Next Article in Journal
Correlation of Salivary Occult Blood with the Plasma Concentration of Branched-Chain Amino Acids: A Cross-Sectional Study
Next Article in Special Issue
How Is Professional Identity Associated with Teacher Career Satisfaction? A Cross-Sectional Design to Test the Multiple Mediating Roles of Psychological Empowerment and Work Engagement
Previous Article in Journal
Increase in Referrals of Children and Adolescents to the Psychiatric Emergency Room Is Evident Only in the Second Year of the COVID-19 Pandemic—Evaluating 9156 Visits from 2010 through 2021 in a Single Psychiatric Emergency Room
Previous Article in Special Issue
Teacher Trainees’ Well-Being—The Role of Personal Resources
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Vocal Behavior of Teachers Reading with Raised Voice in a Noisy Environment

Freiburg Institute for Musicians’ Medicine, Medical Faculty of the Albert-Ludwigs—University Freiburg, University of Music Freiburg, 79110 Freiburg, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(15), 8929; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19158929
Submission received: 23 June 2022 / Revised: 20 July 2022 / Accepted: 21 July 2022 / Published: 22 July 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Teachers' Well-Being at Work and Quality of Life)

Abstract

:
(1) Objective: Teaching is a particularly voice-demanding occupation. Voice training provided during teachers’ education is often insufficient and thus teachers are at risk of developing voice disorders. Vocal demands during teaching are not only characterized by speaking for long durations but also by speaking in noisy environments. This provokes the so-called Lombard effect, which intuitively leads to an increase in voice intensity, pitch and phonation time in laboratory studies. However, this effect has not been thoroughly investigated in realistic teaching scenarios. (2) Methods: This study thus examined how 13 experienced, but vocally untrained, teachers behaved when reading in a noisy compared to quiet background environment. The quiet and noisy conditions were provided by a live audience either listening quietly or making noise by talking to each other. By using a portable voice accumulator, the fundamental frequency, sound pressure level of the voice and the noise as well as the phonation time were recorded in both conditions. (3) Results: The results showed that the teachers mainly responded according to the Lombard effect. In addition, analysis of phonation time revealed that they failed to increase inhalation time and appeared to lose articulation through the shortening of voiceless consonants in the noisy condition. (4) Conclusions: The teachers demonstrated vocally demanding behavior when speaking in the noisy condition, which can lead to vocal fatigue and cause dysphonia. The findings underline the necessity for specific voice training in teachers’ education, and the content of such training is discussed in light of the results.

1. Introduction

For occupational voice users, the voice is an important tool for the successful practice of their profession [1]. A particularly voice-demanding profession is that of classroom teachers. The amount of voice-use during a school day is characterized by instructions and explanations to impart knowledge, establish classroom management, and to develop a relationship with the class. It has been shown that teachers speak more during work than during leisure time [2]. Teaching therefore involves various vocal risk factors [3,4] which may cause the development of dysphonia [5,6]. The duration of speaking is not the only significant risk factor as speech volume also plays an important role: teachers were found to talk with a raised voice 61% of the time during actual classroom lessons [7]. Thus, in addition to the quantitative factor of prolonged speaking duration, elevation of sound pressure level is also a factor worth considering in regard to the occupational vocal load of teachers.
Teachers are at higher risk of developing pathological vocal symptoms [8]. A large number of publications found that teachers report voice problems significantly more often than other occupational groups [9,10,11,12]. This includes vocal discomfort, decreased voice quality and vocal endurance [13]. A meta-analysis found that female gender, additional airway problems, high caffeine consumption and higher number of classes per week were additional risk factors for voice problems among teachers [14]. Further vocal demands can also be caused by psychological stress [15], size and discipline problems in the class [16], as well as poor indoor air quality in the classroom [17].
These occupational voice risk factors are important not only in terms of teachers’ health, but also concerning the quality of teaching. Studies showed that teaching with a stressed voice, i.e., recognizably hoarse and rough, led to lower learning outcomes in pupils compared to teaching with a clear voice [18,19,20,21]. Findings suggest that children listening to a stressed voice require certain cognitive capacities to process the speech signal at the expense of comprehension [22]. Furthermore, background noise in connection with a poor voice quality of the teacher degrades listening abilities in classrooms drastically [23].
Regardless of how long teachers speak during class, which depends strongly on the lesson subject, students’ age, and the kind of teaching method [7], it is of particular interest to understand why teachers raise their voice and what happens in these instances. One reason to increase voice intensity is when speaking in noisy environments. These situations trigger adaptations in the speaker’s voice production, which are commonly referred to as the Lombard effect [24]. Speech production in noise is therefore called Lombard speech, whereby specific voice modifications in accordance with the noise source aim to facilitate intelligibility and maintain comprehension in communicative situations.
A review article showed that numerous studies have investigated vocal changes during Lombard speech [25]. The most common changes found in healthy, vocally untrained subjects describe adaptations of increasing the voice sound pressure level (SPL) and fundamental frequency (ƒo) [26,27,28] as well as additional effects on the acoustic properties of voice production, such as pronounced amplitude modulations [29] and increases in duration and intensity of vowel production [27,30]. In particular, changes in the duration of vowels and voiced consonants were found to be typical for Lombard speech [24,31,32]. Generally, lengthened vocalizations in noisy environments were found to be interrelated with the voice SPL, particularly on stressed words [24]. Similarly, ƒo and the duration of words were also found to be correlated [33]. However, this may be caused by rhetorical pronunciations where words are intentionally modulated for prosaic expression. When speaking in noisy environments with generally raised voice instead of just specific words, the increased intensity contour of the voice was found to be equal across all words in the utterance [34]. Nevertheless, while the duration of vowels increases, the duration of unvoiced stops and fricatives, i.e., voiceless consonants, decreased [28,35].
In addition to the acoustic parameters of the voice, specific respiratory strategies also change when speaking in noisy environments. During speaking, respiratory kinematics are characterized by short and fast inhalations while voicing is produced on prolonged exhalation [36]. The depth and duration of the breathing is rather irregular and depends on the utterance produced. When reading aloud, the inhalation is synchronized with the linguistic structure of the text. Speakers take breaths related to syntactic components such as the end of a sentence or a paragraph. When investigating the lung volume variability during reading, high consistencies in the location of inspirations across participants were found at grammatically appropriate places in the texts [37]. Increased inhalation duration and volume have been observed, especially associated with louder utterances, at sentence and paragraph boundaries, and in the initial breath before the reading [38].
Studies investigating vocal behavior in noisy environments were usually performed in experimental settings, i.e., laboratories or sound studios, where speakers were recorded with and without masking noise [39,40]. Very few studies used realistic environments to analyze Lombard speech. For instance, Patel and Schell [34] examined speech production during a cooperative game in which two people interacted with each other verbally in a quiet or a noisy setting. The authors found that the speakers proportionally increased voice intensity, ƒo and word duration in accordance with the noise level. However, in this scenario only game-related words in spontaneous speech were analyzed.
Investigating Lombard speech in experimental and clinical settings does not necessarily represent the environments of occupational voice users. For instance, in previous studies, 85 dB broad band noise has been used to imitate a noisy environment [26,27,28]. This is in contrast to the typical classroom background noise of about 65–70 dB [7,41,42]. The acoustical characteristics of noise in a classroom would also be better characterized by the acoustic properties of cocktail party noise than broadband white noise. Former studies also often used specific words or single sentences for acoustical evaluation, and yet, it is of great importance to understand the normal and healthy function of the voice in a typical work environment [8].
To investigate the vocal demand responses of teachers during a working day, several studies have used a portable voice dosimeter to record both voice and background noise. This device is a voice accumulator with a lightweight neck collar carrying a microphone and an accelerometer. It switches between measuring the ambient noise and the person’s own voice, depending on whether or not the person phonates. It thus allows the monitoring of the ƒo, SPL and phonation time during phonation. SPL is recorded for the vocal intensity and the background noise. This technique was used to evaluate vocal behavior in realistic environments over a longer period. Vocal SPL, ƒo and the phonation time were found to be significantly higher in teaching situations compared to non-teaching situations [2,43,44,45] confirming that teachers experience extensive vocal demands at work. Analysing the vocal behavior of teachers during classroom lessons with a voice dosimeter, teachers showed rather individual vocal demand responses according to the subject being taught, with the highest voice intensities in sports lessons [42]. While the noise level varied considerably during the lesson and was loudest at the beginning and at the end of a lesson, the SPL of the voice varied less within the lessons.
Speaking in noisy environments has been shown to cause voice problems and can lead to chronic voice disorders [46]. Intensifying voice production is associated with an increase in subglottic pressure, airflow over the glottis and maximum airflow declination rate [47,48] and also an increase in the mechanical impact stress of the vocal folds [49]. This has been considered as a key factor to the ethology of voice disorders, including secondary organic vocal fold lesions such as nodules or polyps [50]. Therefore, this relationship indicates that a prolonged period of Lombard speech is a potential risk factor for the development of voice disorders. This was shown in a study with individuals diagnosed with phonotrauma who experienced higher environmental noise levels in their daily life than individuals diagnosed with a functional voice disorder [51].
Based on their research findings, there is a call for the urgent promotion of occupational voice health [8] and strong recommendations for the general implementation of vocal education for teachers [6,52]. This is consistent with other studies calling for more research on vocally demanding occupations and environmental noise contributing to vocal health and preventing occupational voice disorders [46,53]. There is evidence that voice intervention programs can have positive impacts on voice quality in short-term [54,55,56,57] and even in long-term perspective [58]. While the effects of Lombard speech have been studied before and after the vocal effort of a typical teachers’ workday [45], there is still a lack of knowledge regarding the intuitive compensatory strategy of teachers during the Lombard speech episode itself in a realistic setting. Individualized training programs, which are adapted in detail to the vocal demands of teachers, should concretely expound these possibly uneconomical compensation strategies being used by teachers intuitively during Lombard speech.
The goal of this study was to investigate the impact of environmental noise on the voice production of teachers within a realistic setting. The participants were asked to read a text aloud in a quiet and a noisy condition. The voice was recorded using a voice dosimeter, which can provide an authentic picture of vocal demand responses [59]. In contrast to laboratory situations, this study was performed in a semi-controlled real situation with a live audience and voice changes were investigated. To achieve this, rooms very similar in size and acoustics to a typical classroom were used, with people sitting at tables and the speaker standing in front of them. The audience was either quietly listening or chatting with each other. The latter simulates a common classroom situation where, for example, students work in small groups, and especially at the beginning and at the end of a lesson [42].
According to the literature, it was expected that voice SPL, ƒo and word durations would increase during Lombard speech. In addition to these parameters, of particular interest was the phonation time, the voicing, and inhalation duration across both conditions. It was assumed that the phonation time would also increase in the noisy condition due to a lengthening of the words. However, it was also expected that the pauses between words would shorten and the duration for inhaling would increase due to a greater subglottic air-pressure being needed during loud reading. The results of this study might help to further understanding of physiological variables that influence vocal demand responses of occupational voice users, and thus allow further insight into the pathophysiology and the prevention of voice disorders associated with Lombard speech.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The sample of this study included 13 high school teachers (7 female and 6 male). The mean age was 52.0 years (SD 10.5 years) without significant difference between genders. They were participants of voice workshops with group sizes between 8 and 13 persons. In each of the workshop, two persons (one female, one male) were asked to participate in the experiment. After explaining the procedure and the measuring technique, they volunteered for participation. The experiment was performed at the beginning of the workshops. At this point, the participants were not yet influenced by the content of the workshop. They also had not taught or worked before the workshop so that they were not vocally tired. After the experiment, the individual results of the participants were discussed in the workshop.
Except for the gender and the age, no further information was gathered from the participants. The study was included in a larger project of teacher’s vocal health [55] and within this project, the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University Clinic Freiburg.

2.2. Procedure

The participants read an unknown text aloud in front of the workshop group. While the audience was sitting at tables, the speakers were standing in front of them. The audience consisted of the other workshop participants, i.e., 7–12 persons roughly evenly divided in genders. The study was conducted in three different rooms. The sizes and the acoustics of the rooms were rather similar and resembled a typical classroom for a size of about 25 students. Reverberation times were not measured.
In the first condition (Quiet situation), the speakers were asked to read the first part of the text in a usual manner of reading a text to an audience (i.e., a school class). The other participants in the group listened silently. In the second condition (Noisy situation), the audience produced some background noise and the participants read the second part of the text. To produce a standardized noisy environment that is similar to people chatting, the audience was asked to speak a single word (“Rhubarb”) continuously to each other with a vocal intensity of telling a story at close distance. The use of a single word comes from early movies. To simulate a realistic babble of voices, actors had to constantly repeat the word “Rhabarber”.
The speakers had to read the text with the intention of maintaining intelligibility above the background noise. This represents a demanding classroom situation when the teacher attempts to reach the students when they are noisy and active. To do this, the teacher tries to calm down the class and make them quiet. Generally, it does not take long to get the class quiet. However, the vocal demands response of the teachers in such situations is very similar to the noisy condition of this study.
All speakers performed first the quiet condition and then the noisy condition. There was a little pause in between for instructing the audience. The performance lasted about 5 min per person including the installation of the voice-measuring device. Since a loud speech might lead to vocal fatigue, it was assumed that speaking in a silent situation at normal voice intensity would cause only low vocal strain and therefore minimize the degree of fatigue. In addition, all speakers were perceived as vocally healthy teachers by the leader of the workshop. They also did not verbally report any vocal symptoms. This supported the assumption of low vocal demands for the participants in the quiet situation. Other health related issues were not collected from the participants.

2.3. The Text

The chosen text for the experiment was a German text about music in the Middle Ages. It was divided into two parts, one for each condition. Both were nearly identical from a phonological point of view. The first part contains 118 words with 876 characters (including spaces) with 46% monosyllabic words (1 syllable) and 31% polysyllabic words (≥3 syllables). The mean syllables per word is 2.1 and the mean number of characters per word is 6.4. The passage has 38% vowels and 30% voiced consonants (sonorants). The second part contains 166 words with 1150 characters (including spaces). This part also includes 45% monosyllabic words and 23% polysyllabic words. Similar to the first part, the mean syllables per word is 1.9 and the mean number of characters per word is 6.0 and it contains 38% vowels and 31% voiced consonants. With approximately 1% of the characters being punctuation marks, both texts consist of 30% voiceless consonants. Since the number of syllables per word and the number of vowels and consonants are very similar in both parts, it can be assumed that both parts are phonetically the same.
All participants read the text in a very straightforward manner, i.e., without long pauses or interruptions. They did not perform with any emphasized phrasings or accentuations that might have caused longer duration of vowels [60]. The average reading time across all participants was 57.6 s (SD 13.5 s) for the first part and 72.3 s (SD 14.4 s) for the second part.

2.4. Measuring Technique

The portable dosimeter VoxLog (Sonvox, Umea, Sweden) was used. The device is a lightweight neck collar with a microphone and an accelerometer. The microphone records the sound of the environment. The accelerometer sensor detects when the person phonates, labelling this as voice data. When the person did not speak, the recording was saved as ambient noise data. The recording frequency was set to 0.1 s.
With the software of the device (“VoxLog Connect 3.1.13”, Levis, QC, Canada), the ƒo and the SPL of the voice and the noise were collected. The device has previously been successfully used to collect voice data of teachers in natural environments [42,44,61,62,63].
The accelerometer records speech data only when the subject is producing sounds via vocal fold vibration (e.g., phonation of vowels and voiced consonants). During this time, the value of the noise was set to zero. In the analysis, all noise values with zero scores were excluded for calculating the mean noise SPL. Similarly, the mean voice SPL was also calculated only when the voice value was not zero.
The phonation and the pausing times were analyzed from the voice data. The phonation time was averaged within the sampling frequency and reported in % of the recorded time frame. The times of no phonation were classed as pausing times. Since a text was constantly read aloud, the moments of no phonation were either when the person was inhaling or at little pauses between words. Due to the sampling frequency, very short voiceless consonants were included in the phonation time. Only pauses of at least 100 ms were recorded as non-phonation time.
In previous studies, the mean inhalation duration was found to be 0.54 s (SD 0.18 s) with a range from a minimum of 0.19 s to a maximum of 1.21 s during a reading task [64]. Therefore, pauses >200 ms were considered to be inhalation durations. Very long pauses (>2 s) were also excluded as they might contain more breaths or other behaviors such as swallowing. Therefore, three voice conditions have been analyzed: the mean durations of (1) the voicing, (2) the short pauses, and (3) the long pauses including inhalation.
The analysis of the recordings started with the first phonated word of the speaker. Therefore, it did not include the initial inhalation before the reading. The measurements also ended with the last spoken word. Therefore, the analysis was only performed on the pure reading time.

2.5. Statistics

The statistical analysis was performed with SPSS 28 (SPSS Inc., Armonk, NY, USA). Descriptive statistics were reported with mean values and standard deviations (SD). Comparisons of parametric variables between the conditions were performed with paired sample t-tests. Influences of further factors such as gender were investigated using univariate repeated measures ANOVA. Non-parametric comparisons were analyzed with a cross-table and Pearson’s Chi2 was reported. Correlations between two parametric variables were described with the coefficient Pearson’s r. The level of significance was set to p = 0.05.

3. Results

The mean values of the vocal parameters in both the quiet and the noisy condition are shown in Table 1. The fundamental frequencies ƒo has been separately analyzed by gender. All values significantly increased in the noisy condition compared to the quiet condition. While the noise SPL increased by about 11.4 dB(A), the voice SPL increased by 8.4 dB(A). There were no significant interaction effects of gender for changes in voice SPL or the phonation time.
In both the quiet and the noisy condition, there were no significant correlations between voice SPL, noise SPL and phonation time. The correlations of the SPL difference between the conditions of the voice and the noise with the phonation time difference are shown in Figure 1. Increases in the voice SPL showed a significant correlation with increases in the phonation time (r = 0.58, p = 0.039). The noise SPL difference showed no significant correlation with the phonation time differences (r = −0.52, p = 0.071). The voice SPL difference and the noise SPL difference were not significantly correlated (r = 0.10; p = 0.741).
The mean durations of the voicing and the inhalation pauses (>200 ms) are shown in Figure 2. The voicing durations increased significantly from 1.19 s (SD 0.33 s) in the quiet condition to 1.77 s (SD 0.08 s) in the noisy condition (t(12) = 7.58, p < 0.001). The duration of the inhalation pauses were not significantly different between the conditions (t(12) = 1.55, p = 0.148), with an average of 561 ms (SD 110 ms) in the quiet condition and 521 ms (SD 96 ms) in the noisy condition. The mean durations were very similar to other studies [38]. For both voicing and inhalation durations, there were no significant interaction effects found between genders.
The short pause durations were divided into two groups with pauses of 100 ms and 200 ms. For both groups, the number of pause occurrences was counted. Table 2 shows these numbers and the percentage of the total amount of occurrences in the quiet and the noisy conditions. The distribution of the pause occurrences between the two conditions was significantly different (Chi2 = 3.99, p = 0.046).

4. Discussion

In this study, the vocal demand responses [13] of teachers in a realistic environment when reading a text aloud in front of an audience were investigated in a quiet and a noisy background condition. For occupational voice users such as teachers, high noise levels during work require specific vocal behaviors. The results showed that the teachers increased ƒo, voice SPL and phonation time in the noisy condition. At first glance, the teachers’ vocal performance in this realistic scenario was similar to the findings of previous studies in experimental and clinical settings. However, some specific differences were noted.
With an increase in the ambient noise, an increase in vocal intensity was observed, which is typical in Lombard speech. Nevertheless, no significant correlation was found between the changes in the voice SPL and the changes in the noise SPL. This suggests that the increase in vocal intensity was rather independent of the ambient noise level. It seems that it did not matter how much the noise level increased, the teachers raised their voice to a certain individual level. It is possible that they were performing as they would usually do in a classroom. This finding indicates that teachers who regularly raise their voice during work may adapt to the vocal demands by adopting an individual vocal effort of increasing the voice SPL to a rather high level.
Speaking with increased voice SPL is a risk factor for impairing voice quality due to an increase in vocal fold collision stress, which strongly depends on the vocal fold velocity at the time of impact [65]. Additionally vocal fold impact stress correlates positively with subglottic pressure which in turn is also closely related to vocal intensity. Such mechanical stress has been considered as a key factor to the ethology of voice disorders including secondary organic focal fold lesions such as nodules or polyps [50]. Therefore, the behavior of the teachers in increasing the voice SPL independently of the noise creates an unnecessary amount of vocal fatigue. This can be avoided if the vocal effort of increasing the voice SPL fits the situational increase in the ambient noise SPL only to the degree of maintaining the intelligibility and reduces with a decreasing of the noise level. Former studies showed that teachers reacted rather differently to noise. While some teachers adapted their voice intensity to the noise level, others did not [41,42]. It is therefore necessary to not only train teachers in how to raise their voice, but also in how to adapt their voice appropriately to the situation.
Another possible vocal risk factor is to speak with increased ƒo. It was also found in the present study that a noisy speaking condition caused an intuitive increase in ƒo of the teachers by about a half-semitone per 1 dB increase in SPL, which is in accordance with the literature in terms of magnitude [66]. This is typical in Lombard speech but also generally for louder speaking phonation [67]. Whereas in previous studies this increase was mainly attributed to an increase in subglottic pressure, which would passively raise ƒo [66], it was later described to be used in an active manner to increase vocal intensity as it results in a larger number of speech pressure cycles per time unit which also raises SPL [67]. Still, an increase in pitch would not only be related to a higher number of vibratory cycles and subglottic pressure but also greater acceleration of the vocal folds and thus higher inertia- as well as collision-forces which might correlate with a greater load for the vocal folds [68]. In one study, voice changes in 22 female teachers were measured before and after a typical workday, and it was found that some teachers showed an increased ƒo at the end of the day while the ƒo decreased in others [45]. The authors interpreted the increased ƒo as an adaptation of the voice due to the vocal demands and the decreased ƒo as an acute inflammation or muscle fatigue. It was also found that vocally trained speakers were able to maintain ƒo when speaking in noisy environments with increased voice SPL [69]. This might reflect better vocal control as the result of vocal training, which might also reduce the amount of vocal fatigue. However, the increase in the ƒo for loud phonation was not only shown for untrained voice users but also for singers and actresses [66,70] who, because of their training, should not behave in a fundamentally uneconomic way. However, it is not unlikely that singers, for example, choose appropriate resonance strategies according to their genre (anti-megaphone vocal tract for classical singers or megaphone-shaped vocal tract for contemporary music singer [71,72]) for higher phonation, which could additionally support the vocal folds in their vibration due to source–filter interactions. On the other hand, it could be that untrained subjects achieve the increase in pitch in a more uneconomic way (e.g., laryngeal elevation, which was associated with muscle tension dysphonia [73]). Thus, it could be concluded that a certain increase in pitch might be advantageous for loud speech, although the right amount and the realisation certainly plays an important role here, meaning that the negative properties in terms of increased mechanical stress do not predominate. Still, mean speaking pitch was found to be lower for trained voices compared to untrained participants [69] and professional broadcasters, for example, who often use a lower speaking pitch which was also found to arouse a greater sense of credibility, trust, and confidence, which could also be an important factor for teachers [74].
Furthermore, the performed correlation analysis showed a clear relationship between the increase in vocal intensity and the increase in phonation time. The participant not only spoke louder, but, proportionally, they also spoke for longer. The detailed analysis yielded that they mainly increased the duration of the vocalisation, i.e., the duration of vowels and voiced consonants. This finding is in line with previous studies [24,27,30,32]. With an increase of 12 dB(A) of the voice SPL, it was found that vowels were significantly lengthened by 33 ms on average [28]. The results showed that the voice SPL of the teachers increased by about 8.4 dB(A) and the mean voicing duration increased by 58 ms. The stretching of the voiced words in this study was therefore slightly longer than in Garnier and Henrich [28], but also shorter than the word duration increase of 77 ms found in Pittman and Wiley [31]. These findings indicate that the longer phonation time was mainly caused by the lengthening of the voiced parts of the words.
Nevertheless, stretching the voicing of words while speaking can sound like an artificially exaggerated lengthening. To avoid that and to keep a rather similar speaking pace, a possible strategy would be to shorten the soundless parts. One particular voiceless part is the pause to inhale. A study showed that during a reading task in German, there was a significant difference in inhalation duration according to the length of the sentence, but not regarding the syntactic complexity [38]. They found nearly identical pause durations in complex and simple sentences (548 ms and 540 ms, respectively). These durations were very similar to the inhalation durations found in this study. The duration of 561 ms in the quiet condition was even slightly longer than in the noisy condition with 521 ms.
Thus, the results showed that the durations of inhalation were not significantly different between the quiet and the noisy condition. The teachers inhaled with rather similar speed in both conditions. As the duration of inhalation is strongly correlated with the inhaled lung volume, it can be assumed that the teachers also inhaled approximately the same lung volume for phonation in both the quiet and noisy conditions. This might be another indication of uneconomic vocal behavior which can increase vocal fatigue when speaking louder: An increase in vocal intensity is frequently associated with an increase in subglottic pressure [75]. As deeper inhalation generates higher subglottic pressures through activation of a greater amount of passive recoil forces [76], it is believed to be a meaningful adaptation for loud phonation. The inspiratory airflow duration and the value for the inhaled volume of professional speakers during loud speaking were found to be significantly higher compared to untrained speakers [69]. The authors claimed that this is related to better management of breathing function due to years of voice training. Moreover, another study observed that professional speakers used higher lung volumes and greater volume excursions when performing monologs compared to conversational speech [77]. Conversely, phonation on low lung volume was more often associated with pressed phonation and voice disorders [78]. To avoid increasing duration and thus volume of inhalation during loud phonation means that the increase in subglottic pressure was not generated via passive recoil forces but might be related to changes in the vibratory pattern, which could be associated with higher adduction and thus higher mechanical stress during vocal fold vibration. Additionally, deeper inhalation should be associated with higher diaphragm activity which is closely related to the amount of tracheal pull [79]. A greater tracheal pull can affect the vertical laryngeal position in a caudal direction and increase vocal fold abduction, both of which are frequently desired in the function of the speaking voice [80,81]. It is therefore recommended to intentionally take more time to inhale more deeply when speaking louder to avoid an increase in mechanical impact stress on the vocal folds due to pressed phonation.
Other studies have found that the duration of voiceless consonants decreases when spoken louder [28,35]. This was supported by the findings of the teachers in this study. The distribution of the short pauses with 100 ms and 200 ms were significantly different between the quiet and the noisy condition. The distributions showed that there were more 200 ms pauses in the noisy condition than in the quiet condition. However, considering that the two sections of text are very similar in terms of the number of vowels, and voiced and unvoiced consonants, the total number of short pauses should be quite similar. This was not the case. There were fewer short pauses of 100 ms in the noisy condition than in the quiet condition but a rather similar number of 200 ms pauses. It can therefore be assumed that the speakers shortened many unvoiced consonants below the sampling frequency, which were not detected. It has been found that voiceless consonants were significantly shortened by 10 ms on average [28]. The analysis therefore suggests that the teachers did shorten the voiceless pauses and consonants. As an outcome, this can lead to unclear and blurred pronunciation and a loss of clear articulation, which can impair intelligibility and thus the need for increased SPL. It is therefore recommended to keep the articulation as clear as possible especially when speaking with a raised voice.
A study with trained and untrained singers found that untrained participants overexerted their vocal system after a duration of loud speaking and began to show strong signs of vocal fatigue [82]. In contrast, the trained participants were able to use their knowledge and abilities to maintain vocal health during intensive speaking. Therefore, vocal training focusing on speaking with a raised voice is highly recommended for teachers. The studies with professional speakers in particular demonstrate that voice training can reduce vocal effort and maintain long term vocal health [69,77].
To summarize, the teachers generally adapted their voices in a noisy environment according to previous findings considering Lombard speech. In contrast to the fact that the subjects were highly occupational voice users, they still exhibited some behaviors which could rather be attributed to an uneconomic voice production (see Table 3). Therefore, it can be seen as essential to provide some specific recommendations for teachers who are required to raise their voices. Five particular features of voicing in noise have been elicited from this study. These are: increases of the voice SPL, the ƒo and the voicing duration, as well as the similar inhalation duration and the shortening of voiceless consonants in a noisy environment compared to a quiet environment. These aspects are listed in Table 3 along with the potential risk factors and some recommendations on how to avoid vocal fatigue.
The recommendations are common methods from voice therapy and voice training. To implement these recommendations, certain exercises can be used. These are very individual and require contact with a professional voice coach.
It was found that individuals diagnosed with phonotrauma spoke more in environments with higher noise levels than individuals with functional voice symptoms [51]. However, it is still unclear how much the noisy surrounding really caused the vocal impairment. Nevertheless, during a classroom lesson, there are two types of sound which compete: the noise of the class and the voice of the teacher. While studies recommend reducing noise in the classroom, this may not be practical in some situations. Since the voice can withstand higher intensity for short durations, it would make sense to train the voice for such circumstances. From a didactical perspective, it would be advisable to use the trained voice to reduce the noise during class and create an optimal and quiet learning atmosphere.

Limitations

As recommended by several studies, research on vocal demand responses in realistic environments is necessary. However, this requires special measurement procedures and a corresponding realistic situation. Such studies are quite extensive and take time to conduct. For these reasons, the sample size of this study was rather small. It is necessary to conduct further studies in classroom environments with more participants to obtain more detailed data and findings.
For that, it would be recommended to measure other acoustic variables of the rooms as well, such as the reverberation time. This might have an influence on the vocal demand response. In this study, typical classrooms were chosen to simulate the usual situation.
Regarding the sample, it would have been interesting to collect more details of the participants such as general health, vocal habits, and vocal symptoms. However, this would require a larger sample size to provide the possibility of selecting groups according to their background.
Furthermore, the recording device of the portable dosimeter was limited to a rather low sampling frequency and did not include spectral measures. However, an increase in SPL and perceived loudness during speaking phonation might also be related to a change in vocal tract articulation and thus resonance properties of the vocal tract (e.g., resonance maxima in the frequency region where our ear is most sensitive, as with the speakers’ formant [83]) as well as interactions between the vocal tract and the vocal fold vibrations [83]. Differences in supraglottal adaptions were shown, for example, in an endoscopic study between actors and untrained subjects [84] and point to the importance of this subject.
Much better measuring devices have been used in experimental and clinical studies. However, these devices are quite sensitive and require certain circumstances and settings to function properly. Devices that are to be used in realistic environments must be adjusted accordingly and are often reduced in their recording capacity. Future studies should consider the best possible measurement device, but also prioritize an environment as naturalistic as possible to maintain a semi-controlled but ecological study design.

5. Conclusions

As expected, this study observed that teachers exhibit vocal demand responses in Lombard speech in realistic noisy environments. Vocal effort was found to be quite high which can lead to vocal fatigue in the long term. The findings emphasize the need for further studies investigating occupational vocal demand responses in order to develop individual prevention and treatment methods. To this end, voice accumulation in work-related situations can provide evidence-based responses to determine safe and healthy performance to meet vocal demands [8]. The results provide evidence for an association between ambient noise and vocal demand response, which has been found to cause the development of vocal symptoms [7]. It is therefore highly recommended that voice training be provided for teachers to maintain their vocal health. To provide optimal listening conditions for the students, it is important to create good acoustical circumstances and equip individuals with the knowledge and ability to improve voice ergonomics [1,85] and to retain clear articulation.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation, data collection and analysis were performed by M.N. The first draft of the manuscript was written by M.N. and B.R., A.I. and L.T. contributed supporting comments. All authors participated equally in editing the final version. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The authors did not receive support from any organization for the submitted work. No funding was received to assist with the preparation of this manuscript. No funding was received for conducting this study. We acknowledge support by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Freiburg.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical approval was obtained from the Ethics Committee of University Clinic Freiburg.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated during the study are available from the corresponding author on request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Sala, E.; Rantala, L. Voice Ergonomics: Occupational and Professional Voice Care; Cambridge Scholars Publishing: Cambridge, UK, 2019; ISBN 9781527527591. [Google Scholar]
  2. Szabo Portela, A.; Hammarberg, B.; Södersten, M. Speaking Fundamental Frequency and Phonation Time during Work and Leisure Time in Vocally Healthy Preschool Teachers Measured with a Voice Accumulator. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2013, 65, 84–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Chen, S.H.; Chiang, S.-C.; Chung, Y.-M.; Hsiao, L.-C.; Hsiao, T.-Y. Risk Factors and Effects of Voice Problems for Teachers. J. Voice 2010, 24, 183–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Rossi-Barbosa, L.A.R.; Barbosa, M.R.; Morais, R.M.; de Sousa, K.F.; Silveira, M.F.; Gama, A.C.C.; Caldeira, A.P. Self-Reported Acute and Chronic Voice Disorders in Teachers. J. Voice 2016, 30, 755.e25–755.e33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kooijman, P.G.C.; de Jong, F.I.C.R.S.; Thomas, G.; Huinck, W.; Donders, R.; Graamans, K.; Schutte, H.K. Risk Factors for Voice Problems in Teachers. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2006, 58, 159–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Van Houtte, E.; Claeys, S.; Wuyts, F.; Van Lierde, K. The Impact of Voice Disorders Among Teachers: Vocal Complaints, Treatment-Seeking Behavior, Knowledge of Vocal Care, and Voice-Related Absenteeism. J. Voice 2011, 25, 570–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Kristiansen, J.; Lund, S.P.; Persson, R.; Shibuya, H.; Nielsen, P.M.; Scholz, M. A Study of Classroom Acoustics and School Teachers’ Noise Exposure, Voice Load and Speaking Time during Teaching, and the Effects on Vocal and Mental Fatigue Development. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2014, 87, 851–860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Phyland, D.; Miles, A. Occupational Voice Is a Work in Progress: Active Risk Management, Habilitation and Rehabilitation. Curr. Opin. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2019, 27, 439–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cantor Cutiva, L.C.; Vogel, I.; Burdorf, A. Voice Disorders in Teachers and Their Associations with Work-Related Factors: A Systematic Review. J. Commun. Disord. 2013, 46, 143–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Martins, R.H.G.; Pereira, E.R.B.N.; Hidalgo, C.B.; Tavares, E.L.M. Voice Disorders in Teachers. A Review. J. Voice 2014, 28, 716–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Morawska, J.; Niebudek-Bogusz, E. Risk Factors and Prevalence of Voice Disorders in Different Occupational Groups—A Review of Literature. Otorynolaryngologia-Przegląd Klin. 2017, 16, 94–102. [Google Scholar]
  12. Pereira, E.R.B.N.; Tavares, E.L.M.; Martins, R.H.G. Voice Disorders in Teachers: Clinical, Videolaryngoscopical, and Vocal Aspects. J. Voice 2015, 29, 564–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Hunter, E.J.; Cantor-Cutiva, L.C.; van Leer, E.; van Mersbergen, M.; Nanjundeswaran, C.D.; Bottalico, P.; Sandage, M.J.; Whitling, S. Toward a Consensus Description of Vocal Effort, Vocal Load, Vocal Loading, and Vocal Fatigue. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2020, 63, 509–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Byeon, H. The Risk Factors Related to Voice Disorder in Teachers: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. de Jong, F.I.C.R.S. An Introduction to the Teacher’s Voice in a Biopsychosocial Perspective. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2010, 62, 5–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Marçal, C.C.B.; Peres, M.A. Self-Reported Voice Problems Among Teachers: Prevalence and Associated Factors. Rev. Saúde Pública 2011, 45, 503–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Vertanen-Greis, H.; Löyttyniemi, E.; Uitti, J.; Putus, T. Self-Reported Voice Disorders of Teachers and Indoor Air Quality in Schools: A Cross-Sectional Study in Finland. Logop. Phoniatr. Vocol. 2021, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Imhof, M.; Välikoski, T.-R.; Laukkanen, A.-M.; Orlob, K. Cognition and Interpersonal Communication: The Effect of Voice Quality on Information Processing and Person Perception. Stud. Commun. Sci. 2014, 14, 37–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Morton, V.; Watson, D.R. The Impact of Impaired Vocal Quality on Children’s Ability to Process Spoken Language. Logoped. Phoniatr. Vocol. 2001, 26, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Rogerson, J.; Dodd, B. Is There an Effect of Dysphonic Teachers’ Voices on Children’s Processing of Spoken Language? J. Voice 2005, 19, 47–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Sahlén, B.; Haake, M.; von Lochow, H.; Holm, L.; Kastberg, T.; Brännström, K.J.; Lyberg-Åhlander, V. Is Children’s Listening Effort in Background Noise Influenced by the Speaker’s Voice Quality? Logop. Phoniatr. Vocol. 2018, 43, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Lyberg-Åhlander, V.; Haake, M.; Brännström, J.; Schötz, S.; Sahlén, B. Does the Speaker’s Voice Quality Influence Children’s Performance on a Language Comprehension Test? Int. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2015, 17, 63–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Schiller, I.S.; Remacle, A.; Durieux, N.; Morsomme, D. Effects of Noise and a Speaker’s Impaired Voice Quality on Spoken Language Processing in School-Aged Children: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2022, 65, 169–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Brumm, H.; Zollinger, S.A. The Evolution of the Lombard Effect: 100 Years of Psychoacoustic Research. Behaviour 2011, 148, 1173–1198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Uma Maheswari, S.; Shahina, A.; Nayeemulla Khan, A. Understanding Lombard Speech: A Review of Compensation Techniques towards Improving Speech Based Recognition Systems. Artif. Intell. Rev. 2021, 54, 2495–2523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Castellanos, A.; Benedí, J.M.; Casacuberta, F. An Analysis of General Acoustic-Phonetic Features for Spanish Speech Produced with the Lombard Effect. Speech Commun. 1996, 20, 23–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Junqua, J.C. The Lombard Reflex and Its Role on Human Listeners and Automatic Speech Recognizers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1993, 93, 510–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Garnier, M.; Henrich, N. Speaking in Noise: How Does the Lombard Effect Improve Acoustic Contrasts between Speech and Ambient Noise? Comput. Speech Lang. 2014, 28, 580–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bosker, H.R.; Cooke, M. Talkers Produce More Pronounced Amplitude Modulations When Speaking in Noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2018, 143, EL121–EL126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Van Summers, W.; Pisoni, D.B.; Bernacki, R.H.; Pedlow, R.I.; Stokes, M.A. Effects of Noise on Speech Production: Acoustic and Perceptual Analyses. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1988, 84, 917–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Pittman, A.L.; Wiley, T.L. Recognition of Speech Produced in Noise. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2001, 44, 487–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Vlaj, D.; Kacic, Z. The Influence of Lombard Effect on Speech Recognition. In Speech Technologies; Ipsic, I., Ed.; InTech: Rijeka, Croatia, 2011; ISBN 9789533079967. [Google Scholar]
  33. Vainio, M.; Aalto, D.; Suni, A.; Arnhold, A.; Raitio, T.; Seijo, H.; Järvikivi, J.; Alku, P. Effect of Noise Type and Level on Focus Related Fundamental Frequency Changes. In Proceedings of the INTERSPEECH 2012, 13th Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association, Portland, OR, USA, 9–13 September 2012. [Google Scholar]
  34. Patel, R.; Schell, K.W. The Influence of Linguistic Content on the Lombard Effect. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2008, 51, 209–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hansen, J.H.L. Analysis and Compensation of Stressed and Noisy Speech with Application to Robust Automatic Recognition. Signal Process. 1989, 17, 282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Rochet-Capellan, A.; Fuchs, S. Take a Breath and Take the Turn: How Breathing Meets Turns in Spontaneous Dialogue. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2014, 369, 20130399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Winkworth, A.L.; Davis, P.J.; Ellis, E.; Adams, R.D. Variability and Consistency in Speech Breathing During Reading: Lung Volumes, Speech Intensity, and Linguistic Factors. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1994, 37, 535–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Fuchs, S.; Petrone, C.; Krivokapić, J.; Hoole, P. Acoustic and Respiratory Evidence for Utterance Planning in German. J. Phon. 2013, 41, 29–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Cooke, M.; Lu, Y. Spectral and Temporal Changes to Speech Produced in the Presence of Energetic and Informational Maskers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2010, 128, 2059–2069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Marcoux, K.; Cooke, M.; Tucker, B.V.; Ernestus, M. The Lombard Intelligibility Benefit of Native and Non-Native Speech for Native and Non-Native Listeners. Speech Commun. 2022, 136, 53–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Lindstrom, F.; Waye, K.P.; Södersten, M.; McAllister, A.; Ternström, S. Observations of the Relationship between Noise Exposure and Preschool Teacher Voice Usage in Day-Care Center Environments. J. Voice Off. J. Voice Found. 2011, 25, 166–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Nusseck, M.; Richter, B.; Spahn, C.; Echternach, M. Analysing the Vocal Behaviour of Teachers during Classroom Teaching Using a Portable Voice Accumulator. Logop. Phoniatr. Vocol. 2018, 43, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hunter, E.J.; Titze, I.R. Variations in Intensity, Fundamental Frequency, and Voicing for Teachers in Occupational Versus Nonoccupational Settings. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2010, 53, 862–875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Lyberg Åhlander, V.; Pelegrín García, D.; Whitling, S.; Rydell, R.; Löfqvist, A. Teachers’ Voice Use in Teaching Environments: A Field Study Using Ambulatory Phonation Monitor. J. Voice 2014, 28, 841.e5–841.e15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Remacle, A.; Garnier, M.; Gerber, S.; David, C.; Petillon, C. Vocal Change Patterns During a Teaching Day: Inter- and Intra-Subject Variability. J. Voice 2018, 32, 57–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Toki, E.I.; Fakitsa, P.; Plachouras, K.; Vlachopoulos, K.; Kalaitzidis, N.; Pange, J. How Does Noise Pollution Exposure Affect Vocal Behavior? A Systematic Review. AIMS Med. Sci. 2021, 8, 116–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Drugman, T.; Alku, P.; Alwan, A.; Yegnanarayana, B. Glottal Source Processing: From Analysis to Applications. Comput. Speech Lang. 2014, 28, 1117–1138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Holmberg, E.B.; Hillman, R.E.; Perkell, J.S. Glottal Airflow and Transglottal Air Pressure Measurements for Male and Female Speakers in Soft, Normal, and Loud Voice. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1988, 84, 511–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Titze, I.R. Mechanical Stress in Phonation. J. Voice 1994, 8, 99–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Jiang, J.J.; Titze, I.R. Measurement of Vocal Fold Intraglottal Pressure and Impact Stress. J. Voice 1994, 8, 132–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Szabo Portela, A.; Granqvist, S.; Ternström, S.; Södersten, M. Vocal Behavior in Environmental Noise: Comparisons Between Work and Leisure Conditions in Women With Work-Related Voice Disorders and Matched Controls. J. Voice Off. J. Voice Found. 2018, 32, 126.e23–126.e38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Schei, T.B.; Åvitsland, B.S.; Schei, E. Forgetting the Audible Body. Voice Awareness in Teacher Education. Nord. Res. Music. Educ. 2019, 19, 197–215. [Google Scholar]
  53. Hackworth, R.S. Vocal Health Implications for Music Teachers: A Literature Review. Update Appl. Res. Music. Educ. 2021, 875512332110611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. López, J.M.; Catena, A.; Montes, A.; Castillo, M.E. Effectiveness of a Short Voice Training Program for Teachers: A Preliminary Study. J. Voice 2017, 31, 697–706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Richter, B.; Nusseck, M.; Spahn, C.; Echternach, M. Effectiveness of a Voice Training Program for Student Teachers on Vocal Health. J. Voice 2016, 30, 452–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Gassull, C.; Godall, P.; Polini, E.; Amador, M.; Casanova, C. Effects of a Voice Training Program on Acoustics, Vocal Use, and Perceptual Voice Parameters in Catalan Teachers. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2020, 72, 411–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Nallamuthu, A.; Boominathan, P.; Arunachalam, R.; Mariswamy, P. Outcomes of Vocal Hygiene Program in Facilitating Vocal Health in Female School Teachers with Voice Problems. J. Voice 2021, S0892199721000187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Nusseck, M.; Immerz, A.; Spahn, C.; Echternach, M.; Richter, B. Long-Term Effects of a Voice Training Program for Teachers on Vocal and Mental Health. J. Voice 2021, 35, 438–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Atará-Piraquive, Á.P.; Cantor-Cutiva, L.C. Gender Differences in Vocal Doses among Occupational Voice Users: A Systematic Review of Literature and Meta-Analysis. Logop. Phoniatr. Vocol. 2021, 47, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Edwards, J.; Beckman, M.E.; Fletcher, J. The Articulatory Kinematics of Final Lengthening. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1991, 89, 369–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Mehta, D.D.; Van Stan, J.H.; Zañartu, M.; Ghassemi, M.; Guttag, J.V.; Espinoza, V.M.; Cortés, J.P.; Cheyne, H.A.; Hillman, R.E. Using Ambulatory Voice Monitoring to Investigate Common Voice Disorders: Research Update. Front. Bioeng. Biotechnol. 2015, 3, 155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Södersten, M.; Salomão, G.L.; McAllister, A.; Ternström, S. Natural Voice Use in Patients With Voice Disorders and Vocally Healthy Speakers Based on 2 Days Voice Accumulator Information From a Database. J. Voice 2015, 29, 646.e1–646.e9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Whittico, T.H.; Ortiz, A.J.; Marks, K.L.; Toles, L.E.; Van Stan, J.H.; Hillman, R.E.; Mehta, D.D. Ambulatory Monitoring of Lombard-Related Vocal Characteristics in Vocally Healthy Female Speakers. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2020, 147, EL552–EL558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Wang, Y.-T.; Green, J.R.; Nip, I.S.B.; Kent, R.D.; Kent, J.F. Breath Group Analysis for Reading and Spontaneous Speech in Healthy Adults. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2010, 62, 297–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. DeJonckere, P.H.; Lebacq, J. Vocal Fold Collision Speed in Vivo: The Effect of Loudness. J. Voice 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Gramming, P.; Sundberg, J.; Ternström, S.; Leanderson, R.; Perkins, W.H. Relationship between Changes in Voice Pitch and Loudness. J. Voice 1988, 2, 118–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Alku, P.; Vintturi, J.; Vilkman, E. Measuring the Effect of Fundamental Frequency Raising as a Strategy for Increasing Vocal Intensity in Soft, Normal and Loud Phonation. Speech Commun. 2002, 38, 321–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Horáček, J.; Laukkanen, A.-M.; Šidlof, P.; Murphy, P.; Švec, J.G. Comparison of Acceleration and Impact Stress as Possible Loading Factors in Phonation: A Computer Modeling Study. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2009, 61, 137–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Master, S.; Guzman, M.; Azócar, M.J.; Muñoz, D.; Bortnem, C. How Do Laryngeal and Respiratory Functions Contribute to Differentiate Actors/Actresses and Untrained Voices? J. Voice 2015, 29, 333–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Master, S.; Guzman, M.; Carlos de Miranda, H.; Lloyd, A. Electroglottographic Analysis of Actresses and Nonactresses’ Voices in Different Levels of Intensity. J. Voice 2013, 27, 187–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Titze, I.; Worley, A.S.; Story, B. Source-Vocal Tract Interaction in Female Operatic Singing and Theatre Belting. J. Sing. 2011, 67, 561–572. [Google Scholar]
  72. Titze, I.R.; Worley, A.S. Modeling Source-Filter Interaction in Belting and High-Pitched Operatic Male Singing. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009, 126, 1530–1540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Lowell, S.Y.; Kelley, R.T.; Colton, R.H.; Smith, P.B.; Portnoy, J.E. Position of the Hyoid and Larynx in People with Muscle Tension Dysphonia. Laryngoscope 2012, 122, 370–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Neil, E.; Worrall, L.; Day, A.; Hickson, L. Voice and Speech Characteristics and Vocal Hygiene in Novice and Professional Broadcast Journalists. Adv. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2003, 5, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Plant, R.L.; Younger, R.M. The Interrelationship of Subglottic Air Pressure, Fundamental Frequency, and Vocal Intensity during Speech. J. Voice 2000, 14, 170–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Sundberg, J. The Science of the Singing Voice; Northern Illinois University Press: DeKalb, IL, USA, 1987; ISBN 9780875801209. [Google Scholar]
  77. Watson, P.J.; Hixon, T.J.; Maher, M.Z. To Breathe or Not to Breathe—That Is the Question: An Investigation of Speech Breathing Kinematics in World Class Shakespearean Actors. J. Voice 1987, 1, 269–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Gillespie, A.I.; Gartner-Schmidt, J.; Rubinstein, E.N.; Abbott, K.V. Aerodynamic Profiles of Women With Muscle Tension Dysphonia/Aphonia. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2013, 56, 481–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Sundberg, J.; Leanderson, R.; von Euler, C. Activity Relationship between Diaphragm and Cricothyroid Muscles. J. Voice 1989, 3, 225–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Iwarsson, J.; Sundberg, J. Effects of Lung Volume on Vertical Larynx Position during Phonation. J. Voice 1998, 12, 159–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Iwarsson, J.; Thomasson, M.; Sundberg, J. Effects of Lung Volume on the Glottal Voice Source. J. Voice 1998, 12, 424–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Gelfer, M.P.; Andrews, M.L.; Schmidt, C.P. Effects of Prolonged Loud Reading on Selected Measures of Vocal Function in Trained and Untrained Singers. J. Voice 1991, 5, 158–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Nawka, T.; Anders, L.C.; Cebulla, M.; Zurakowski, D. The Speaker’s Formant in Male Voices. J. Voice 1997, 11, 422–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Guzman, M.; Ortega, A.; Olavarria, C.; Muñoz, D.; Cortés, P.; Azocar, M.J.; Cayuleo, D.; Quintana, F.; Silva, C. Comparison of Supraglottic Activity and Spectral Slope Between Theater Actors and Vocally Untrained Subjects. J. Voice 2016, 30, 767.e1–767.e8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Pirilä, S.; Jokitulppo, J.; Niemitalo-Haapola, E.; Yliherva, A.; Rantala, L. Teachers’ and Children’s Experiences after an Acoustic Intervention and a Noise-Controlling Workshop in Two Elementary Classrooms. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2020, 72, 454–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Increase in the phonation time of the voice showed a significant correlation with increases in sound pressure level (SPL) of the voice but not the noise. The figure displays differences in the voice SPL (orange) and the noise SPL (blue) between the conditions against the difference of the phonation time.
Figure 1. Increase in the phonation time of the voice showed a significant correlation with increases in sound pressure level (SPL) of the voice but not the noise. The figure displays differences in the voice SPL (orange) and the noise SPL (blue) between the conditions against the difference of the phonation time.
Ijerph 19 08929 g001
Figure 2. Significant increase in duration of phonation from quiet to noisy condition with stable duration of inhalations. The figure displays Mean durations of the voicing and the pauses (>200 ms) between conditions (error bars: standard error of the mean; *: p < 0.01).
Figure 2. Significant increase in duration of phonation from quiet to noisy condition with stable duration of inhalations. The figure displays Mean durations of the voicing and the pauses (>200 ms) between conditions (error bars: standard error of the mean; *: p < 0.01).
Ijerph 19 08929 g002
Table 1. Mean values (with standard deviations) of the measured parameters by condition.
Table 1. Mean values (with standard deviations) of the measured parameters by condition.
Quiet ConditionNoisy ConditionStatistics
Fundamental Frequency (in Hz)Female: 220.7 (28.1)Female: 286.4 (50.9)t = −7.095; p < 0.001
Male: 142.5 (13.8)Male: 185,4 (29.9)t = −3.595; p < 0.001
Voice SPL (in dB(A))76.3 (2.3)84.6 (2.2)t = −13.464; p < 0.001
Noise SPL (in dB(A))57.4 (4.9)68.8 (4.8)t = −9.758; p < 0.001
Phonation time (in percent)60.7 (7.4)71.6 (5.6)t = −6.550; p < 0.001
Table 2. Occurrences of short pauses during reading in number and percentage by condition.
Table 2. Occurrences of short pauses during reading in number and percentage by condition.
Pause DurationQuiet ConditionNoisy Condition
100 ms266 (81.3%)167 (74.2%)
200 ms61 (18.7%)58 (25.8%)
Total327 (100%)225 (100%)
Table 3. Vocal demand responses in noisy environments, the resulting vocal risk and recommendations to maintain vocal health.
Table 3. Vocal demand responses in noisy environments, the resulting vocal risk and recommendations to maintain vocal health.
Voice AspectResponseVocal RiskRecommendation
Voice SPLIncreasesIncrease in subglottal pressure andIncrease only as high as necessary for maintaining communication and decrease when not necessary
Mechanical stress on vocal folds
ƒoIncreasesincreased number vocal fold vibrations, subglottic pressure and mechanical stress on vocal foldsIncrease speaking pitch with caution and combine it with supportive resonance strategies
Voicing durationLengthenincreased number vocal fold vibrationsUse it effectively for sound production
Inhalation durationRemain similarWith increased voicing leads to longer phonation on lower lung volume, related to higher laryngeal position and pressed phonationTake time to inhale deeper
Short pausesShortensLoss of articulationMaintain clear articulation
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Nusseck, M.; Immerz, A.; Richter, B.; Traser, L. Vocal Behavior of Teachers Reading with Raised Voice in a Noisy Environment. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 8929. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19158929

AMA Style

Nusseck M, Immerz A, Richter B, Traser L. Vocal Behavior of Teachers Reading with Raised Voice in a Noisy Environment. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(15):8929. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19158929

Chicago/Turabian Style

Nusseck, Manfred, Anna Immerz, Bernhard Richter, and Louisa Traser. 2022. "Vocal Behavior of Teachers Reading with Raised Voice in a Noisy Environment" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 15: 8929. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19158929

APA Style

Nusseck, M., Immerz, A., Richter, B., & Traser, L. (2022). Vocal Behavior of Teachers Reading with Raised Voice in a Noisy Environment. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(15), 8929. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19158929

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop