Next Article in Journal
Efficacy of Mouth Rinses and Nasal Spray in the Inactivation of SARS-CoV-2: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of In Vitro and In Vivo Studies
Next Article in Special Issue
Religious and Non-Religious Workplace Mobbing Victims: When Do People Turn to Religious Organisations?
Previous Article in Journal
Diagnostic Performance Evaluation of the Novel Index Combining Urinary Cotinine and 4-(Methylnitrosamino)-1-(3-pyridyl)-1-butanol in Smoking Status Verification and Usefulness for Trend Monitoring of Tobacco Smoking Exposure
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Effect of Antecedents of Teachers’ Subjective Career Success
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Stress Is Related to Age, Education, Physical Activity, Body Mass Index, and Body Fat Percentage in Adult Polish Men?

by
Monika Lopuszanska-Dawid
1,*,
Przemysław Kupis
1,
Anna Lipowicz
2,
Halina Kołodziej
2 and
Alicja Szklarska
3
1
Department of Human Biology, Faculty of Physical Education, Józef Piłsudski University of Physical Education in Warsaw, 00-968 Warsaw, Poland
2
Department of Anthropology, Faculty of Biology and Animal Science, Wroclaw University of Environmental and Life Sciences, 50-375 Wroclaw, Poland
3
Polish Academy of Sciences, Poland, Palace of Culture and Science, Defilad Square 1, 00-901 Warsaw, Poland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(19), 12149; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191912149
Submission received: 10 July 2022 / Revised: 18 September 2022 / Accepted: 22 September 2022 / Published: 25 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Stress and Work)

Abstract

:
Stressful events and chronic tension are considered a burden and a threat to physical, mental, and social health. The aim of the study was to demonstrate the associations of variation in stress exposure with social factors, physical activity, basic components of physical fitness, body mass index (BMI) and percentage of body fat (BFP). An additional objective was to identify the main BFP modifiers among those analyzed. The material consisted of data of ethnically homogeneous group 355 men (32–87 yrs), invited to the study as part of the Wroclaw Male Study research project. The analyzed features included socioeconomic status (age, educational level), elements of lifestyle (physical activity), major and most important stressful life events—Social Readjustment Rating Scale (SRRS) and basic parameters of the somatic structure of the body (BMI, BFP). Statistical analyses included: chi-square test, Mann–Whitney U test and backward stepwise regression (significance level α = 0.05). Stress exposure showed significant socioeconomic variation among the adult Poles studied. Higher levels of education were associated with higher levels of stress. Significant correlations between SRRS and physical activity were found, especially in men older than 60 years and with higher levels of education. A positive relationship was shown between SRRS and BFP, especially in men under 60 years of age. BFP appeared to depend mainly on age and stress. The main determinants of SRRS were age and education level, while BFP turned out to be more sensitive to stress than BMI. The modifying force of physical activity for SRRS appears to be age dependent.

1. Introduction

In most societies, stressful events and chronic tension are recognized as a burden and threat to both physical, mental, and social health, as has been repeatedly demonstrated empirically [1,2,3,4]. The more stressful and negative events a person encounters, the more likely they are to suffer injury, illness, disability, or even death [5,6]. Accumulating misfortune, worry, and affliction necessitate many life changes over a short period of time, often exceeding a person’s ability to cope and adapt, increasing vulnerability to infection, injury, and illness. Consequently, emotional disorders, inflammation, migraine, muscle pain, skin diseases, respiratory disorders, gastric disorders, and additionally other serious diseases such as obesity, autoimmune diseases, depression, and atherosclerosis may develop [7,8,9,10]. Not insignificant are the consequences in social health, which can manifest as conflicts with others, exclusion, and problems with building relationships [2]. Many studies dealing with stress in the context of health prevention mainly consider significant life events, often unexpected [11]. Other researchers indicate that it is chronic tensions and ongoing difficulties, although of lower magnitude and expected, that may have worse health effects. Examples of such tensions include family conflicts, difficult financial situation, disability coexisting with dependence on others, problems at work, and unfavorable environments [8,11]. It is suggested that these ongoing difficulties may have a stronger impact on mental health than single and more severe events, such as wars or natural disasters [12,13,14,15]. When analyzed together, unexpected stressors along with chronic tensions and ongoing difficulties can explain up to 40% of the variation in psychological distress and depressive symptoms [12,13].
Although being increasingly studied, the health consequences of stress still require in-depth and multifaceted analyses to identify the most important modifiers [16]. In most cases, stress is triggered by the occurrence of a stressor that threatens or limits control and causes negative emotions and agitation. The duration of stress-induced reactions and their intensity are determined by several factors, mainly related to the characteristics of a given stressor and the possibilities of recovery from the situation [17]. Many coping strategies are known, among them the three main ones: being task-focused, emotion-focused, and avoidance-focused. From the perspective of this study, and because the data does not include task-focused measures, avoidance-focused and emotion-focused coping appear to be of key importance. In many cases, using avoidance may, unfortunately, have worse health consequences than the stressor itself [18]. Dealing with stress aimed at regulating emotions, mainly lowering unpleasant emotional states and strengthening positive emotions, will rather have a positive effect on health, mainly on the immune and endocrine system [19]. There are also some moderators that mitigate these reactions and their effects, including support from others, individual psychophysical endurance, self-esteem, coping skills, preparation for circumstances, and exercise [14].
Among the adult populations of many countries, one of the key modifiers of the intensity of perceived stress is the social position associated with individuals’ educational levels and occupational status. Some researchers point to the varying abilities to adapt to stressors of both individuals and entire social groups, meaning that the effects of long-term psychosocial stress may vary socially. The observed variation in sensitivity to stress stimuli, but also unequal stress coping abilities, may determine the overall body response or group reaction. In this light, differences in the level of experienced psychosocial stress are considered to be one of the important reasons for the presence of social gradients in biological measures of health [20].
Stress-induced health effects can occur both directly, through specific bodily responses to stressors that occur [21], and indirectly, through changes in health behavior, habits, or decisions that may be risky to health. Unhealthy diet, overweight, physical inactivity, smoking tobacco, alcohol abuse, and irregular sleep are examples of changes in basic lifestyle elements that undoubtedly have implications for human health [17,22]. Unhealthy behavior is seen as rewarding and soothing in the context of stress [23]. Associations between stress and these variables have been repeatedly demonstrated in research, although analyzing them is not easy because of the multiplicity of other relevant factors, and the findings are not always in agreement [17]. The impact of stress on health behavior can vary in direction and strength, which depend on many factors (mainly personal) and the stage of life a person is at. Lack of sense of control, low self-esteem, and stressor-induced anxiety can foster unhealthy behavior (if not going to include dispositional factor). Health behavior is particularly important from the perspective of individual biological condition, as it becomes habitual and has a significant impact on health condition [24]. It is usually considered a way to cope with stress. The likelihood of falling into unhealthy habits under stress has been shown to be higher in young people, even before working age. Adults, on the other hand, are more likely to change health behavior than youth. The nature of the effect of such a change that occurs under stress is age-dependent. Another equally important concept is the reverse causality of stress and health behavior. Engaging in certain health behaviors may increase or decrease the likelihood of a stressor [24].
Research has shown that lifestyle modifications to stressors are highly dependent on psychological predispositions and can be dichotomously different, such as increasing the habit of snacking versus lack of appetite and aversion to food intake [25], increasing alcohol consumption versus abstinence, or increasing versus decreasing tobacco smoking [23,26,27]. Stress-induced eating behavior varies according to gender, baseline body mass index, and other individual preferences and factors [28]. One such factor is cortisol, which, when secreted as a response to stress, has a direct effect on weight gain. Cortisol leads to fat accumulation and inhibits growth hormone secretion, which would counteract this accumulation. Therefore, exposure to the effects of stress through elevated cortisol levels can lead to fat accumulation. However, the production of this hormone has wide individual variation [29]. Many studies have explored the importance of stress as a factor in weight gain and future obesity through eating disorders associated with physical inactivity [28]. However, the relationships shown are often weak or not always present, which may be due to the time it takes to gain or lose weight from the time the stressor occurred, so the effects are observed with some delay, even several months. The body mass index, or BMI, which has been most commonly chosen for testing, is also a significant limitation, and in many cases is subject to a significant error [30].
The aim of the study was to demonstrate the directions and strength of stress exposure variability with social factors, physical activity, basic physical fitness factors, body mass index (BMI), and percentage of body fat (BFP). The necessity to carry out in-depth research in this area results from the existing gap in the scientific literature. Most of the research concerns single-factor relationships of the stress-BMI type. The presented results focus on multifaceted and multilevel relationships, including groups of modifiers of perceived stress, such as social factors, lifestyle elements and basic parameters of biological condition. In today’s turbulent times, the issue of stress and its effect on an individual’s life requires in-depth and multifaceted research. Further exploration of modifiers of perceived stress and its health effects appears to be particularly important during unstable and stressful periods. Identification of groups at risk of elevated stress may have important practical implications, especially in terms of education on methods of coping with stressors to mitigate its health consequences.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

The material for the study consisted of sociological and biomedical data of 355 men aged 32–87 years, randomly invited to the study as part of the Wroclaw Male Study research project (WMS) [20,31,32,33,34,35]. The men were active, healthy inhabitants of the city of Wroclaw (Poland, Lower Silesia, population in 2010: 632,996, including men: 294,960), and they were medically examined in 2000 and in 2010/11 under the WMS research project in the Lower Silesian Medical Centre DOLMED S.A. in Wroclaw (Poland).
The group has been described in detail in previous publications, especially the most recent one [20]. The group was ethnically homogeneous (Poles, Caucasian) with no national, linguistic, religious, or racial minorities. This population had no history of any physical and mental disorders. On physical examination, they presented no pathologies. Men included in the study did not suffer from any essential chronic diseases actually and had not done so in the past. Subjects taking medications, especially drugs that could interfere with hormone metabolism or mental health, were excluded from the analysis. The men were not selected for any biological parameters or in another way. To eliminate the impact of human mobility on the biological condition, the important inclusion criteria were that participants of this study were inhabitants of the city of Wroclaw [36]. The original group consisted of 384 individuals, but 7.6 percent of men were excluded from the analysis and the final database for further analysis included 355 men. The enrolment rate was similar to that observed in Poland and amounted to about 25 percent [37,38]. Unfortunately, determining the direction of possible material selectivity is impossible [20]. It is probable that only individuals with a high level of health awareness and volunteers for all types of preventive medical examinations took part in this research project. Thus, the results obtained may be more favorable than expected.

2.2. Ethical Approval

The study proposal was approved by the Bioethical Committee of Medical University in Wroclaw, Poland (approval number: 477/2000) and conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. All persons were orally informed about the aims of the projects and all testing procedures and all gave their informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study. At any time, the subjects could withdraw without giving any reason. All participants signed informed consent.

2.3. Measurements

2.3.1. Qualitative Survey Data

The respondents completed a questionnaire containing basic information about their socioeconomic status: age (in years) (1: under 50 yrs, 2: 50–59 yrs, 3: 60–69 yrs, 4: 70 yrs and more), educational level (1: university, 2: secondary school, 3: vocational (and/or primary school)), and level of physical activity (1: inactive, 2: medium, 3: active).

2.3.2. Anthropometric Measurements

The relative body mass index (BMI) was calculated based on self-measured body height and body weight according to the standard Martin technique [39,40,41]. Body height was measured as the distance of the vertex point from the basis (B-v: Basis–vertex) while maintaining the Frankfurt plane by the Martin anthropometer (GMP Gneupel Präzisions-Mechanik, Zurich, Switzerland) with an accuracy of 1 mm [42]. Although it is more time-consuming, body height measurements give much more reliable results than in the case of body height declared by study participants [43]. Body weight (to the nearest 0.1 kg) and percentage of body fat (PBF) (to the nearest 0.1%) were measured during the same examination, using a scale with a Soehnle Professional 7850 body composition analyzer (Soehnle Industrial Solutions GmbH, Backnang, Germany). The measurements were taken in the morning. The participants were standing upright, without shoes, in their underwear, and they were fasting. For participants up to age 65, normal BMI was adopted as 18.5–24.9 [44], whereas for age 65 and over, this was 25.0 to 27.0 [45,46]. There were no subjects with a below-normal BMI among the participants.

2.3.3. The Social Readjustment Rating Scale

The Social Readjustment Rating Scale (SRRS) [47] was used for identifying major and most important stressful life events. SRRS helps assess the stress load, more specifically, objective stressors, i.e., those that cause characteristic symptoms in all or almost all people. The tool is formed by a list of 43 stressful life events that are assigned corresponding Life Change Units (LCU) on a scale of 0 to 100, depending on how traumatic an impact they had in the study on the project participants (from Death of spouse rated as 100 LCU to Change in eating habits rated as 15 LCU) [20]. Stress is cumulative; in order to estimate the experienced total stress, the scores from each event that occurred over a 12-month period must be added together (which gives the total number of stressful life events for an individual). The authors noted a significant positive relationship between the magnitude of stress and health, and thus, as the number of LCUs increases, so does the incidence of diseases, i.e., the risk of stress-related health breakdown. There is a statistically significant correlation between the strength of stressors and the probability of developing a serious illness within two consecutive years. If the total strength of stressors is 150–199 LCU points, the probability of getting sick is 37%, 200–299 LCU points: 51%, over 300 LCU points: 79% [47]. Because the sample under study consists of a substantially healthy group and due to the small number of respondents declaring stressor levels 150 LCU points and above, the original scale has been extended and three additional lowest levels of strength of perceived stressors have been introduced: 0 LCU points, 1–49 LCU points, 50–149 LCU points. Details: In this study, the SRRS scale modified by the authors was used. The following categories of LCU stress points were used: 1: 0 points, 2: 1–49 points, 3: 50–149 points, 4: 150–199 points, 5: 200–299 points, 6: over 300 points. Categories 1–3 were introduced by the authors to refine the original category 0–149 points. In order to estimate the amount of the probability of developing a serious illness within two consecutive years, the authors calculated that since the original 0–149 points category generates max. 36% risk, therefore 1 point generates 0.24% risk (36%/149 points = 0.24% per points). Therefore the levels of strength of perceived stressors were distinguished 1–6, which may correspond to, respectively, approx. 0%, 6%, 24%, 37%, 51% and 79% of the risk. Method for calculating the risk for the selected new categories (1–3): 1: 0 points—0 * 0.24% = 0% of the risk; 2: 1–49 points—((1 * 0.24%) + (49 * 0.24%))/2 = 12%/2 = 6%; 3: 50–149 points—((50 * 0.24%) + (149 * 0.24%))/2 = 47.76%/2 = 23.88%, therefore rounded to the whole—24%.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

Descriptive and mathematical statistical methods were used to present the results (e.g., medians, percentage frequencies). The distributions of continuous variables were not normal, as verified using the Shapiro–Wilk and Jarque–Bera tests and therefore non-parametric tests were applied: chi-square test (χ2) and Mann–Whitney U test. Differences between the proportions of age and educational level categories (respectively: under 50 yrs, 50–59 yrs, 60–69 yrs, 70 yrs and more; university, secondary, vocational school) in terms of level of physical activity (inactive, medium, active), BMI (in the norm, above of the norm), BFP categories (10–19%, 20–29%, 30%+) and stress categories (0, 1–49, 50–149, 150 and more points) were analysed with the use of the chi-square test. The Mann–Whitney U test was used to evaluate the mean differences between the independent samples (respectively: 1. in terms of age and education levels, 2. levels of physical activity in terms of age and educational level, 3. BMI categories in age and education groups, 4. PBF categories in groups age and educational level).
The main modifiers of the basic physical fitness parameter of the male participants were selected in the course of the subsequent analysis steps in the manuscript and confirmed in the literature. Backward stepwise regression was used to assess the strength of the relationships of the main modifiers of the basic biological characteristics of the studied men. BFP was a dependent variable, whereas the age, stress (as continuous variables), educational level and physical activity (as qualitative variables) were independent variables (not continuous variables were used as a dummy variables). The basic model containing all potential independent variables was constructed, and then the variables were gradually eliminated from the model to maintain the final model with the highest value of the coefficient of determination while retaining only significant parameters. The values of standardized regression coefficient (β—Beta), level of statistical significance (pp-value), R-squared values of determination (R2), the adjusted R2 values of determination (R2 adj.) (information about the percentage of the variance of the dependent variable explained by the determining variable) and model adjustment Fisher test (F) were given. The level of significance was set at α = 0.05. The STATISTICA 13.5, and 12.0. packages were used for analyses (Tulusa, OK, USA) [48].

3. Results

Table 1 part A shows the prevalence of males in the stress categories, followed by age and education categories and the differences in distribution. There were statistically significant differences in distribution of stress intensity with age and education (respectively, p = 0.003 and p = 0.008). Higher levels of stress (150 points and more) are more common in younger age groups (25.8% among men under 50 yrs), while the 60 and over group reported the lowest stress exposure (60–69 yrs: 5.5%, 70 yrs and more: 11.8%). College education, compared to vocational or high school education, had different distributions of the proportions of stress categories. People with higher levels of education were more likely to report higher categories of stress exposure (university versus vocational school—1–49 LCU points: 29.6% versus 17.5%; 50–149 LCU points: 32.1% versus 28.6%; 150+ LCU points: 17.0% versus 6.3%).
Table 1 part B, illustrating the results of the Mann–Whitney U test, reveals significant differences (p = 0.001) in median stress between categories under 50 years and 60–69 years and between 50–59 years and 60–69 years (model I). The median stress of the participants aged 60–69 yrs and 70 yrs and more was found to be lower, but not statistically significant, than the median for those under 60 (respectively, Me = 23 and Me = 39 versus Me = 40 for men under 50 yrs and Me = 51 for 50–59 years old men). In the model II, two age groups were compared: under 60 yrs and 60 years old and over (younger versus older). The median stress of the participants aged under 60 yrs was found to be significantly higher than the median for those under 60 (respectively, Me = 49.5 versus Me = 25). For education, a significant difference occurred between vocational and high school education and between vocational and college education (respectively, p = 0.001, p = 0.028). The median stress of those with vocational education was the lowest (Me = 12) (Table 1, part B).
The above results formed the basis for assumptions in further analyses. Hence, the subsequent analyses compare two age groups: under 60 and 60 years old and over (younger versus older) and two educational groups: college and high school education combined and vocational education (higher levels of education versus lower levels of education). This approach is supported by the lack of the statistically significant differences between the median stress for men under 50 and 50–59 years of age, and for men aged 60–69 and 70 and more (Table 1, part B, model I), the occurrence of significant statistical differences between the median stress for men under 60 and 60 yrs and more (Table 1, part B, model II), and the biological differences in men up to 60 and after 60 years of age identified in previous scientific studies concerning Polish men [30].
Table 2, part A shows the prevalence of males in the stress categories, followed by physical activity level by two age groups and two education groups and the differences in distribution. There were statistically significant differences in distribution of stress intensity with physical activity for men aged 60 yrs and more (p = 0.011) and education for men with higher levels of education (p = 0.002). Among men 60 yrs and more, higher levels of stress are more common in active men (LCU = 150+: 7.8% among active versus 3.7% among inactive; LCU = 50–149: 36.7% among active men versus 29.6% among inactive). Most of the Polish men (60 yrs and more) with lower stress levels reported the lowest physical activity (LCU = 0: 59.3% inactive men versus 25.6% active). Among men with higher levels of education, inactive compared to medium or active had different distributions of the proportions of stress categories. Physically active, well-educated men were more likely to report higher categories of stress exposure (active versus inactive–1–49 LCU points: 31.2% versus 11.1%; 50–149 LCU points: 32.4% versus 25.9%; 150+ LCU points: 17.3% versus 16.7%).
The results of the Mann–Whitney U tests (Table 2 part B) indicated that statistically significant differences for median stress were obtained in the 60+ group between inactive and active individuals (p = 0.008) and between moderately active and active individuals (p = 0.030). In this group, the median stress of the very active individuals (Me = 39) was found to be significantly higher than for other groups (inactive and moderately active, Me = 0 and Me = 5.5, respectively). This result confirms and clarifies the results presented above. For the more highly educated group, there was a significant difference between the inactive and the active individuals (p = 0.040). The median stress of very active (Me = 49) versus inactive individuals (Me = 21.5) was more than twice as high.
Table 3, part A presents the prevalence of males in the stress categories, followed by Body Mass Index by two age groups and two education groups and the differences in distribution. There were no statistically significant differences in distribution of stress intensity with BMI categories for age and education categories (respectively, p = 0.437 for men under 60 yrs; p = 0.611 for men 60 yrs and more; p = 0.715 for well-educated; p = 0.639 for poorly educated). However, a trend can be observed that, among men the highest level of stress, the normal range of BMI was more common (for example, in the group under 60 yrs with LCU = 150+ 23.3% men had BMI in the norm versus 17.9% were above of the norm; in group 60 yrs and more, respectively: 8.6% versus 7.1%) (Table 3, part A).
Table 3, part B, illustrating the results of the Mann–Whitney U test, reveals no significant differences in median stress between Body Mass Index categories under age and education groups (p-values range between 0.078 and 0.788).
Table 4, part A presents the prevalence of males in the stress categories, followed by Body Fat Percentage categories by two age groups and two education groups and the differences in distribution. Although the results of the χ2 were found to be statistically insignificant (p-values range between 0.111 and 0.517), it is worth noting that, in the group with lower stress categories (e.g., LCU = 0), men with the highest category of BFP (30% and more) were slightly more frequent (among men under 60 yrs: 35.5% had BFP 30%+ versus 16.7% had BFP 10–19%; among men 60 yrs and more: respectively 44.4% versus 20.0%). There were no statistically significant differences in distribution of stress intensity with BFP categories for education categories, but the trends in the proportions are similar to those described slightly above. Among men from the lowest stress category (LCU = 0), a slightly larger fraction of men had BFP above 30% than BFP within 10–19% (in the group with higher levels of education had LCU = 0, 36.6% and BFP = 30% and more, whereas 18.8% had 10–19%; in groups with lower levels of education, the corresponding values are 60.9% versus 0%).
The results of the Mann–Whitney U tests (Table 4 part B) indicated that statistically significant differences for median stress were obtained in the younger group (under 60 yrs) between men with a BFP of 10–19% and over 30% (p = 0.012). The median stress in men with more than 30% (Me = 25) body fat percentage appeared to be significantly lower than in those with the lowest body fat (Me = 88). In the group with a university and secondary level of education, the Mann–Whitney U test scores were significantly different between those with 10–19% and over 30% (p = 0.005) and between 20–29% and over 30% body fat (p = 0.017). The median stress (LCU points) for groups 10–19%, 20–29% and over 30% were, respectively, 52, 48 and 32). For the group with vocational education, a significant difference also occurred between those with 10–19% and over 30% body fat (p = 0.027), with an equally significant difference in medians of Me = 65 to Me = 0, respectively.
Given the significance of the associations of stress exposure with age, education, physical activity level, and body fat in a backward regression model, we examined the extent to which BFP, being one of the main parameters of biological fitness, depends on a set of other factors. Table 5 presents the results of backward stepwise regression (both basic and final models), with age, education, physical activity level, and stress being the independent variables and BFP being the dependent variable.
The basic model fit R2 was 21.30, which means that the independent variables (age, education, physical activity, SRRS) explain more than 21% of the variation in body fat of the male participants. At a significance level of 5%, age and SRRS were found to significantly affect the percentage of body fat. For age, a positive relationship with SRRS and a β coefficient of 0.413 were found, whereas, for SRRS, the relation was negative at a coefficient of −0.102. Subsequent backward regression steps discarded irrelevant variables (educational level and physical activity level), and as a result, the model adjustment increased from 23.408 to 45.099. The final model indicates that body fat percentage, with other factors controlled, increases with age and decreases with level of SRRS exposure.

4. Discussion

Stress exposure among male respondents is noticeably socially differentiated. There was also a significant positive correlation between SRRS and physical activity, particularly pronounced for men over 60 years and with a high school or college education. Furthermore, a significant positive relationship was found between SRRS and body fat for men under 60 years of age and in all education groups. It is difficult to unequivocally indicate the direction of the relationship between SRRS and physical activity and body fat, but it appears that education largely determines the results obtained for these variables.

4.1. Stress and Education

A social diversity analysis that took into account the age and education of the participants led to predictable conclusions, which were consistent with those of other authors. Older people appeared to be less stressed. The limit of change in the proportional distribution of SRRS categories was around 60 years of age. Other studies have found that stress levels peak in early adulthood [24]. Stressors such as success in the job market, getting married, and raising children are observed in early or middle adulthood and begin to fade over time. In contrast, the decrease in stress at later ages may be related to the fact that people are transitioning or approaching retirement, thus becoming less exposed to work-related stress. The reduced number of commitments, stressful situations, and a kind of quietude associated with the transition into old age may explain the reduced exposure to stress during this period of life [49]. For the group analyzed in the study, the decline in stress intensity with age may be even stronger due to the high proportion of college-educated individuals whose overall life situation, e.g., financial and health status, may be better than that of their less-educated peers.
Types of stressors, whether related to the socioeconomic environment or the nature of the work performed, tend to be passed from generation to generation, gradually increasing the differences in exposure between advantaged and disadvantaged groups [2]. Stress exposure is unevenly distributed in the population and can cause disparities in well-being, and mental and physical health. Exposure to stressors, according to some studies, is inversely related to social status, putting those with lower social status at a greater disadvantage. They have to face more stress and overcome more tensions [7,11]. This disparity in exposure to stressful events and coping skills may explain health disparities between different social groups [50]. Difficulties and stress occurring in one domain of life can also easily spread to other domains, e.g., from work to home, and cause problems to accumulate [51,52].
The effect of stressors on health and general well-being may be weaker for individuals who have high self-esteem and support from others, use appropriate coping strategies and experience, and are prepared for the stressors that occur [2]. These social and individual assets can effectively undermine the effects of stressors and prevent the process of their accumulation and reproduction. However, these assets are unevenly distributed among social groups, leading to the widening of already existing disparities [2]. A perfect example of this is men and women who approach coping with stressful situations differently by choosing different strategies. Men tend to focus on task-oriented strategies, while women often choose emotion-oriented strategies and seek social support [53].
The results of the present study concerning education indicate that men who were least stressed were the least educated, followed by those with vocational and high school education. The explanation of this can be also found in the literature, suggesting a link between higher levels of education and more demanding occupations. Having such jobs entails higher responsibility, pressures, and more potential stressors, which may be reflected in the individual responses of study participants [17]. However, most scientific papers and the arguments posed in them point to an inverse relationship between stress and education than that obtained here. It is suggested that better-educated people use better coping strategies, have higher self-esteem, better support from those around them, and have greater overall control over their lives [54,55]. It is difficult to argue with such reasoning, but one should take into consideration the strength of the factors mentioned. However, it is worth referring again to the specifics of the group studied, since the age of the participants may be a period in which work is the main and strongest source of stress, overshadowing other factors, even those that are supposed to have a protective effect.

4.2. Stress and Physical Activity

In the context of stress, physical activity is often described as a sort of buffer to mitigate the effects caused by the stressor. Physically active leisure activities serve to calm and relax all body systems. Those who choose this strategy report lower levels of stress than their peers who do not. However, this assessment is subjective and does not necessarily correspond to the actual level of stress exposure [56]. In addition to physical activity, studies have also analyzed physical fitness, which in itself, as physical resistance to a stressor, or indirectly through psychological benefits (e.g., higher self-esteem, endurance) [57], may facilitate coping with stress. It is difficult to separate the effects of these two variables (physical activity and physical fitness) as they are correlated to some extent. As research shows, both can lead to physical and psychological symptoms caused by stress. The intensity and type of physical activity turn out to be of little importance compared to the amount of time spent on it. Active leisure becomes an effective escape from stressor-induced pressures and a process that adds new energy and vigor to face stress. In this context, regular participation in physical activity becomes a kind of protective activity because stress, especially increased stress, is inherently unpredictable [14]. The literature also indicates that highly stressed individuals tend to choose passive leisure activities because, unlike physical activity, they are immediately rewarded with rest and passivity [23]. The results obtained in this study concerning PA seem to confirm mainly the latter direction of the relationship. Physically inactive respondents were significantly less stressed than very active respondents, whether broken down by age group or education group. Highly stressed respondents were more likely to be physically active, using physical activity to relieve the effects (tension) caused by stress.

4.3. SRRS, BMI, and BFP

Basic biological status parameters such as BMI and percentage of body fat showed significantly different and distinct associations with SRRS. BFP was found to be highly significantly associated with stress while BMI was not at all. The men most exposed to stressful events were found to have the lowest levels of body fat, while the least exposed men had the highest levels. In the context of the positive associations of higher levels of physical activity in men with higher stress exposure, the results for BFP are consistent, although different from what is typically presented in research [28]. It seems that, again, education and age may explain such findings. Analysis of other variables would require operating on age-education subgroups, which is not possible due to the sample size because education and age may determine exposure to stressors much more strongly than other variables analyzed in this study. A recent analysis of the determinants of body fat showed that, even with other variables controlled, a significant negative relationship between stress and body fat is observed. This fact also suggests the previously neglected but arguably fundamental problem of endogeneity i.e., cause-and-effect feedback, which we cannot resolve due to a lack of access to relevant instrumental variables.

4.4. Limitation Study

When proceeding to any interpretation of the results obtained in this study, one should take into account a relatively specific study group consisting of relatively well-educated men living in large cities, and the specificity of the stress testing tool. Future research devoted to this topic would benefit from combining both the subjects’ filling the SRRS questionnaire and a question of their subjective perceptions of stress. The comparison of such results could allow for a deeper analysis of the problems studied and a search for explanations and cause-and-effect relationships. Additionally, the use of a modified by authors version of the SRRS scale may be a certain limitation of the study, although it seems that extending the scale may enrich the inference. Subsequent limitation studies can be used a subjective questionnaire to assess the level of physical activity, instead of other objective methods, e.g., accelerometers. Thus, when generalizing the results to the general population, the limitation study must be considered.

5. Conclusions

The issue of stress and its importance to an individual’s life still seems to be neglected these days. Education in methods of coping with stressors and the health effects of exposure to prolonged or intense stress is either absent or occurs superficially without practical value. It seems particularly justified to educate adolescents and young adults in this field, since the accumulation of stress in early adulthood should be preceded by adequate preparation for this period of life. Further scientific exploration of these issues is not without significance, as many questions remain without reliable and proven answers.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.L.-D. and P.K.; methodology, M.L.-D. and P.K.; software, M.L.-D.; validation, M.L.-D., P.K., A.L., H.K. and A.S.; formal analysis, M.L.-D. and P.K.; investigation, M.L.-D., A.L., H.K. and A.S.; resources, M.L.-D.; data curation, M.L.-D.; writing—original draft preparation, M.L.-D. and P.K.; writing—review and editing, M.L.-D., P.K., A.L. and A.S.; visualization, M.L.-D.; supervision, M.L.-D.; project administration, M.L.-D.; funding acquisition, M.L.-D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Polish National Science Center in Poland, followed by Grant No. N N303 543 239 and 6POC 04820 and by the Ministry of Science and Education in 2020/2022 as part of the Scientific School of the University of Physical Education in Warsaw—SN No. 5 “Biomedical determinants of physical fitness and sports training in adult population” (Task 1: “Determinants of biological condition and physical fitness of adult men and women in Poland”) The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study proposal was approved by the Bioethical Committee of Medical University in Wroclaw, Poland (approval No.: 477/2000 z dn. 24.08.2000), and the work described has been carried out in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) for experiments involving humans. The manuscript is in line with the Recommendations for the Conduct, reporting, editing and publication of scholarly work in medical journals.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are within the manuscript. The selected data underlying the results represented in the study are available from the authors. There are ethical/legal restrictions on sharing a raw data set, which includes many confidential data (sensitive patient information) (EU Regulation EU, 2016/679 of the European Parliament and of the Council of 27 April 2016 on the protection of natural persons with regard to the processing of personal data and on the free movement of such data, and repealing Directive 95/46/EC—General Data Protection Regulation, https://eur-lex.europa.eu/eli/reg/2016/679/oj, accessed on 1 January 2022). If necessary, access will be given.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the men who took part in the Wroclaw Male Study project and Ewa A. Jankowska for her help in the WMS project.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Tennant, C. Life Stress, Social Support and Coronary Heart Disease. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 1999, 33, 636–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Thoits, P.A. Stress and Health: Major Findings and Policy Implications. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2010, 51, 41–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Turner, R.J. Understanding Health Disparities: The Promise of the Stress Process Model. In Advances in the Conceptualization and Study of the Stress Process: Essays in Honor of Leonard I. Pearlin; Avison, W.R., Aneshensel, C.S., Schieman, S., Wheaton, B., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 3–21. [Google Scholar]
  4. Liczbińska, G.; Králík, M. Body size at birth in babies born during World War II: The evidence from Poland. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 2020, 32, e23421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Cooper, C.L. Handbook of Stress Medicine and Health; CRC Press: London, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  6. Cohen, S.; Janicki-Deverts, D.; Miller, G.E. Psychological Stress and Disease. JAMA 2007, 298, 1685–1687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Dohrendwend, B.P. Problems in Defining and Sampling the Relevant Population of Stressful Life Events. In Stressful Life Events: Their Nature and Effects; Dohrenwend, B.S., Dohrenwend, B.P., Eds.; John Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1974; pp. 275–310. [Google Scholar]
  8. Brown, G.W.; Harris, T.O. Social Origins of Depression: A Study of Psychiatric Disorder in Women; Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  9. Thoits, P.A. Dimensions of Life Events that Influence Psychological Distress: An Evaluation and Synthesis of the Literature. In Psychosocial Stress: Trends in Theory and Research; Kaplan, H.B., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 33–103. [Google Scholar]
  10. Mirowsky, J.; Ross, C.E. Social Causes of Psychological Distress, 2nd ed.; Aldine de Gruyter: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  11. Pearlin, L.I. The Stress Process Revisited: Reflections on Concepts and Their Interrelationships. In Handbook of the Sociology of Mental Health; Aneshensel, C.S., Phelan, J.C., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 395–415. [Google Scholar]
  12. Turner, R.J.; Wheaton, B.; Lloyd, D.A. The Epidemiology of Social Stress. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1995, 60, 104–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Wheaton, B. The Nature of Stressors. In A Handbook for the Study of Mental Health; Horwitz, A.V., Scheid, T.L., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999; pp. 176–197. [Google Scholar]
  14. Carmack, C.L.; Boudreaux, E.; Amaral-Melendez, M.; Brantley, P.J.; De Moor, C. Aerobic fitness and leisure physical activity as moderators of the stress-illness relation. Ann. Behav. Med. 1999, 21, 251–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. da Cruz Fernandes, I.M.; Ribeiro, A.M.; Gomes, R.L.; Silva Lopes, J.S.; Vanderlei, L.C.M.; Lorençoni, M.R. Anxiety, depression and stress among employees of a public higher education institution in São Paulo, Brazil. Rev. Bras. Med. Trab. 2019, 17, 530–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Ayer, E. Everything You Need to Know about Stress; The Rosen Publishing Group, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  17. Siegrist, J.; Rödel, A. Work stress and health risk behaviour. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2006, 32, 473–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Parker, J.; Endler, N. Coping with coping assessment: A critical review. Eur. J. Pers. 1992, 6, 321–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Olff, M.; Brosschot, J.F.; Godaert, G.; Benschop, R.J.; Ballieux, R.E.; Heijnen, C.J.; de Smet, M.B.; Ursin, H. Modulatory effects of defense and coping on stress-induced changes in endocrine and immune parameters. Int. J. Behav. Med. 1995, 2, 85–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Lopuszanska-Dawid, M.; Kołodziej, H.; Lipowicz, A.; Szklarska, A. Age, Education, and Stress Affect Ageing Males’ Symptoms More than Lifestyle Does: The Wroclaw Male Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 5044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Lipowicz, A.; Szklarska, A.; Mitas, A. Biological costs of economic transition: Stress levels during the transition from Communism to Capitalism in Poland. Econ. Hum. Biol. 2016, 21, 90–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Arazi, H.; Dadvand, S.; Suzuki, K. Effects of exercise training on depression and anxiety with changing neurotransmitters in methamphetamine long term abusers: A narrative review. Biomed. Hum. Kinet. 2022, 14, 117–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Ng, D.M.; Jeffery, R.W. Relationships between Perceived Stress and Health Behaviors in a Sample of Working Adults. Health Psychol. 2003, 22, 638–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Umberson, D.; Liu, H.; Reczek, C. Stress and health behaviour over the life course. Adv. Life Course Res. 2008, 13, 19–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Kivimäki, M.; Head, J.; Ferrie, J.; Shipley, M.J.; Brunner, E.; Vahtera, J.; Marmot, M. Work Stress, Weight Gain and Weight Loss: Evidence for Bidirectional Effects of Job Strain on Body Mass Index in the Whitehall II Study. Int. J. Obes. 2006, 30, 982–987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Cooper, M.; Russell, M.; Skinner, J.; Frone, M.; Mudar, P. Stress and Alcohol Use: Moderating Effects of Gender, Coping, and Alcohol Expectancies. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1992, 101, 139–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Nichter, M.; Nichter, M.; Carkoglu, A. Reconsidering stress and smoking: A qualitative study among college students. Tob. Control 2007, 16, 211–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Alsultan, N.F.M.; Alanazi, M.D.; Boholigah, A.A.; Alshammari, D.O.; Alnahwi, K.A.; Alsayafi, Z.A.; Alzahrani, Y.M.; Nasserullah, L.Z.; Nasserullah, L.Z. The Influence of Stress on Body Mass Index among Female University Students. Egypt. J. Hosp. Med. 2018, 73, 6359–6366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Berset, M.; Semmer, N.K.; Elfering, A.; Jacobshagen, N.; Meier, L.L. Does stress at work make you gain weight? A two-year longitudinal study. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2011, 37, 45–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Ayanian, J.Z.; Block, J.P.; He, Y.; Zaslavsky, A.M.; Ding, L. Psychosocial stress and change in weight among US adults. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2009, 170, 181–192. [Google Scholar]
  31. Jankowska, E.A.; Biel, B.; Majda, J.; Szklarska, A.; Lopuszanska, M.; Medras, M.; Anker, S.D.; Banasiak, W.; Poole-Wilson, P.A.; Ponikowski, P. Anabolic deficiency in men with chronic heart failure: Prevalence and detrimental impact on survival. Circulation 2006, 114, 1829–1837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Jankowska, E.A.; Szklarska, A.; Lopuszanska, M.; Medras, M. Age and social gradients in the intensity of aging males’ symptoms in Poland. Aging Male 2008, 11, 83–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Kulej-Lyko, K.; Majda, J.; von Haehling, S.; Doehner, W.; Lopuszanska, M.; Szklarska, A.; Banasiak, W.; Anker, S.D.; Ponikowski, P.; Jankowska, E.A. Could gonadal and adrenal androgen deficiencies contribute to the depressive symptoms in men with systolic heart failure? Aging Male 2016, 19, 221–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Łopuszańska, M.; Szklarska, A.; Jankowska, E.A. Assessment of severity of depressive symptoms using the Polish version IA of Beck Depression Inventory in healthy men, inhabitants of Wrocław. Pol. Psychiatry 2013, 47, 1001–1009. [Google Scholar]
  35. Lopuszanska-Dawid, M.; Szklarska, A.; Kołodziej, H.; Lipowicz, A.; Jankowska, E.A. The relationship between: Occupational status, biological condition and androgen hormone level among Polish adult men: The Wroclaw Male Study. Aging Male 2016, 19, 231–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Szklarska, A.; Lipowicz, A.; Łopuszańska, M.; Jankowska, E.A.; Bielicki, T.; Koziel, S. Biological condition of adult migrants and non-migrants in Wroclaw, Poland. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 2008, 20, 139–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kopiczko, A. Determinants of bone health in adults Polish women: The influence of physical activity, nutrition, sun exposure and biological factors. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0238127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Łopuszańska, M.; Szklarska, A.; Lipowicz, A.; Koziel, S.; Jankowska, E.A. Life satisfaction and cardiovascular risk in Poland. Arch. Med. Sci. 2013, 4, 629–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Martin, R.; Saller, K. Lehrbuch der Anthropologie in Systematischer Darstellung mit Besonderer Berücksichtigung der Anthropologischen Methoden; G. Fisher: Stuttgart, Germany, 1957. [Google Scholar]
  40. Lipowicz, A.; Łopuszańska, M.; Kołodziej, H.; Szklarska, A.; Bielicki, T. Secular trends in BMI and the prevalence of obesity in young Polish males from 1965 to 2010. Eur. J. Public Health 2015, 25, 279–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Łopuszańska-Dawid, M.; Szklarska, A. Growth change in Polish women: Reduction of the secular trends? PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0242074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Kołodziej, H.; Łopuszańska, M.; Lipowicz, A.; Szklarska, A.; Bielicki, T. Secular trends in body height and body mass in 19-year-old Polish men based on six national surveys from 1965 to 2010. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 2015, 27, 704–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Łopuszańska, M.; Lipowicz, A.; Kołodziej, H.; Szklarska, A.; Bielicki, T. Measured versus self-reported body height and weight in Polish adult men: The risk of underestimating obesity rates. Anthropol. Anz. 2015, 72, 263–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. WHO. Obesity: Preventing and Managing the Global Epidemic; Report of a WHO Consultation; WHO Technical Report Series 894; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  45. De Hollander, E.L.; Van Zutphen, M.; Bogers, R.P.; Bemelmans, W.J.E.; De Groot, L.C. The impact of body mass index in old age on cause-specific mortality. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2012, 1, 100–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Kvamme, J.M.; Holmen, J.; Wilsgaard, T.; Florholmen, J.; Midthjell, K.; Jacobsen, B.K. Body mass index and mortality in elderly men and women: The Tromsø and UNT studies. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2012, 66, 611–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Holmes, T.H.; Rahe, R.H. The social readjustment rating scale. J. Psychosom. Res. 1967, 11, 213–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. StatSoft, Inc. STATISTICA (Data Analysis Software System); Versions 12 and 13; StatSoft, Inc.: Tulusa, OK, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  49. Ensel, W.M.; Peek, M.K.; Lin, N.; Lai, G. Stress in the life course: A life history approach. J. Aging Health 1996, 8, 389–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Turner, R.J.; Avison, W.R. Status Variations in Stress Exposure: Implications for the Interpretation of Research on Race, Socioeconomic Status, and Gender. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2003, 44, 488–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pearlin, L.I.; Aneshensel, C.S.; LeBlanc, A.J. The Forms and Mechanisms of Stress Proliferation: The Case of AIDS Caregivers. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1997, 38, 223–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Wheaton, B.; Clarke, P. Space Meets Time: Integrating Temporal and Contextual Influences on Mental Health in Early Adulthood. Am. Sociol. Rev. 2003, 68, 680–706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Guszkowska, M.; Zagórska-Pachucka, A.; Kuk, A.; Skwarek, K. Gender as a factor in differentiating strategies of coping with stress used by physical education students. Health Psychol. Rep. 2016, 4, 237–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Ross, C.E.; Wu, C.L. Education, Age, and the Cumulative Advantage in Health. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1996, 37, 104–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Roohafza, H.; Sadeghi, M.; Sarraf-Zadegan, N.; Baghaei, A.; Kelishadi, R.; Mahvash, M.; Sajjadi, F.; Toghianifar, N.; Talaei, M. Short communication: Relation between stress and other life style factors. Stress Health 2007, 23, 23–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Aldana, S.G.; Sutton, L.D.; Jacobson, B.H.; Quirk, M.G. Relationships between Leisure Time Physical Activity and Perceived Stress. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1996, 82, 315–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. de Souza Vale, R.; Guimarães, A.; Cader, S.; Wood, R.; André, H.; de Castro, J.; Dantas, E. Balance, physical conditioning, and health perception in elderly women submitted to a 32-week physical exercise program. Biomed. Hum. Kinet. 2022, 14, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Associations of stress with age and education: Part A—number of observations (n) and proportions (%) of stress categories by age and education groups and results of χ2 and p; Part B—results of the Mann–Whitney U test for stress by age and education groups.
Table 1. Associations of stress with age and education: Part A—number of observations (n) and proportions (%) of stress categories by age and education groups and results of χ2 and p; Part B—results of the Mann–Whitney U test for stress by age and education groups.
A partStress Categories (LCU)Sum
01–4950–149150+
n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)N (%)
Age (years)
 under 50 yrs20 (22.5)25 (28.1)21 (23.6)23 (25.8)89 (100)
 50–59 yrs25 (23.8)27 (25.7)39 (37.1)14 (13.3)105 (100)
 60–69 yrs43 (39.1)26 (23.6)35 (31.8)6 (5.5)110 (100)
 70 yrs and more18 (35.3)12 (23.5)15 (29.4)6 (11.8)51 (100)
All ages106 (29.9)90 (25.4)110 (31.0)49 (13.8)355 (100)
χ2, p25.011, 0.003
Educational level
 university34 (21.4)47 (29.6)51 (32.1)27 (17.0)159 (100)
 secondary school42 (31.6)32 (24.1)41 (30.8)18 (13.5)133 (100)
 vocational school30 (47.6)11 (17.5)18 (28.6)4 (6.3)63 (100)
All106 (29.9)90 (25.4)110 (31.0)49 (13.8)355 (100)
χ2, p17.244, 0.008
B partnMe (LCU)UpUpUpUp
Age (years, model I) under 50 yrs50–59 yrs60–69 yrs70 yrs+
 under 50 yrs8940--------
 50–59 yrs105510.7350.462------
 60–69 yrs110233.3990.0013.2100.001----
 70 yrs and more51391.5220.1281.0980.272−1.2870.198--
Age (years, model II) under 60 yrs60 yrs nad more
 under 60 yrs19449.5----
 60 yrs and more161253.6080.001--
Educational level universitysecondary schoolvocational school
 university15948------
 secondary school13339−1.3690.171----
 vocational school6312−3.4030.001−2.1920.028--
Legend: Stress Categories (LCU)—stress categories are cumulative LCU (Life Change Units) of stressful life events for the preceding 12-months; n—sample size with specific parameters; N—total sample size; yrs—years; χ2—chi-square test, pp-value, level of significance level; Me—median; U—Mann–Whitney U test.
Table 2. Associations of level of physical education and stress (with age and education control): Part A—number of observations (n) and proportions (%) of stress categories by age and physical activity level and results of χ2 and p; Part B—results of the Mann–Whitney U test for stress by age and physical activity level.
Table 2. Associations of level of physical education and stress (with age and education control): Part A—number of observations (n) and proportions (%) of stress categories by age and physical activity level and results of χ2 and p; Part B—results of the Mann–Whitney U test for stress by age and physical activity level.
A partStress Categories (LCU)Sum
Physical activity01–4950–149150+
n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)N (%)
Age: under 60 yrs
 inactive13 (35.1)4 (10.8)10 (27.0)10 (27.0)37 (100)
 medium10 (21.7)14 (30.4)18 (39.1)4 (8.7)46 (100)
 active22 (19.8)34 (30.6)32 (28.8)23 (20.7)111 (100)
Sum45 (23.2)52 (26.8)60 (30.9)37 (19.1)194 (100)
χ2, p12.548, 0.051
Age: 60 yrs and more
 inactive16 (59.3)2 (7.4)8 (29.6)1 (3.7)27 (100)
 medium22 (50.0)9 (20.5)9 (20.5)4 (9.1)44 (100)
 active23 (25.6)27 (30.0)33 (36.7)7 (7.8)90 (100)
Sum61 (37.9)38 (23.6)50 (31.1)12 (7.5)161 (100)
χ2, p16.512, 0.011
Education: higher levels of education
 inactive25 (46.3)6 (11.1)14 (25.9)9 (16.7)54 (100)
 medium18 (27.7)19 (29.2)22 (33.8)6 (9.2)65 (100)
 active33 (19.1)54 (31.2)56 (32.4)30 (17.3)173 (100)
Sum76 (26.0)79 (27.1)92 (31.5)45 (15.4)292 (100)
χ2, p20.871, 0.002
Education: lower levels of education
 inactive4 (40.0)0 (0.0)4 (40.0)2 (20.0)10 (100)
 medium14 (56.0)4 (16.0)5 (20.0)2 (8.0)25 (100)
 active12 (42.9)7 (25.0)9 (32.1)0 (0.0)28 (100)
Sum30 (47.6)11 (17.5)18 (28.6)4 (6.3)63 (100)
χ2, p9.357, 0.154
B partnMe (LCU)UpUpUp
Age: under 60 yrs inactivemediumactive
 inactive3774------
 medium46420.5050.614----
 active111480.0850.933−0.5000.617--
Age: 60 yrs and more --
 inactive270------
 medium445.5−0.6370.524----
 active9039−2.6570.008−2.1710.030--
Education: higher levels of education
 inactive5421.5------
 medium6540−0.7250.469----
 active17349−2.0540.040−1.4580.145--
Education: lower levels of education
 inactive1063.5------
 medium2500.9430.346----
 active28121.2410.215−0.1520.879--
Legend: Stress Categories (LCU)—stress categories are cumulative LCU (Life Change Units) of stressful life events for the preceding 12-months; n—sample size with specific parameters; N—total sample size; yrs—years; χ2—chi-square test; pp-value, level of significance level; Me—median; U—Mann–Whitney U test.
Table 3. Associations of stress with age and BMI category: Part A—number of observations (n) and proportions (%) of stress categories by age and BMI category and results of χ2 and p; Part B—results of the Mann–Whitney U test for stress by age and BMI category.
Table 3. Associations of stress with age and BMI category: Part A—number of observations (n) and proportions (%) of stress categories by age and BMI category and results of χ2 and p; Part B—results of the Mann–Whitney U test for stress by age and BMI category.
A partStress Categories (LCU)Sum
Body Mass Index01–4950–149150+
n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)N (%)
Age: under 60 yrs
 In the norm9 (20.9)8 (18.6)16 (37.2)10 (23.3)43 (100)
 Above of the norm36 (23.8)44 (29.1)44 (29.1)27 (17.9)151 (100)
Sum45 (23.2)52 (26.8)60 (30.9)37 (19.1)194 (100)
χ2, p2.720, 0.437
Age: 60 yrs and more
 In the norm11 (31.4)11 (31.4)10 (28.6)3 (8.6)35 (100)
 Above of the norm50 (39.7)27 (21.4)40 (31.7)9 (7.1)126 (100)
Sum61 (37.9)38 (23.6)50 (31.1)12 (7.5)161 (100)
χ2, p1.817, 0.611
Education: well-educated
 In the norm15 (22.1)18 (26.5)22 (32.4)13 (19.1)68 (100)
 Above of the norm61 (27.2)61 (27.2)70 (31.3)32 (14.3)224 (100)
Sum76 (26.0)79 (27.1)92 (31.5)45 (15.4)229 (100)
χ2, p1.358, 0.715
Education: poorly educated
 In the norm5 (50.0)1 (10.0)4 (40.0)0 (0.0)10 (100)
 Above of the norm25 (47.2)10 (18.9)14 (26.4)4 (7.5)53 (100)
Sum30 (47.6)11 (17.5)18 (28.6)4 (6.3)63 (100)
χ2, p1.691, 0.639
B partnMe (LCU)UpUp
Age: under 60 yrs In the normAbove of the norm
 In the norm4390
 Above of the norm151401.7630.078
Age: 60 yrs and more
 In the norm3539
 Above of the norm126230.7900.429
Education: higher levels of education
 In the norm6812.5
 Above of the norm224391.6500.099
Education: lower levels of education
 In the norm108
 Above of the norm53120.2690.788
Legend: Stress Categories (LCU)—stress categories are cumulative LCU (Life Change Units) of stressful life events for the preceding 12-months; n—sample size with specific parameters; N—total sample size; yrs—years; χ2—chi-square test; pp-value, level of significance level; Me—median; U—Mann–Whitney U test.
Table 4. Associations of stress with age and BFP category: Part A—number of observations (n) and proportions (%) of stress categories by age and Body Fat Percent category and results of χ2 and p; Part B—results of the Mann–Whitney U test for stress by age and BFP category.
Table 4. Associations of stress with age and BFP category: Part A—number of observations (n) and proportions (%) of stress categories by age and Body Fat Percent category and results of χ2 and p; Part B—results of the Mann–Whitney U test for stress by age and BFP category.
A partStress Categories (LCU)Sum
Body Fat Percentage01–4950–149150+
n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)N (%)
Age: under 60 yrs
 10–19%8 (16.7)13 (27.1)16 (33.3)11 (22.9)48 (100)
 20–29%26 (22.6)30 (26.1)36 (31.3)23 (20.0)115 (100)
 30%+11 (35.5)9 (29.0)8 (25.8)3 (9.7)31 (100)
Sum45 (23.2)52 (26.8)60 (30.9)37 (19.1)194 (100)
χ2, p5.215, 0.517
Age: 60 yrs and more
 10–19%1 (20.0)3 (60.0)1 (20.0)0 (0.0)5 (100)
 20–29%28 (33.3)22 (26.2)26 (31.0)8 (9.5)84 (100)
 30%+32 (44.4)18 (18.1)23 (31.9)4 (5.6)72 (100)
Sum61 (37.9)38 (23.6)50 (31.1)12 (7.5)161 (100)
χ2, p7.102, 0.312
Education: higher levels of education
 10–19%9 (18.8)14 (29.2)14 (29.2)11 (22.9)48 (100)
 20–29%38 (23.2)45 (27.4)53 (32.3)28 (17.1)164 (100)
 30%+29 (36.3)20 (25.0)25 (31.3)6 (7.5)80 (100)
Sum76 (26.0)79 (27.1)92 (31.5)45 (15.4)292 (100)
χ2, p10.333, 0.111
Education: lower levels of education
 10–19%0 (0)2 (40.0)3 (60.0)0 (0)5 (100)
 20–29%16 (45.7)7 (20.0)9 (25.7)3 (8.6)35 (100)
 30%+14 (60.9)2 (8.7)6 (26.1)1 (4.3)23 (100)
Sum30 (47.6)11 (17.5)18 (28.6)4 (6.3)63 (100)
χ2, p8.465, 0.206
B partnMe (LCU)UpUpUp
Age: under 60 yrs 10–19%20–29%30%+
 10–19%4888
 20–29%115531.3520.177
 30%+31252.5280.0121.7030.089
Age: 60 yrs and more
 10–19%536
 20–29%8438−0.2900.772
 30%+7221.50.0750.9401.1260.260
Education: higher levels of education
 10–19%4852
 20–29%164481.1740.240
 30%+80322.8110.0052.3930.017
Education: lower levels of education
 10–19%565
 20–29%35121.9240.054
 30%+2302.2120.0270.7280.467
Legend: Stress Categories (LCU)—stress categories are cumulative LCU (Life Change Units) of stressful life events for the preceding 12-months; n—sample size with specific parameters; N—total sample size; yrs—years; χ2—chi-square test; pp-value, level of significance level; Me—median; U—Mann–Whitney U test.
Table 5. Results of backward stepwise regression (basic and final models)—BFP as the determined variable, age, educational level, physical activity and SRRS as the determining variables.
Table 5. Results of backward stepwise regression (basic and final models)—BFP as the determined variable, age, educational level, physical activity and SRRS as the determining variables.
Basic ModelFinal Model
βpβp
age0.4130.0010.4210.001
educational level0.0760.118--
physical activity0.0430.363--
SRRS−0.1020.036−0.1100.024
R221.3020.58
Adj. R220.3920.13
F (p)
(model adjustment)
23.408 (p < 0.001)45.099 (p < 0.001)
Legend: β—Beta standardized regression coefficient; F—Ronald A. Fisher’s test; pp-value, level of significance level; R—R-values of determination; R2—R-squared values of determination; R2 adj.-the adjusted R-squared values of determination.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lopuszanska-Dawid, M.; Kupis, P.; Lipowicz, A.; Kołodziej, H.; Szklarska, A. How Stress Is Related to Age, Education, Physical Activity, Body Mass Index, and Body Fat Percentage in Adult Polish Men? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 12149. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191912149

AMA Style

Lopuszanska-Dawid M, Kupis P, Lipowicz A, Kołodziej H, Szklarska A. How Stress Is Related to Age, Education, Physical Activity, Body Mass Index, and Body Fat Percentage in Adult Polish Men? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(19):12149. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191912149

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lopuszanska-Dawid, Monika, Przemysław Kupis, Anna Lipowicz, Halina Kołodziej, and Alicja Szklarska. 2022. "How Stress Is Related to Age, Education, Physical Activity, Body Mass Index, and Body Fat Percentage in Adult Polish Men?" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 19: 12149. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191912149

APA Style

Lopuszanska-Dawid, M., Kupis, P., Lipowicz, A., Kołodziej, H., & Szklarska, A. (2022). How Stress Is Related to Age, Education, Physical Activity, Body Mass Index, and Body Fat Percentage in Adult Polish Men? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(19), 12149. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191912149

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop