Next Article in Journal
Exposure to Green, Blue and Historic Environments and Mental Well-Being: A Comparison between Virtual Reality Head-Mounted Display and Flat Screen Exposure
Previous Article in Journal
SARS-CoV-2 in Atmospheric Particulate Matter: An Experimental Survey in the Province of Venice in Northern Italy
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Media Use and Bedtime Delays in Young Children: An Adjunct Study of the Japan Environment and Children’s Study

1
Department of Sustainable Health Science, Center for Preventive Medical Sciences, Chiba University, 1-33 Yayoi-cho, Inage-ku, Chiba 263-8522, Japan
2
Medical Support Center for Japan Environment and Children’s Study, National Center for Child Health and Development, 10-1, Okura 2-chome, Setagaya-ku, Tokyo 157-8535, Japan
3
Department of Nutrition and Metabolic Medicine, Center for Preventive Medical Sciences, Chiba University, 1-33 Yayoi-cho, Inage-ku, Chiba 263-8522, Japan
4
Department of Bioenvironmental Medicine, Graduate School of Medicine, Chiba University, 1-8-1 Inohana, Chuo-ku, Chiba 260-8670, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(15), 9464; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19159464
Submission received: 29 June 2022 / Revised: 30 July 2022 / Accepted: 31 July 2022 / Published: 2 August 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Children's Health)

Abstract

:
Excessive screen media use has been reported to cause shorter sleep; however, the types of media environments that affect early childhood sleep are less known. This study examined the association of multiple media use, screen time for each device, and the purpose of smartphone and tablet use with delayed bedtime among 4–8-year-olds. Participants were recruited from the Japan Environment and Children’s Study, a nationwide birth cohort study. Mothers of 1837 children reported screen media use and bedtime in a questionnaire. The association between delayed bedtimes (after 22:00 h) and media device use (smartphones, tablets, portable and console games, and TV/DVDs) was examined by logistic regression analysis. Children who used three or more devices besides TV/DVDs were more likely to have delayed bedtimes. Delayed bedtimes were associated with smartphone use, even with a 0.1–1 h/day screen time, and with prolonged screen time for tablets, portable games, and console games, but not for TV/DVDs. Gaming on smartphones and tablets was also associated with delayed bedtime. To ensure adequate sleep for young children, families must develop feasible measures to discourage children’s use of multiple devices and prolonged device use, especially for games, and a social environment that supports such measures.

1. Introduction

The use of various screen media is rapidly expanding worldwide. Children are increasingly exposed to screen media devices from an early age [1,2,3]. According to a 2017 report from Japan, smartphones, tablets, portable games, and console games were the most popular devices among children aged 0–9, with 3.1% of children less than 1 year old, 55.2% of 5-year-olds, and 89.9% of 9-year-olds using devices with Internet access [3]. The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommends limiting screen use for children aged 2–5 years to high-quality programs for 1 h per day [4]. However, many children use screen media for much longer durations [1,2,3]. While screen media devices with Internet access are a useful tool for acquiring and communicating information, there are grave concerns about the undesirable health effects of screen media use on children and adolescents [5,6].
One of the health-related effects of screen media use is reduced sleep duration. Studies have consistently shown an association between screen media use and later bedtime or shorter total sleep time in school-aged children and adolescents [7,8,9]. In particular, screen media use before bedtime has been reported to be associated with delayed bedtime and shorter total sleep time [9]. Furthermore, late bedtimes and shorter sleep duration have been highlighted as important factors in early childhood development; they have also been shown to be associated with obesity and other adverse health outcomes [10,11,12].
Early childhood is a critical period for forming long-term habits in later life [13] and is more vulnerable to environmental health effects than the period of adolescence. In today’s social environment of widespread and diverse media use, young children are increasingly exposed to a variety of media devices. Studies have shown an association between excessive media use, such as smartphones, video games, TV, or gaming, and shorter sleep time [14,15,16,17], heavier TV or tablet use and later bedtimes [18], and a longitudinal association between computer use or TV viewing and shorter sleep duration [19]. In the Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS), prolonged screen time of TV/DVD or smartphone/portable electronic device was associated with delayed bedtime in children at the age of 3 years [20]. However, in modern society, it is important to evaluate the impact of individual and multiple devices.
Mobile screen media use and sleeping behavior in young children may be affected by accessibility to media devices at home [21]. Regarding the multiple screen media used by young children, identifying the uses associated with delayed bedtime can provide important information to help families and caregivers understand the aspects that need attention. Therefore, this study aimed to clarify the kind of media environments in early childhood that are associated with delayed bedtimes among four- to eight-year-old Japanese children from three perspectives: First, we determined if multiple device use was associated with delayed bedtime. Second, we investigated the associated devices. Finally, we identified the purpose of device use.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Population

This study was conducted as an adjunct study of the JECS, which was a nationwide government-funded birth cohort study that aimed to evaluate the impact of environmental factors on children’s health and development. A total of 97,415 pregnant women (103,062 pregnancies) were registered at 15 regional centers located throughout Japan between January 2011 and March 2014. Details of the JECS can be found in a study by Kawamoto et al. [22].
All mothers enrolled in the JECS at the Chiba Regional Center as of December 2018 were invited to participate in this adjunct study and voluntarily completed a questionnaire. If more than one child per household participated in the JECS, data pertaining to the oldest child were used. In this adjunct study, informed consent was not obtained, but the opportunity to refuse was ensured by indicating that responses to the questionnaire were voluntary. This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of Medicine, Chiba University (3028).

2.2. Data Collection

The questionnaire developed for this study collected data related to the following: (i) children’s regular bedtime, (ii) children’s media device use and screen time, (iii) frequency of children’s screen media use within 1 h before sleeping, (iv) Internet content accessed on smartphones and tablets, (v) parental screen time for mobile devices, (vi) basic parental characteristics (age, educational level, household income, and mother’s occupation), and (vii) family structure. Missing data on parental age and educational level were complemented using the JECS dataset. The following information was also extracted from the JECS dataset: child’s sex, birth year, elder sibling, household income, mother’s occupation and marital status, child’s diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder by age 4, and mother’s and child’s screen media use at 3 years of age. The specific JECS datasets used in this study were “jecs-an-20180131” released in March 2018 and “jecs-qa-20210401” released in February 2022.

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Outcome

Children’s delayed bedtime was the primary outcome measure. Mothers were asked to report their children’s regular bedtime by the following question: “What time does your child usually go to bed at night?” The responses were collected from among five categories (before 19:59, 20:00–20:59, 21:00–21:59, 22:00–22:59, and after 23:00 h). Delayed bedtime was defined as 22:00 h or later. This was based on the reported average bedtime of 21:29 h for Japanese infants and toddlers [23] and the fact that studies of children and Japanese survey reports set the cutoff point for delayed bedtime at 22:00 h [24,25,26].

2.3.2. Exposure

For child’s screen media use as exposure, the following strategies were used in the analysis. First, we examined whether single-device and multi-device use of each of the four devices (smartphone, tablet, portable game, and console game) was associated with delayed bedtime. TV/DVD was not included in the analysis because it was used by nearly 100% of the children (97.0%). Second, we investigated the screen time for which device was strongly associated with delayed bedtime. Third, for smartphones and tablets that allowed access to multiple contents, we examined which uses were associated with delayed bedtimes.
Device use and screen time were investigated by the following questions: “Please indicate the amount of time your child uses the following devices (smartphones, electronic tablets, portable game devices, console game devices, TV/DVDs, and personal computers) on a typical weekday and on weekends.” The devices were selected based on survey reports on children’s media use [1,2,3]. The responses were collected using seven categories (0, 0.1–0.9, 1.0–1.9, 2.0–2.9, 3.0–3.9, 4.0–4.9, and ≥5 h) and converted to corresponding numbers ranging from “0 h” to “5.5 h.” Time used for studying at school, cram school, and so forth, was excluded. Daily screen time on each device was calculated as the average of 5 weekdays and 2 weekends. Screen time on weekdays and weekends was also calculated as an average. Total screen time was calculated for four devices: smartphones, tablets, portable game devices, and console game devices. The total screen time including TV/DVD use was not analyzed because of a small number of children in the “none (0 h/day)” group (14 cases). Use of a personal computer was not included in the analysis as few children used it.

2.3.3. Covariates

Covariates were selected a priori based on the literature [9,17,21] and biological plausibility. The adjusted model included child’s sex (male/female), age (4, 5, 6, and 7–8 years), presence of elder sibling (no/yes), parents’ age (<40 and ≥40 years), parents’ education (<13, 13–15, and ≥16 years), household income (<4 million, 4–5.9 million, and ≥6 million Japanese yen), mother’s occupation (no/yes), and screen media use within 1 h before sleeping (<1, 1–3, and ≥4 times/week). In an analysis of the association between delayed bedtime and child’s screen time for each device (smartphone, tablet, portable game, console game, and TV/DVD), the adjusted model also included screen time for other devices. In addition to the adjusted model, parents’ screen time for mobile devices (≤1, 1.1–2, 2.1–3, and >3 h/day) was included as Adjusted Model 2. Daily screen time for parents’ mobile devices was calculated in the same way as for children’s screen time.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

The characteristics of the participants of this study and other JECS participant mothers were compared using the JECS datasets. Pearson’s chi-squared test or Fisher’s exact test for categorical variables and Mann–Whitney U test for continuous variables were used to test for between-group differences. Multivariable binomial logistic regression analyses were performed to examine the association between screen media use and delayed bedtime. The missing values for covariates were complemented by multiple imputations. In the process, each missing value was replaced with a series of substituted plausible values by creating 10 filled-in complete datasets using exposures, an outcome, and covariates.
We performed two sensitivity analyses to assess the robustness of the findings. (1) Complete case analyses were performed by excluding missing data for covariates. (2) Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (no/yes) for the child was added to Adjusted Model 2 for the association between screen time and delayed bedtime. All analyses were conducted using SPSS ver. 27 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Characteristics of Respondents

Of the 5068 mothers who participated in the JECS at the Chiba Regional Centre, 1909 volunteered for this adjunct study, and 1837 were eligible respondents with complete data for bedtime and screen media use. The eligible respondents were more likely to have slightly higher parental age, higher education and income, and lesser media use among mothers and children when the child was 3 years old. Other characteristics of eligible respondents were similar to the other participants of the JECS (Table S1).
Table 1 shows the characteristics of the eligible respondents. Among children, 51.2% were boys. The children’s ages ranged from 4 years to 8 years and 2 months. More than half of the children went to bed between 21:00 and 21:59 h, and 11.4% after 22:00 h. Most children used TV/DVD (97.0%). For other devices, 67.3% of the children used smartphones, 37.8% used tablets, 37.3% used portable games, and 26.8% used console games. Children’s media device use was displayed in a Venn diagram to visualize the intersection of multi-device use (Figure 1). Among children in the eligible respondents, 11.3% used only TV/DVD, 85.6% used TV/DVD and any devices other than TV/DVD, and 2.3% used only devices other than TV/DVD.

3.2. Association between Single- or Multi-Device Use and Children’s Delayed Bedtime

We examined the association between the number of devices used and delayed bedtime for smartphones, tablets, portable games, and console games (Table 2). Among all children, 222 (12.1%) were non-device users (0.8% did not use any device and 11.3% used only TV/DVD), 627 (34.1%) used only one device (apart from TV/DVD), and 988 (53.8%) used two or more devices. For all four devices, multi-device users were more likely to delay bedtime compared with non-device users, with adjusted odds ratios (aOR) [95% confidence interval (CI)] of 1.98 [1.12–3.50] for smartphone and other device users, 2.08 [1.16–3.71] for tablet and other device users, 2.09 [1.16–3.76] for portable game and other device users, and 2.32 [1.27–4.24] for console game and other device users. Smartphone-only users also tended to delay bedtime. Children who used three or four devices had a higher risk of delayed bedtime, with aOR [95% CI] of 2.31 [1.23–4.34] for three devices and 4.33 [2.11–8.90] for four devices.

3.3. Association between Screen Time and Children’s Delayed Bedtime

For smartphones, a screen time of less than 1 h/day was associated with delayed bedtime (aOR [95% CI], 1.85 [1.25–2.75]), and longer screen time showed a stronger association with delayed bedtime (4.94 [2.85–8.55] for 1.1–2 h/day and 3.70 [1.73–7.87] for >2 h/day) (Table 3). Delayed bedtime was also associated with more than 1 h/day of screen time for portable games (2.33 [1.29–4.20] for 1.1–2 h/day and 2.87 [1.24–6.61] for >2 h/day) and with more than 2 h/day screen time for tablets (2.88 [1.63–5.09]) and console games (4.04 [1.21–13.44]). Conversely, a screen time of 2 h/day or less for TV/DVD was negatively associated with delayed bedtime (0.35 [0.16–0.81] for 0.1–1 h/day and 0.44 [0.20–0.96] for 1.1–2 h/day). Increased total screen time was associated with delayed bedtime (2.30 [1.25–4.24] for 1.1–2 h/day, 3.66 [1.69–7.90] for 3.1–4 h/day, and 8.57 [4.16–17.65] for >4 h/day). When parents’ mobile device screen time was additionally adjusted (Adjusted Model 2), the odds ratios for smartphones, tablets, and portable games changed little. In contrast, the odds ratio for console games with screen time of more than 2 h/day increased.
When screen time was divided into weekdays and weekends, the odds ratio for delayed bedtime increased as the total screen time increased from more than 1 h/day on both days. Overall, children spent less time using media devices on weekdays than on the weekend. However, those who spent a long time using devices on weekdays were more likely to have delayed bedtime (more than 1 h/day for smartphones and portable games, more than 2 h for tablets and console games, and more than 2 h/day of total screen time) (Table 4).

3.4. Association between Internet Content and Children’s Delayed Bedtime

Among the kinds of Internet content accessed on smartphones and tablets, gaming was associated with delayed bedtime (aOR [95% CI] of 1.75 [1.16–2.65] for smartphones and 2.20 [1.31–3.69] for tablets). Intellectual training on smartphones was also associated with delayed bedtime (1.65 [1.05–2.60]). Smartphone users accessing three or more kinds of content were more likely to delay bedtime than users accessing only one kind of content (2.23 [1.34–3.70]) (Table 5).

3.5. Sensitivity Analysis

Two sensitivity analyses were performed, one for complete cases by excluding missing data for covariates (n = 1741), and the other by including doctor-diagnosed autism spectrum disorder (yes = 0.5%), in the adjusted model of the analysis of the association between children’s screen time and delayed bedtime. In both analyses, as in the main analysis, prolonged screen time for smartphones, tablets, portable games, and console games and their total screen time were positively associated with delayed bedtime, and screen time for TV/DVD was negatively associated with delayed bedtime.

4. Discussion

To identify the media environments that are associated with bedtime delay among Japanese children aged 4 to 8 years, we conducted analyses focusing on three key factors: (1) single or multiple device use, including smartphones, tablets, portable games, and console games; (2) screen time per device (including TV/DVD); and (3) Internet content accessed via smartphones and tablets.
Almost all children used TV/DVD (97.0%), but 85.6% of them also used either a smartphone, tablet, portable game, or console game, with a minority (11.3%) using only TV/DVD. Nowadays, even young children use multiple screen media, not just TV/DVDs [1,2,3]. Children who use only TV/DVDs possibly belong to families that strictly control their children’s media use.
Cultural differences may cause differences in bedtime [23,24]. Mindell et al. reported that bedtimes for infants and toddlers in 17 Asian and Caucasian countries ranged from 19:27 to 22:17 h, with 21:29 h being the average in Japan [23]. This study defined delayed bedtime as 22:00 h or later. For four devices, excluding TV/DVD, children who used multiple devices were more likely to delay bedtime than non-device users (those who did not use any of the four devices). In addition, prolonged screen times for individual devices and prolonged total screen times were strongly associated with delayed bedtimes. These results suggest that the overall increase in exposure to these media devices contributed to delayed bedtime. Prolonged screen time on weekdays was particularly strongly associated with delayed bedtime. The associations persisted even after adjusting for risk factors such as children’s device use before sleeping and parental age and education, suggesting that young children’s media use itself was associated with delayed bedtime. While a systematic review has shown that parental device use is associated with increased screen time in children [21], the association between children’s screen time and delayed bedtime was either independent of parental device use or the risk increased after adjusting for parental screen time. Since even young children are likely to use media devices on their own [1,27], it is important to establish rules and create a disciplined environment for children’s media use at home [28].
Smartphones were quite popular among young children [2,3], with 67.3% of children in this study using them. This study showed that smartphone use by young children was remarkably associated with delayed bedtime compared to other devices. Not only multi-device users; but smartphone-only users also tended to delay bedtime. Compared with non-smartphone users, those who used a smartphone were more likely to delay bedtime even for short screen times of 0.1–1 h/day, and longer screen time was strongly associated with delayed bedtime. Children who accessed more types of content on their smartphones tended to delay bedtime; suggesting that smartphone dependence may be linked to later bedtimes.
It should be noted that playing games on any device was likely to lead to delayed bedtimes. Prolonged use of portable and console games was strongly associated with delayed bedtime, and children who used smartphones and tablets for gaming were more likely to delay bedtime. This could be due to the “addictive nature” of gaming [29]. Intellectual training was also found to be associated with delayed bedtime for smartphones. This may be because educational apps for young children are designed to be enjoyed as games. However, video sharing was the most popular use of smartphones and tablets but was not associated with delayed bedtimes.
Our results support other studies that have reported that smartphone/tablet use, and gaming, were related to shorter sleep or later bedtime in young children [14,15,16,17,18]. However, unlike previous findings [17,18], the present study showed a lesser delay in bedtime for children who viewed TV/DVDs than for those who did not. This may be attributed to the fact that considerably fewer children did not use TV/DVD, and among these children, a larger proportion (19.4%) had delayed bedtime. Some other activity other than TV/DVD viewing may have contributed to delayed bedtime. Alternatively, young children may rather watch TV/DVDs at night as a kind of sleep aid [30] and go to bed earlier. Moreover, it could be that children are not as focused on the TV screen as they are on their smartphones, tablets, or game consoles [20].
Several possible mechanisms can be proposed for the association of screen media use with delayed bedtime. First, time spent using media devices can directly delay bedtime [7]. Second, exposure to bright light from the screen at night may suppress the secretion of the sleep-promoting hormone melatonin, and thus, delay the circadian rhythm [31]. Melatonin suppression by exposure to bright light from self-luminous devices may last for 2 h [32]. Third, screen media use, especially gaming in the evenings, may cause physiological arousal [33].
The strength of this study is that it collected a wide range of information on children’s device use. However, this study also has several limitations. First, respondents who participated in this adjunct study and had complete data represented 36.2% of the mothers who participated in the JECS at the Chiba Regional Center. The participation rates of the adjunct study may have caused a non-response bias. Compared to eligible respondents, the other mothers who participated in the JECS had a slightly larger number of children with longer media use at 3 years of age. Therefore, non-response bias may contribute to underestimating the prevalence of prolonged device use. However, the bias may have little effect on the associations between outcomes and factors and is not likely to pose a threat to the validity of the results [34,35]. Second, screen time for media devices in this study was surveyed through a self-administered questionnaire completed by mothers. This method could conceivably underestimate actual screen time due to social desirability bias or parents’ lack of awareness of their children’s device use, but it could also overestimate [36]. Third, other leisure time, such as children’s outdoor play time, was not measured and therefore not adjusted for. In addition, the prolonged use of media devices may include factors related to the child’s atypical development [37,38]. In our sensitivity analysis, additional adjustment for autism diagnosed before age 4 resulted in little change in odds ratios. However, no adjustment was made for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder because data were not available by age 4. Fourth, because this study was conducted in suburban and rural areas for birth cohort participants, caution should be exercised in generalizing the study findings. Nevertheless, the prevalence of screen media use in young children in this study was similar to the results of a survey conducted by the Cabinet Office in Japan in 2018 [3]. Therefore, the present results seem to be representative to some extent. Finally, children’s bedtime, device use, and other factors were examined only cross-sectionally; therefore, their causal relationships could not be clarified. Longitudinal studies are needed to investigate these causal relationships.
The media environment has changed markedly and is expected to continue to change. The COVID-19 pandemic has expanded the use of screen media in early childhood [39]. Creating an environment for the appropriate use of media devices by young children requires an approach that is tailored to each family’s situation. Social support tools, such as the American Academy of Pediatrics recommendations [4], may be necessary to help children and their families develop feasible strategies to determine which media to use and how. Children’s prolonged media use may not be controlled by a parental approach alone. There may be socioeconomic or environmental factors (such as having only a few playgrounds) that lead to children and their families spending a longer time on media [40], and future research should take these factors into account.

5. Conclusions

This study found that multi-device use, prolonged screen time, and smartphone use and gaming were associated with delayed bedtime in early childhood. In order to ensure adequate sleep for young children, a home environment with appropriate media device use is necessary, and school educators and health care providers may need to support each family to address this in a feasible manner.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph19159464/s1, Table S1: Comparison of the characteristics of children and parents between eligible respondents in the current study and other JECS participant mothers.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.Y. and H.M.; methodology, M.Y. and H.M.; formal analysis, M.Y.; investigation, M.Y.; data curation, M.Y. and C.M.; writing—original draft preparation, M.Y.; writing—review and editing, K.S., H.M. and C.M.; supervision, K.S. and C.M.; funding acquisition, M.Y. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported in part by a grant from Chiba Foundation for Health Promotion & Disease Prevention (grant number: 4974-17). JECS was funded by the Ministry of the Environment, Japan. The findings and conclusions of this article are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not represent the official views of the abovementioned government.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of Medicine, Chiba University (3028).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in JECS. In this adjunct study of JECS, an informed consent procedure was not performed, but the opportunity to refuse participation was ensured because the questionnaire stated that answering the questions was voluntary.

Data Availability Statement

Data are unsuitable for public deposition due to ethical restrictions and legal framework of Japan. It is prohibited by the Act on the Protection of Personal Information (Act No. 57 of 30 May 2003, amendment on 9 September 2015) to publicly deposit the data containing personal information. Ethical Guidelines for Medical and Health Research Involving Human Subjects enforced by the Japan Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology and the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare also restricts the open sharing of epidemiologic data.

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our gratitude to all the study participants.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Holloway, D.; Green, L.; Livingstone, S. Zero to Eight: Young Children and Their Internet Use; EU Kids Online; LSE: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Rideout, V. Zero to Eight: Children’s Media Use in America; Common Sense Media: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  3. Cabinet Office. Heisei 28 Survey on Internet Usage Environment among Youth. 2017. Available online: https://www8.cao.go.jp/youth/youth-harm/chousa/h28/net-jittai_child/pdf-index.html (accessed on 24 July 2022). (In Japanese).
  4. American Academy of Pediatrics. AAP Announces New Recommendations for Children’s Media Use. 2016. Available online: dnr.vnr1.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/10/PR-Media-Use-FINAL-101216.pdf (accessed on 24 July 2022).
  5. Lissak, G. Adverse Physiological and Psychological Effects of Screen Time on Children and Adolescents: Literature Review and Case Study. Environ. Res. 2018, 164, 149–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Mougharbel, F.; Goldfield, G.S. Psychological Correlates of Sedentary Screen Time Behaviour Among Children and Adolescents: A Narrative Review. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2020, 9, 493–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Cain, N.; Gradisar, M. Electronic Media Use and Sleep in School-aged Children and Adolescents: A Review. Sleep Med. 2010, 11, 735–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Hale, L.; Guan, S. Screen Time and Sleep among School-aged Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Literature Review. Sleep Med. Rev. 2015, 21, 50–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Carter, B.; Rees, P.; Hale, L.; Bhattacharjee, D.; Paradkar, M.S. Association Between Portable Screen-Based Media Device Access or Use and Sleep Outcomes: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2016, 170, 1202–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Dutil, C.; Podinic, I.; Sadler, C.M.; da Costa, B.G.; Janssen, I.; Ross-White, A.; Saunders, T.J.; Tomasone, J.R.; Chaput, J.P. Sleep Timing and Health Indicators in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Health Promot. Chronic Dis. Prev. Can. 2022, 42, 150–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Matricciani, L.; Paquet, C.; Galland, B.; Short, M.; Olds, T. Children’s Sleep and Health: A Meta-review. Sleep Med. Rev. 2019, 46, 136–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Quist, J.S.; Sjödin, A.; Chaput, J.P.; Hjorth, M.F. Sleep and Cardiometabolic Risk in Children and Adolescents. Sleep Med. Rev. 2016, 29, 76–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Jones, R.A.; Hinkley, T.; Okely, A.D.; Salmon, J. Tracking Physical Activity and Sedentary Behavior in Childhood: A Systematic Review. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2013, 44, 651–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Kim, S.Y.; Han, S.; Park, E.J.; Yoo, H.J.; Park, D.; Suh, S.; Shin, Y.M. The Relationship Between Smartphone Overuse and Sleep in Younger Children: A Prospective Cohort Study. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2020, 16, 1133–1139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Moorman, J.D.; Harrison, K. Beyond Access and Exposure: Implications of Sneaky Media Use for Preschoolers’ Sleep Behavior. Health Commun. 2019, 34, 529–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Nathanson, A.I.; Beyens, I. The Relation Between Use of Mobile Electronic Devices and Bedtime Resistance, Sleep Duration, and Daytime Sleepiness Among Preschoolers. Behav. Sleep Med. 2018, 16, 202–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Zhang, Z.; Adamo, K.B.; Ogden, N.; Goldfield, G.S.; Okely, A.D.; Kuzik, N.; Crozier, M.; Hunter, S.; Predy, M.; Carson, V. Longitudinal Correlates of Sleep Duration in Young Children. Sleep Med. 2021, 78, 128–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Beyens, I.; Nathanson, A.I. Electronic Media Use and Sleep Among Preschoolers: Evidence for Time-Shifted and Less Consolidated Sleep. Health Commun. 2019, 34, 537–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Magee, C.A.; Lee, J.K.; Vella, S.A. Bidirectional Relationships Between Sleep Duration and Screen Time in Early Childhood. JAMA Pediatr. 2014, 168, 465–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Nishioka, T.; Hasunuma, H.; Okuda, M.; Taniguchi, N.; Fujino, T.; Shimomura, H.; Tanaka, Y.; Shima, M.; Takeshima, Y.; Japan Environment And Children’s Study Group. Effects of Screen Viewing Time on Sleep Duration and Bedtime in Children Aged 1 and 3 Years: Japan Environment and Children’s Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 3914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Paudel, S.; Jancey, J.; Subedi, N.; Leavy, J. Correlates of Mobile Screen Media Use Among Children Aged 0–8: A Systematic Review. BMJ Open 2017, 7, e014585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Kawamoto, T.; Nitta, H.; Murata, K.; Toda, E.; Tsukamoto, N.; Hasegawa, M.; Yamagata, Z.; Kayama, F.; Kishi, R.; Ohya, Y.; et al. Rationale and Study Design of the Japan Environment and Children’s Study (JECS). BMC Public Health 2014, 14, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Mindell, J.A.; Sadeh, A.; Wiegand, B.; How, T.H.; Goh, D.Y. Cross-Cultural Differences in Infant and Toddler Sleep. Sleep Med. 2010, 11, 274–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Oka, Y.; Suzuki, S.; Inoue, Y. Bedtime Activities, Sleep Environment, and Sleep/Wake Patterns of Japanese Elementary School Children. Behav. Sleep Med. 2008, 6, 220–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Lin, J.D.; Tung, H.J.; Hsieh, Y.H.; Lin, F.G. Interactive Effects of Delayed Bedtime and Family-Associated Factors on Depression in Elementary School Children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 2036–2044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare. Heisei 27 National Nutrition Survey on Preschool Children. 2016. Available online: https://www.mhlw.go.jp/file/06-Seisakujouhou-11900000-Koyoukintoujidoukateikyoku/0000134460.pdf (accessed on 24 July 2022). (In Japanese).
  27. Kabali, H.K.; Irigoyen, M.M.; Nunez-Davis, R.; Budacki, J.G.; Mohanty, S.H.; Leister, K.P.; Bonner, R.L., Jr. Exposure and Use of Mobile Media Devices by Young Children. Pediatrics 2015, 136, 1044–1050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Strasburger, V.C.; Hogan, M.J. Children, Adolescents, and the Media. Pediatrics 2013, 132, 958–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Gentile, D.A.; Bailey, K.; Bavelier, D.; Brockmyer, J.F.; Cash, H.; Coyne, S.M.; Doan, A.; Grant, D.S.; Green, C.S.; Griffiths, M.; et al. Internet Gaming Disorder in Children and Adolescents. Pediatrics 2017, 140 (Suppl. 2), S81–S85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Owens, J.; Maxim, R.; McGuinn, M.; Nobile, C.; Msall, M.; Alario, A. Television-Viewing Habits and Sleep Disturbance in School Children. Pediatrics 1999, 104, e27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Higuchi, S.; Motohashi, Y.; Liu, Y.; Maeda, A. Effects of Playing a Computer Game Using a Bright Display on Presleep Physiological Variables, Sleep Latency, Slow Wave Sleep and REM Sleep. J. Sleep Res. 2005, 14, 267–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Wood, B.; Rea, M.S.; Plitnick, B.; Figueiro, M.G. Light Level and Duration of Exposure Determine the Impact of Self-Luminous Tablets on Melatonin Suppression. Appl. Ergon. 2013, 44, 237–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Anderson, C.A.; Bushman, B.J. Effects of Violent Video Games on Aggressive Behavior, Aggressive Cognition, Aggressive Affect, Physiological Arousal, and Prosocial Behavior: A Meta-analytic Review of the Scientific Literature. Psychol. Sci. 2001, 12, 353–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Van Loon, A.J.; Tijhuis, M.; Picavet, H.S.; Surtees, P.G.; Ormel, J. Survey Non-response in the Netherlands: Effects on Prevalence Estimates and Associations. Ann. Epidemiol. 2003, 13, 105–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Cheung, K.L.; Ten Klooster, P.M.; Smit, C.; de Vries, H.; Pieterse, M.E. The Impact of Non-response Bias Due to Sampling in Public Health Studies: A Comparison of Voluntary Versus Mandatory Recruitment in a Dutch National Survey on Adolescent Health. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Anderson, D.R.; Field, D.E.; Collins, P.A.; Lorch, E.P.; Nathan, J.G. Estimates of Young Children’s Time with Television: A Methodological Comparison of Parent Reports with Time-Lapse Video Home Observation. Child Dev. 1985, 56, 1345–1357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Beyens, I.; Valkenburg, P.M.; Piotrowski, J.T. Screen Media Use and ADHD-Related Behaviors: Four Decades of Research. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 9875–9881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Slobodin, O.; Heffler, K.F.; Davidovitch, M. Screen Media and Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic Literature Review. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2019, 40, 303–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. McArthur, B.A.; Racine, N.; Browne, D.; McDonald, S.; Tough, S.; Madigan, S. Recreational Screen Time Before and During COVID-19 in School-aged Children. Acta Paediatr. 2021, 110, 2805–2807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Wang, Q.; Ma, J.; Maehashi, A.; Kim, H. The Associations between Outdoor Playtime, Screen-Viewing Time, and Environmental Factors in Chinese Young Children: The “Eat, Be Active and Sleep Well” Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Venn diagram of children’s media use. Values are in %.
Figure 1. Venn diagram of children’s media use. Values are in %.
Ijerph 19 09464 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of children and parents.
Table 1. Characteristics of children and parents.
N or Mean% or (SD)
1837
Basic characteristics
Child’s sex
Male94051.2
Female89748.8
Child’s age, years
443223.5
561633.5
662634.1
7–81638.9
Elder sibling
082244.7
≥1101555.3
Mother’s current age, years
mean (SD)37.6(4.8)
Father’s current age, years
mean (SD)39.5(5.9)
Missing5.00.3
Mother’s education
Junior or senior high school55330.1
Junior college or vocational81044.1
Undergraduate or above46625.4
Missing80.4
Father’s education
Junior or senior high school69437.8
Junior college or vocational43623.7
Undergraduate or above69437.8
Missing130.7
Household income, million Japanese Yen
<4 55230.0
4 to <663134.3
≥658732.0
Missing673.6
Mother’s occupation
No51327.9
Yes130971.3
Missing150.8
Mother’s marital status
Married173594.4
Single 1025.6
Child’s screen media use within 1 h before sleeping
<1 time/week38821.1
1–3 times/week48926.6
4–7 times/week95552.0
Missing50.3
Child’s media device use
Smartphone123767.3
Tablet69437.8
Portable game68537.3
Console game49226.8
TV/DVD178197.0
Child’s regular bedtime
Before 19:59663.6
20:00–20:5955230.0
21:00–21:59100954.9
22:00–22:5919610.7
After 23:00140.8
SD, standard deviation.
Table 2. Associations between children’s delayed bedtime (after 22:00 h) and use of media devices (N = 1837).
Table 2. Associations between children’s delayed bedtime (after 22:00 h) and use of media devices (N = 1837).
Use Of Media Devicen % of CasesCrudeAdjusted
aOR[95% CI]aOR[95% CI]
Non-device users2227.21.00reference1.00reference
Smartphone
Smartphone-only users40312.71.87[1.04–3.36]1.68[0.92–3.06]
Smartphone and other device users83414.02.10[1.22–3.62]1.98[1.12–3.50]
Tablet
Tablet-only users1195.90.80[0.32–2.01]0.68[0.27–1.73]
Tablet and other device users57515.12.30[1.31–4.01]2.08[1.16–3.71]
Portable game
Portable game-only users636.30.87[0.28–2.71]0.83[0.26–2.64]
Portable game and other device users62213.82.07[1.18–3.61]2.09[1.16–3.76]
Console game
Console game-only users422.40.31[0.04–2.43]0.42[0.05–3.30]
Console game and other device users45014.72.21[1.25–3.92]2.32[1.27–4.24]
Number of devices used
None2227.21.00reference1.00reference
1 device62710.01.44[0.81–2.55]1.32[0.74–2.37]
2 devices58810.51.52[0.86–2.69]1.49[0.82–2.69]
3 devices29514.92.26[1.24–4.12]2.31[1.23–4.34]
4 devices10523.84.02[2.04–7.93]4.33[2.11–8.90]
aOR: adjusted odds ratio; CI: confidence interval. The model included child’s sex, child’s age, presence of elder sibling, parents’ age, parents’ education, household income, mother’s occupation, and screen media use within 1 h before sleeping. Bold numbers indicate p < 0.05.
Table 3. Associations between children’s delayed bedtime (after 22:00 h) and screen time (N = 1837).
Table 3. Associations between children’s delayed bedtime (after 22:00 h) and screen time (N = 1837).
Screen Time n % of casesCrudeAdjustedAdjusted 2
aOR[95% CI]aOR[95% CI]aOR[95% CI]
Smartphone
None6007.01.00reference1.00reference1.00reference
0.1–1 h/day105511.21.56[1.08–2.27]1.85[1.25–2.75]1.90[1.28–2.82]
1.1–2 h/day12528.04.66[2.80–7.75]4.94[2.85–8.55]5.16[2.96–9.00]
>2 h/day5726.33.94[1.96–7.92]3.70[1.73–7.87]3.75[1.74–8.09]
Tablet
None114310.11.00reference1.00reference1.00reference
0.1–1 h/day4639.91.02[0.71–1.47]1.03[0.70–1.51]1.06[0.71–1.56]
1.1–2 h/day14415.31.64[1.00–2.68]1.16[0.68–1.99]1.22[0.70–2.10]
>2 h/day8729.93.51[2.08–5.91]2.88[1.63–5.09]2.76[1.52–5.01]
Portable game
None115210.41.00reference1.00reference1.00reference
0.1–1 h/day5369.70.89[0.62–1.26]1.00[0.68–1.46]1.01[0.69–1.49]
1.1–2 h/day10823.12.76[1.69–4.51]2.33[1.29–4.20]2.45[1.34–4.49]
>2 h/day4131.73.78[1.86–7.66]2.87[1.24–6.61]2.86[1.22–6.70]
Console game
None134510.61.00reference1.00reference1.00reference
0.1–1 h/day39811.81.16[0.81–1.65]1.43[0.96–2.12]1.42[0.95–2.13]
1.1–2 h/day7716.91.68[0.88–3.21]1.26[0.60–2.65]1.27[0.60–2.69]
>2 h/day1741.26.55[2.40–17.87]4.04[1.21–13.44]4.69[1.39–15.77]
TV/DVD
None5619.61.00reference1.00reference1.00reference
0.1–1 h/day3548.20.33[0.15–0.72]0.35[0.16–0.81]0.36[0.16–0.84]
1.1–2 h/day6669.30.41[0.20–0.84]0.44[0.20–0.96]0.45[0.20–1.00]
>2 h/day76114.20.62[0.31–1.25]0.56[0.26–1.22]0.57[0.26–1.26]
Total screen time (4 devices excluding TV/DVD)
None2227.21.00reference1.00reference1.00reference
0.1–1 h/day8617.91.10[0.63–1.94]1.08[0.60–1.92]1.09[0.61–1.95]
1.1–2 h/day41713.42.00[1.12–3.57]2.30[1.25–4.24]2.34[1.26–4.35]
2.1–3 h/day14612.31.81[0.89–3.68]1.74[0.83–3.68]1.73[0.81–3.72]
3.1–4 h/day9419.13.05[1.48–6.28]3.66[1.69–7.90]3.59[1.63–7.91]
>4 h/day9735.16.95[3.60–13.41]8.57[4.16–17.65]8.37[3.88–18.05]
aOR: adjusted odds ratio; CI: confidence interval. Adjusted model included child’s sex, child’s age, presence of elder sibling, parents’ age, parents’ education, household income, mother’s occupation, screen media use within 1 h before sleeping, and child’s screen time of other devices (models for each device). Adjusted model 2 included parents’ screen time of mobile devices to Adjusted model. Bold numbers indicate p < 0.05.
Table 4. Associations between children’s delayed bedtime (after 22:00 h) and screen time on weekdays and weekends (N = 1837).
Table 4. Associations between children’s delayed bedtime (after 22:00 h) and screen time on weekdays and weekends (N = 1837).
WeekdaysWeekends
Screen Timen % of casesaOR[95% CI]n % of casesaOR[95% CI]
Smartphone
None7427.71.00reference6576.81.00reference
0.1–1 h/day92911.71.56[1.11–2.20]92012.21.83[1.27–2.64]
1.1–2 h/day11424.63.85[2.27–6.55]15516.12.50[1.46–4.29]
>2 h/day5230.85.70[2.84–11.45]10526.74.52[2.58–7.90]
Tablet
None122010.01.00reference117010.31.00reference
0.1–1 h/day40310.41.01[0.69–1.48]3729.90.92[0.62–1.37]
1.1–2 h/day14015.71.43[0.86–2.39]14311.91.04[0.60–1.80]
>2 h/day7432.43.72[2.13–6.49]15223.02.08[1.33–3.25]
Portable game
None125310.51.00reference117410.21.00reference
0.1–1 h/day4479.40.96[0.65–1.41]4319.71.04[0.70–1.53]
1.1–2 h/day10424.03.06[1.80–5.18]14719.02.46[1.50–4.02]
>2 h/day3333.34.88[2.18–10.94]8523.53.34[1.85–6.04]
Console game
None146310.91.00reference136010.51.00reference
0.1–1 h/day29112.01.28[0.85–1.93]31812.91.47[0.99–2.17]
1.1–2 h/day7414.91.60[0.80–3.20]11213.41.55[0.85–2.83]
>2 h/day955.613.41[3.19–56.34]4723.43.15[1.49–6.66]
TV/DVD
None8313.31.00reference6317.51.00reference
0.1–1 h/day3839.10.59[0.28–1.25]2246.70.34[0.14–0.80]
1.1–2 h/day6739.10.61[0.30–1.24]4859.50.53[0.25–1.11]
>2 h/day69814.80.94[0.46–1.93]106513.00.63[0.31–1.30]
Total screen time (4 devices excluding TV/DVD)
None3757.21.00reference2546.31.00reference
0.1–1 h/day8639.31.32[0.83–2.11]7608.31.31[0.73–2.34]
1.1–2 h/day33911.21.79[1.04–3.08]36611.72.23[1.19–4.15]
2.1–3 h/day12415.32.43[1.25–4.71]18414.12.36[1.19–4.67]
3.1–4 h/day6624.24.73[2.26–9.88]11017.33.53[1.68–7.43]
>4 h/day7042.912.09[6.09–24.03]16326.45.94[3.04–11.59]
aOR: adjusted odds ratio; CI: confidence interval. The model included child’s sex, child’s age, presence of elder sibling, parents’ age, parents’ education, household income, mother’s occupation, screen media use within 1 h before sleep, and child’s screen time of other devices (models for each device). Bold letters indicate p < 0.05.
Table 5. Associations between children’s delayed bedtime (after 22:00 h) and types of Internet content accessed on smartphones or tablets among Internet users on the devices.
Table 5. Associations between children’s delayed bedtime (after 22:00 h) and types of Internet content accessed on smartphones or tablets among Internet users on the devices.
Smartphone (n = 979)Tablet (n = 623)
n% of casesaOR[95% CI]n% of casesaOR[95% CI]
Internet content accessed on the device 1 (reference: non-use of the content)
Video sharing 69114.31.17[0.76–1.81]48413.21.09[0.60–2.00]
Game49917.41.75[1.16–2.65]27417.22.20[1.31–3.69]
Intellectual training18919.01.65[1.05–2.60]16912.40.94[0.53–1.66]
Music13014.60.94[0.53–1.65]636.30.37[0.13–1.09]
Others9117.61.49[0.80–2.77]3713.51.11[0.39–3.15]
Number of Internet contents accessed on the device
154212.51.00reference33912.71.00reference
229815.41.27[0.83–1.94]18214.31.14[0.66–1.99]
3 or more13920.92.23[1.34–3.70]10214.71.51[0.77–2.97]
aOR: adjusted odds ratio; CI: confidence interval. The model included child’s sex, child’s age, presence of elder sibling, parents’ age, parents’ education, household income, mother’s occupation, and screen media use within 1 h before sleeping. Bold numbers indicate p < 0.05. 1 The model also included all contents.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yamamoto, M.; Mezawa, H.; Sakurai, K.; Mori, C. Association between Media Use and Bedtime Delays in Young Children: An Adjunct Study of the Japan Environment and Children’s Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 9464. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19159464

AMA Style

Yamamoto M, Mezawa H, Sakurai K, Mori C. Association between Media Use and Bedtime Delays in Young Children: An Adjunct Study of the Japan Environment and Children’s Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(15):9464. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19159464

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yamamoto, Midori, Hidetoshi Mezawa, Kenichi Sakurai, and Chisato Mori. 2022. "Association between Media Use and Bedtime Delays in Young Children: An Adjunct Study of the Japan Environment and Children’s Study" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 15: 9464. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19159464

APA Style

Yamamoto, M., Mezawa, H., Sakurai, K., & Mori, C. (2022). Association between Media Use and Bedtime Delays in Young Children: An Adjunct Study of the Japan Environment and Children’s Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(15), 9464. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19159464

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop