google Scholar]
Variables | Category | Korea N (%) | Japan N (%) |
---|---|---|---|
Gender | Male | 256 (51.2) | 285 (49.8) |
Female | 244 (48.8) | 287 (50.2) | |
Age | 19–29 | 116 (23.2) | 110 (19.2) |
30–39 | 114 (22.8) | 142 (24.8) | |
40–49 | 136 (27.2) | 165 (28.8) | |
Over 50 | 134 (26.8) | 155 (271) | |
Education | Doctorate | 6 (1.2) | 7 (1.2) |
Master’s | 43 (8.6) | 20 (3.5) | |
Bachelor’s | 342 (68.4) | 313 (54.7) | |
Junior College | 70 (14) | 26 (4.5) | |
High School & Below | 158 (15.2) | 206 (36.0) | |
Household Income | Less than 2 million (M) won | 42 (8.4) | 78 (13.6) |
2 M won | 62 (18.6) | 107 (18.7) | |
3 M won | 93 (18.6) | 97 (17.0) | |
4 M won | 89 (17.8) | 95 (16.6) | |
5 M won | 82 (17.8) | 65 (11.4) | |
6 M won | 52 (10.4) | 46 (8.0) | |
7 M won | 26 (5.2) | 34 (5.9) | |
8 M won & above | 52 (10.8) | 50 (8.7) |
Composite Name | Alpha Values | Survey Questions | Korea (Mean/SD) | Japan (Mean/SD) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Belief (Risk perception toward nuclear power) | 0.832 a/0.847 b |
| 3.46/0.95 | 3.56/1.05 |
| 3.75/0.95 | 3.89/1.00 | ||
| 3.70/0.97 | 3.81/1.00 | ||
| 3.60/0.87 | 3.36/0.98 | ||
| 3.79/0.90 | 3.33/0.93 | ||
| 3.57/0.88 | 3.16/0.84 | ||
Attitude (Judgment on evaluative dissatisfaction with nuclear power) | 0.844 a/0.891 b |
| 3.14/0.96 | 3.23/1.07 |
| 3.23/0.85 | 3.25/1.09 | ||
| 3.42/0.85 | 3.39/0.97 | ||
| 3.22/0.84 | 3.54/0.93 | ||
| 3.21/1.0 | 3.35/0.90 | ||
| 3.26/0.99 | 3.22/0.82 | ||
Behavior (Actions against nuclear power) | 0.873 a/0.912 b |
| 2.77/0.99 | 2.74/1.12 |
| 2.75/0.97 | 2.69/1.00 | ||
| 2.66/1.0 | 2.52/0.97 | ||
| 2.61/0.99 | 2.56/1.08 | ||
| 2.40/0.99 | 2.53/1.08 | ||
Trust in government | 0.919 a/0.981 b |
| 2.94/0.91 | 2.92/0.90 |
| 2.91/0.86 | 2.67/0.96 | ||
| 2.76/0.92 | 2.85/0.93 | ||
| 2.93/0.93 | 2.57/0.86 | ||
| 2.98/0.93 | 2.52/0.89 | ||
| 2.96/0.93 | 2.48/0.86 | ||
| 2.89/0.87 | 2.5/0.89 | ||
| 2.99/0.84 | 2.74/0.94 | ||
| 2.89/0.85 | 2.73/0.90 | ||
| 2.98/0.80 | 2.51/0.87 | ||
| 2.88/0.90 | 2.59/0.87 | ||
Trust in media | 0.771 a/0.768 b |
| 3.0/0.85 | 2.80/0.96 |
| 2.9/0.85 | 2.87/0.94 | ||
| 3.26/0.90 | 3.04/0.80 | ||
| 2.92/0.90 | 2.78/0.86 |
Korea | Japan | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Means/SD | F (df) | Means/SD | F (df) | |||
Belief (Risk perception toward nuclear power) | Gender | Women | 3.52/0.75 | 17.295 (499) *** | 3.63/0.72 | 0.439 (571) |
Men | 3.77/0.55 | 3.40/0.73 | ||||
Age | Older | 3.58/0.70 | 4.136 (499) * | 3.59/0.74 | 6.534 (571) ** | |
Younger | 3.70/0.64 | 3.43/0.71 | ||||
Education | Higher | 3.67/0.68 | 2.785 (499) | 3.53/0.76 | 0.387 (571) | |
Lower | 3.54/0.66 | 3.49/0.68 | ||||
Income | Higher | 3.62/0.68 | 0.655 (499) | 3.57/0.74 | 3.272 (571) | |
Lower | 3.67/0.66 | 3.46/0.73 | ||||
Attitude (Judgment on evaluative dissatisfaction with nuclear power) | Gender | Women | 3.11/0.70 | 22.143 (499) *** | 3.40/0.75 | 5.510 (571) ** |
Men | 3.39/0.62 | 3.25/0.79 | ||||
Age | Older | 3.21/0.66 | 1.324 (499) | 3.39/0.81 | 4.389 (571) * | |
Younger | 3.28/0.69 | 3.26/0.73 | ||||
Education | Higher | 3.27/0.67 | 3.168 (499) | 3.35/0.79 | 0.678 (571) | |
Lower | 2.14/0.70 | 3.29/0.79 | ||||
Income | Higher | 3.24/0.65 | 0.004 (499) | 3.30/0.73 | 0.693 (571) | |
Lower | 3.24/0.71 | 3.35/0.82 | ||||
Behavior (Actions against nuclear power) | Gender | Women | 2.56/0.86 | 4.919 (499) * | 2.63/0.83 | 0.430 (571) |
Men | 2.72/0.74 | 2.58/0.93 | ||||
Age | Older | 2.63/0.76 | 0.000 (499) | 2.71/0.91 | 6.364 (571) ** | |
Younger | 2.63/0.85 | 2.51/0.86 | ||||
Education | Higher | 2.67/0.82 | 3.446 (499) | 2.62/0.93 | 0.406 (571) | |
Lower | 2.51/0.76 | 2.58/0.82 | ||||
Income | Higher | 2.62/0.84 | 0.191 (499) | 2.61/0.93 | 0.008 (571) | |
Lower | 2.65/0.75 | 2.50/0.84 |
Korea | Japan | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Means/SD | F (df) | Means/SD | F (df) | |||
Cogntion (Risk perception toward nuclear power) | Trust in G | Low group | 3.50/0.72 | 18.482 (499) *** | 3.82/0.73 | 91.069 (571) *** |
High Group | 3.76/0.60 | 3.27/0.63 | ||||
Trust in M | Low group | 3.64/0.73 | 0.024 (499) | 3.47/0.83 | 2.053 (571) | |
High Group | 3.64/0.62 | 3.55/0.64 | ||||
Attitute (Judgment on evaluative dissatisfaction with nuclear power) | Trust in G | Low group | 3.23/0.71 | 0.259 (499) | 3.64/0.84 | 84.712 (571) *** |
High Group | 3.26/0.64 | 3.07/0.61 | ||||
Trust in M | Low group | 3.22/0.72 | 0.536 (499) | 3.27/0.90 | 2.690 (571) | |
High Group | 3.26/0.63 | 3.37/0.65 | ||||
Behavior (Actions against nuclear power) | Trust in G | Low group | 2.42/0.75 | 33.823 (499) *** | 2.69/0.96 | 4.537 (571) * |
High Group | 2.83/0.80 | 2.53/0.82 | ||||
Trust in M | Low group | 2.49/0.77 | 13.941 (499) *** | 2.42/0.92 | 20.097 (571) *** | |
High Group | 2.76/0.82 | 2.75/0.89 |
High Involvement Model: Moderated Mediation | Low Involvement Model: Moderated Mediation | Hedonic Model: Moderated Mediation | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 5 | DV: Behavior b (t) | DV: Attitude b (t) | DV: Behavior b (t) | Model 5 | DV: Behavior b (t) | DV: Belief b (t) | DV: Behavior b (t) | Model 4 | DV: Attitude b (t) | DV: Belief b (t) | DV: Attitude b (t) |
X: Attitude | 0.503 (3.005) ** | 0.604 (4.675) ** | 0.618 (3.234) ** | X: Attitude | 0.254 (1.724) | 0.761 (6.184) ** | −0.138 (−0.726) | X: Behavior | 0.543 (4.625) ** | 0.660 (5.255) ** | 0.181 (1.368) |
Mo: Gov Trust | 0.508 (2.369) * | −0.097 (−0.588) | 0.341 (1.741) | Mo: Gov Trust | 0.009 (0.053) | 0.454 (3.310) ** | 0.341 (1.741) | Mo: Gov Trust | −0.087 (−0.866) | 0.466 (4.323) ** | −0.257 (−1.733) |
XMo: X * Mo | −0.044 (−0.746) | 0.005 (0.108) | −0.213 (−3.232) ** | XMo: X * Mo | 0.124 (2.791) | −0.068 (−1.637) | 0.255 (4.009) ** | XMo: X * Mo | −0.016 (−0.438) | −0.125 (−3.097) ** | 0.057 (1.355) |
Me: Attitude | −0.138 (1.726) | Me: Belief | 0.618 (3.234) ** | Me: Belief | 0.558 (3.678) ** | ||||||
MeMo: Me * Mo | 0.255 (4.009) ** | MeMo: Me * Mo | −0.213 (−3.232) ** | MeMo: Me * Mo | −0.025 (−0.476) |
High Involvement Model: Moderated Mediation | Low Involvement Model: Mediated Moderation | Hedonic Model: Moderated Mediation | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 6 | DV: Behavior b (t) | DV: Attitude b (t) | DV: Behavior b (t) | Model 3 | DV: Behavior b (t) | DV: Belief b (t) | DV: Behavior b (t) | Model 5 | DV: Attitude b (t) | DV: Belief b (t) | DV: Attitude b (t) |
X: Attitude | 0.528 (12.276) ** | 0.793 (30.454) ** | −0.185 (−0.939) | X: Belief | 0.544 (3.722) ** | 1.083 (12.090) ** | −0.162 (−0.702) | X: Behavior | 0.944 (8.976) ** | 0.693 (6.667) ** | 0.383 (4.243) ** |
Mo: Gov Trust | 0.197 (4.229) ** | 0.095 (3.346) ** | 0.171 (3.675) ** | Mo: Gov Trust | 0.201 (1.063) | 0.292 (2.512) * | 0.070 (0.384) | Mo: Gov Trust | 0.357 (3.623) ** | 0.260 (2.674) ** | 0.255 (2.309) |
XMo: X * Mo | 0.060 (4.220) ** | −0.030 (−3.498) ** | 0.282 (3.775) ** | XMo: X * Mo | 0.014 (0.264) | −0.086 (−2.675) ** | 0.119 (1.509) | XMo: X * Mo | −0.159 (−4.499) | −0.098 (−2.814) ** | −0.069 (−2.294) |
Me: Belief | 0.766 (3.848) ** | Me: Attitude | 0.681 (3.474) ** | Me: Belief | 0.841 (7.926) ** | ||||||
MeMo: Me * Mo | −0.214 (−2.937)** | MeMo: Me * Mo | −0.070 (−1.040) | MeMo: Me * Mo | −0.039 (−1.033) |
High Involvement Model: Mediated Moderation | Hedonic Model: Moderated Mediation | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 2 | DV: Behavior b (t) | DV: Attitude b (t) | DV: Behavior b (t) | Model 5 | DV: Attitude b (t) | DV: Belief b (t) | DV: Attitude b (t) |
X: Attitude | −0.133 (−0.620) | 0.654 (3.972) ** | −0.129 (−0.528) | X: Behavior | 0.296 (2.234) * | 0.322 (2.230) * | 0.022 (0.177) |
Mo: Media | −0.555 (−1.962) * | 0.081 (0.372) | −0.717 (−2.767) ** | Mo: Media | −0.292 (−2.456) * | −0.143 (−1.103) | 0.222 (1.127) |
XMo: X * Mo | 0.226 (2.962) ** | −0.021 (−0.368) | 0.106 (1.270) | XMo: X * Mo | 0.060 (1.40B7) | 0.012 (0.251) | 0.102 (2.493) * |
Me: Attitude | 0.042 (0.197) | Me: Belief | 0.868 (5.314) ** | ||||
MeMo: Me * Mo | 0.176 (2.499) * | MeMo: Me * Mo | −0.155 (−2.698) * |
High Involvement Model: Moderated Median | Hedonic Model: Mediated Moderation | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 4 | DV: Behavior b (t) | DV: Attitude b (t) | DV: Behavior b (t) | Model 1 | DV: Behavior b (t) | DV: Attitude b (t) | DV: Behavior b (t) |
X: Behavior | 0.376 (4.437) ** | 0.365 (4.423) ** | −0.220 (−2.991) ** | X: Belief | 0.554 (4.266) ** | 1.165 (14.972) ** | 0.699 (3.001) ** |
Mo: Media | −0.454 (−5.037) ** | −0.397 (−4.521) ** | 0.398 (4.116) ** | Mo: Media | 0.044 (0.253) | 0.395 (3.757) ** | −0.183 (−1.105) |
XMo: X * Mo | 0.040 (1.268) | 0.018 (0.580) | 0.165 (5.907) ** | XMo: * Mo | 0.050 (1.022) | −0.132 (−4.538) ** | −0.179 (−2.104) * |
Me: Belief | 1.371 (15.233) ** | Me: Attitude | −0.246 (−1.242) | ||||
MeMo: Me * Mo | −0.269 (−7.822) ** | MeMo: Me * Mo | 0.315 (4.188) ** |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kim, B.J.; Kim, S.; Kang, Y.; Kim, S. Searching for the New Behavioral Model in Energy Transition Age: Analyzing the Forward and Reverse Causal Relationships between Belief, Attitude, and Behavior in Nuclear Policy across Countries. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 6772. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19116772
Kim BJ, Kim S, Kang Y, Kim S. Searching for the New Behavioral Model in Energy Transition Age: Analyzing the Forward and Reverse Causal Relationships between Belief, Attitude, and Behavior in Nuclear Policy across Countries. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(11):6772. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19116772
Chicago/Turabian StyleKim, Byoung Joon, Seoyong Kim, Youngcheoul Kang, and Sohee Kim. 2022. "Searching for the New Behavioral Model in Energy Transition Age: Analyzing the Forward and Reverse Causal Relationships between Belief, Attitude, and Behavior in Nuclear Policy across Countries" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 11: 6772. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19116772
APA StyleKim, B. J., Kim, S., Kang, Y., & Kim, S. (2022). Searching for the New Behavioral Model in Energy Transition Age: Analyzing the Forward and Reverse Causal Relationships between Belief, Attitude, and Behavior in Nuclear Policy across Countries. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(11), 6772. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19116772