The Predictive Role of Quantity and Quality Language-Exposure Measures for L1 and L2 Vocabulary Production among Immigrant Preschoolers in Italy
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Quantity and Quality of Language-Exposure Measures and Vocabulary Production
1.2. The Predictive Role of Language-Exposure Measures for Children’s Vocabulary Production
1.3. Aims of the Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedures
2.3. Tools
2.3.1. Linguistic Biography
2.3.2. The MB-CDI Short-Form Questionnaire for Bilinguals
2.4. Plan of the Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Analysis
3.2. Correlations
3.3. Regressions
4. Discussion
4.1. Children’s Vocabulary Production in L1 and Language-Exposure Measures
4.2. Children’s Vocabulary Production in L2 and Language-Exposure Measures
4.3. Strengths and Limitations of the Study
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Ministero dell’Educazione-Ufficio Statistica e studi. Gli Alunni con Cittadinanza Non Italiana A.S. 2018/2019. [Ministry of Education-Office of Statistics and Studies-Students with Non-Italian Citizenship A.S. 2019/2020.]. 2021. Available online: https://www.miur.gov.it/documents/20182/0/Alunni+con+cittadinanza+non+italiana+2019-2020.pdf/f764ef1c-f5d1-6832-3883-7ebd8e22f7f0?version=1.1&t=1633004501156 (accessed on 1 September 2021).
- Di Cataldo, R.; Roch, M. Are the effects of variation in quantity of daily bilingual exposure and socioeconomic status on language and cognitive abilities independent in preschool children? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bello, A.; Ferraresi, P.; Stefanini, S.; Perucchini, P. L1 and L2 vocabulary and word combinations of preschool children from migrant families in Italy. Int. J. Biling. 2022, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kohnert, K.; Kan, P.F.; Conboy, B.T. Lexical and grammatical associations in sequential bilingual preschoolers. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2010, 53, 684–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simon-Cereijido, G.; Méndez, L.I. Using language-specific and bilingual measures to explore lexical–grammatical links in young Latino dual-language learners. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2018, 49, 537–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Landry, S.H.; Assel, M.A.; Carlo, M.S.; Williams, J.M.; Wu, W.; Montroy, J. The effect of the Preparing Pequeños small-group cognitive instruction program on academic and concurrent social and behavioral outcomes in young Spanish-speaking dual-language learners. J. Sch. Psychol. 2019, 73, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scheffner Hammer, C.; Hoff, E.; Uchikoshi, Y.; Gillanders, C.; Castro, D.C.; Sandilos, E. The language and literacy development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Child Res. Q. 2014, 29, 715–733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cote, L.R.; Bornstein, M.H. Productive vocabulary among three groups of bilingual American children: Comparison and prediction. First Lang. 2014, 34, 467–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hoff, E.; Rumiche, R.; Burridge, A.; Ribot, K.M.; Welsh, S.N. Expressive vocabulary development in children from bilingual and monolingual homes: A longitudinal study from two to four years. Early Child Res. Q. 2014, 29, 433–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bonifacci, P.; Barbieri, M.; Tomassini, M.; Roch, M. In few words: Linguistic gap but adequate narrative structure in preschool bilingual children. J. Child Lang. 2018, 45, 120–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leseman, P.P. Bilingual vocabulary development of Turkish preschoolers in the Netherlands. J. Multiling. Multicult. Dev. 2000, 21, 93–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kan, P.F.; Kohnert, K. Preschoolers learning Hmong and English: Lexical-semantic skills in L1 and L2. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2005, 48, 372–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ertanir, B.; Kratzmann, J.; Frank, M.; Jahreiss, S.; Sachse, S. Dual language competencies of Turkish–German children growing up in Germany: Factors supportive of functioning dual language development. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bronfenbrenner, U.; Morris, P.A. The ecology of developmental processes. In Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 1: Theoretical Models of Human Development; Damon, W., Lerner, R.M., Eds.; John and Wiley and Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1998; pp. 993–1028. [Google Scholar]
- Hoff, E. How social contexts support and shape language development. Dev. Rev. 2006, 26, 55–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, J.; Grüter, T. Introduction to Input and experience in bilingual development. In Trends in Language Acquisition Research: Input and Experience in Bilingual Development; Paradis, J., Grüter, T., Eds.; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014; pp. 1–14. [Google Scholar]
- Pham, G.; Tipton, T. Internal and external factors that support children’s minority first language and English. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2018, 49, 595–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ribot, K.M.; Hoff, E.; Burridge, A. Language use contributes to expressive language growth: Evidence from bilingual children. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 929–940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quiroz, B.G.; Snow, C.E.; Zhao, J. Vocabulary skills of Spanish—English bilinguals: Impact of mother—Child language interactions and home language and literacy support. Int. J. Biling. 2010, 14, 379–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, J.; Jia, R. Bilingual children’s long-term outcomes in English as a second language: Language environment factors shape individual differences in catching up with monolinguals. Dev. Sci. 2017, 20, e12433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sorenson Duncan, T.; Paradis, J. How does maternal education influence the linguistic environment supporting bilingual language development in child second language learners of English? Int. J. Biling. 2020, 24, 46–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rydland, V.; Grøver, V. Language use, home literacy environment, and demography: Predicting vocabulary skills among diverse young dual language learners in Norway. J. Child Lang. 2021, 48, 717–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Florit, E.; Barachetti, C.; Majorano, M.; Lavelli, M. Home Language Activities and Expressive Vocabulary of Toddlers from Low-SES Monolingual Families and Bilingual Immigrant Families. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 3, 296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prevoo, M.J.; Malda, M.; Mesman, J.; Emmen, R.A.; Yeniad, N.; Van Ijzendoorn, M.H.; Linting, M. Predicting ethnic minority children’s vocabulary from socioeconomic status, maternal language and home reading input: Different pathways for host and ethnic language. J. Child Lang. 2014, 41, 963–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gatt, D.; Baldacchino, R.; Dodd, B. Which measure of socioeconomic status best predicts bilingual lexical abilities and how? A focus on four-year-old exposed to two majority languages. J. Child Lang. 2020, 47, 737–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meir, N.; Armon-Lotem, S. Independent and combined effects of socioeconomic status (SES) and bilingualism on children’s vocabulary and verbal short-term memory. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Onofrio, D.; Rinaldi, P.; Pettenati, P. Il primo sviluppo del linguaggio in bambini che imparano più lingue: Una proposta per la valutazione e l’interpretazione del profilo linguistico. Psicol. Clin. Dello Svilupp. 2012, 16, 661–670. [Google Scholar]
- Rinaldi, P.; Anghinoni, E.; La Marchina, E.; Pettenati, P.; Remi, L.; Simonelli, A.; Stefanini, S.; Pasqualetti, P.; Caselli, M.C. Identification of bilingual toddler at risk in migrant families: Lexical profile and environmental factors. in preparation.
- O’Toole, C.; Gatt, D.; Hickey, T.M.; Miękisz, A.; Haman, E.; Armon-Lotem, S.; Rinker, T.; Ohana, O.; dos Santos, C.; Kern, S. Parent report of early lexical production in bilingual children across varied contexts: A CDI study. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 2017, 20, 124–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradis, J.; Genesee, F.; Crago, M.B. Dual Language Development & Disorder; Paul H. Brookes: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Chondrogianni, V.; Marinis, T. Differential effects of internal and external factors on the development of vocabulary, tense morphology and morpho-syntax in successive bilingual children. Linguist. Approaches Biling. 2011, 3, 318–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arndt, H.L.; Granfeldt, J.; Gullberg, M. Reviewing the potential of the Experience Sampling Method (ESM) for capturing second language exposure and use. Second Lang. Res. 2021, 39, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Linguistic Outcome Variables | M (%) | SD | Range |
---|---|---|---|
Children’s vocabulary production in L1 | 37.9 | 28.3 | 0–100 |
Children’s vocabulary production in L2 | 85.6 | 25.7 | 0–100 |
Predictor variables | |||
Demographic | |||
Child’s chronological age (months) | 49.92 | 7.10 | 37–67 |
Mother’s education (years) | 12.27 | 3.63 | 4–18 |
Father’s education (years) | 11.82 | 3.45 | 5–18 |
Quantity of language exposure | |||
L1 parental input | 2.50 | 0.54 | 1–3 |
L1 child output | 2.12 | 0.67 | 1–3 |
L2 parental input | 2.47 | 0.52 | 1–3 |
L2 child output | 2.83 | 0.42 | 1–3 |
Length of L2 exposure at preschool (months) | 28.20 | 13.43 | 6–53 |
Quality of language exposure | |||
L1 HLE-active | 1.86 | 0.84 | 1–4 |
L1 HLE-play | 1.60 | 1.08 | 1–4 |
L1 HLE-passive | 1.95 | 1.25 | 1–4 |
L2 HLE-active | 2.10 | 0.91 | 1–4 |
L2 HLE-play | 3.08 | 1.03 | 1–4 |
L2 HLE-passive | 3.83 | 0.53 | 1–4 |
Correlations | Children’s Vocabulary Production in L1 | Children’s Vocabulary Production in L2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
rs | p | rs | p | |
Child’s chronological age | 0.159 | 0.226 | 0.221 | 0.098 |
Mother’s education | 0.172 | 0.189 | 0.168 | 0.212 |
Father’s education | 0.000 | 0.999 | 0.172 | 0.189 |
L1 parental input | 0.329 | 0.010 | −0.174 | 0.195 |
L1 child output | 0.513 | 0.001 | −0.119 | 0.379 |
L2 parental input | −0.438 | 0.001 | 0.245 | 0.066 |
L2 child output | −0.388 | 0.002 | 0.357 | 0.006 |
Length of L2 exposure at preschool | 0.142 | 0.277 | 0.512 | <0.001 |
Parental L2oral fluency | 0.162 | 0.216 | 0.155 | 0.250 |
L1 HLE-active | 0.490 | <0.001 | −0.033 | 0.808 |
L1 HLE-play | 0.394 | <0.002 | −0.142 | 0.294 |
L1 HLE-passive | 0.254 | 0.050 | −0.120 | 0.376 |
L2 HLE-active | −0.026 | 0.843 | 0.357 | 0.006 |
L2 HLE-play | 0.157 | 0.230 | 0.374 | 0.004 |
L2 HLE-passive | −0.191 | 0.145 | 0.041 | 0.763 |
Children’s Vocabulary Production in L1 | |||||||||
Step 1 | Step 2 | Step 3 | |||||||
β | t | p | β | t | p | β | t | p | |
Child’s chronological age | 0.095 | 0.684 | 0.497 | −0.047 | −0.320 | 0.752 | −0.065 | −0.451 | 0.654 |
Mother’s education | 0.185 | 1.014 | 0.315 | 0.282 | 1.606 | 0.102 | 0.197 | 1.198 | 0.237 |
Father’s education | −0.049 | −0.300 | 0.765 | −0.049 | −0.300 | 0.765 | −0.091 | −0.587 | 0.560 |
F(3, 52) = 6.15, p = 0.608; R2 = 0.034 | |||||||||
Length of L2 exposure at preschool | 0.120 | 0.818 | 0.417 | 0.222 | 1.497 | 0.141 | |||
L1 parental input | 0.033 | 0.212 | 0.833 | −0.077 | −0.488 | 0.628 | |||
L2 parental input | −0.551 | −0.3.291 | 0.002 | −0.332 | 1.855 | 0.070 | |||
Parental L2 oral fluency | 0.045 | 0.324 | 0.748 | 0.059 | 0.446 | 0.658 | |||
F(4, 48) = 4.530, p = 0.003, R2 = 0.299 | |||||||||
L1 child output | 0.308 | 1.855 | 0.070 | ||||||
L2 child output | −0.205 | −0.1286 | 0.205 | ||||||
F(2, 46) = 3.878, p = 0.028, R2 = 0.400 | |||||||||
Children’s Vocabulary Production in L2 | |||||||||
Step 1 | Step 2 | Step 3 | |||||||
β | t | p | β | t | p | β | t | p | |
Child’s chronological age | 0.139 | 0.982 | 0.331 | −0.023 | −0.151 | 0.881 | 0.092 | 0.608 | 0.546 |
Mother’s education | 0.176 | 0.978 | 0.333 | 0.024 | 0.134 | 0.894 | 0.061 | 0.351 | 0.727 |
Father’s education | 0.044 | 0.244 | 0.808 | −0.015 | −0.091 | 0.928 | −0.009 | −0.057 | 0.955 |
F(3, 49) = 1.175, p = 0.329; R2 = 0.067 | |||||||||
Length of L2 exposure at preschool | 0.390 | 2.475 | 0.017 | 0.206 | 1.260 | 0.214 | |||
Parental input in L1 | 0.104 | 0.640 | 0.525 | 0.072 | 0.435 | 0.666 | |||
Parental input in L2 | 0.227 | 1.313 | 0.196 | 0.027 | 0.150 | 0.882 | |||
Parental L2 oral fluency | 0.130 | 0.846 | 0.402 | 0.206 | 1.260 | 0.214 | |||
F(4, 45) = 2.917, p = 0.031; R2 = 0.259 | |||||||||
Child output in L1 | 0.067 | 0.384 | 0.703 | ||||||
Child output in L2 | 0.454 | 2.724 | 0.009 | ||||||
F(2, 43) = 3.822, p = 0.030; R2 = 0.371 |
Children’s Vocabulary Production in L1 | |||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | Step 2 | Step 3 HLE-active | Step 3 HLE-play | Step 3 HLE-passive | |||||||||||
β | t | p | β | t | p | β | t | p | β | t | p | β | t | p | |
Child’s chronological age | 0.095 | 0.684 | 0.457 | 0.057 | 0.476 | 0.636 | 0.073 | 0.625 | 0.535 | 0.055 | 0.459 | 0.648 | 0.070 | 0.567 | 0.573 |
Mother’s education | 0.185 | 1.014 | 0.315 | 0.227 | 1.421 | 0.161 | 0.084 | 0.501 | 0.619 | 0.178 | 1.102 | 0.276 | 0.235 | 1.458 | 0.151 |
Father’s education | −0.096 | −0.529 | 0.599 | −0.070 | −0.455 | 0.651 | −0.123 | −0.822 | 0.415 | −0.086 | −0.571 | 0.570 | −0.089 | −0.550 | 0.585 |
F(3, 52) = 6.15, p = 0.608, R2 = 0.034 | |||||||||||||||
L2 parental input | −0.343 | −2.292 | 0.026 | −0.202 | −1.297 | 0.201 | −0.379 | −0.2377 | 0.021 | −0.304 | −1.920 | 0.061 | |||
L1 child output | 0.311 | 2.181 | 0.034 | 0.272 | 1.156 | 0.056 | 0.253 | 1.715 | 0.093 | 0.299 | 2.073 | 0.044 | |||
F(2, 50) = 12.129, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.350 | |||||||||||||||
L1 HLE-active | 0.361 | 2.437 | 0.019 | ||||||||||||
L2 HLE-active | −0.074 | −0.604 | 0.548 | ||||||||||||
F(2, 48) = 2.983, p = 0.060, R2 = 0.422 | |||||||||||||||
L1 HLE-play | 0.136 | 0.958 | 0.343 | ||||||||||||
L2 HLE-play | 0.225 | 1.827 | 0.074 | ||||||||||||
F(2, 48) = 1.989, p = 0.148, R2 = 0.399 | |||||||||||||||
L1 HLE-passive | 0.136 | 1.079 | 0.286 | ||||||||||||
L2 HLE-passive | 0.024 | 0.189 | 0.851 | ||||||||||||
F(4, 48) = 3.945, p = 0.586, R2 = 0.365 |
Children’s Vocabulary Production in L2 | |||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | Step 2 | Step 3 HLE-active | Step 3 HLE-play | Step 3 HLE-passive | |||||||||||
β | t | p | β | t | p | β | t | p | β | t | p | β | t | p | |
Child’s chronological age | 0.139 | 0.982 | 0.331 | 0.192 | 1.576 | 0.122 | 0.238 | 2.134 | 0.038 | ||||||
Mother’s education | 0.176 | 0.978 | 0.333 | 0.159 | 1.036 | 0.306 | 0.004 | 0.024 | 0.981 | ||||||
Father’s education | 0.044 | 0.244 | 0.808 | 0.016 | 0.103 | 0.919 | 0.019 | 0.134 | 0.894 | ||||||
F(3, 49) = 1.175 p = 0.329; R2 = 0.067 | |||||||||||||||
L2 child output | 0.518 | 4.349 | <0.001 | 0.555 | 4.609 | <0.001 | 0.479 | 3.343 | 0.002 | 0.533 | 4.348 | <0.001 | |||
F(1, 48) = 18.912, p < 0.001 R2 = 0.331 | |||||||||||||||
L1 HLE-active | 0.128 | 0.922 | 0.361 | ||||||||||||
L2 HLE-active | 0.341 | 2.886 | 0.006 | ||||||||||||
F(2, 46) = 6.039, p = 0.005,R2 = 0.470 | |||||||||||||||
L1 HLE-play | 0.060 | 0.420 | 0.677 | ||||||||||||
L2 HLE-play | 0.189 | 1.444 | 0.155 | ||||||||||||
F(2, 46) =1.086, p = 0.346 R2 = 0.361 | |||||||||||||||
L1 HLE-passive | 0.036 | 0.291 | 0.773 | ||||||||||||
L2 HLE-passive | 0.141 | 1.087 | 0.282 | ||||||||||||
F(2, 46) = 0.597, p = 0.555, R2 = 0.348 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bello, A.; Ferraresi, P.; Caselli, M.C.; Perucchini, P. The Predictive Role of Quantity and Quality Language-Exposure Measures for L1 and L2 Vocabulary Production among Immigrant Preschoolers in Italy. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 1966. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20031966
Bello A, Ferraresi P, Caselli MC, Perucchini P. The Predictive Role of Quantity and Quality Language-Exposure Measures for L1 and L2 Vocabulary Production among Immigrant Preschoolers in Italy. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(3):1966. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20031966
Chicago/Turabian StyleBello, Arianna, Paola Ferraresi, Maria Cristina Caselli, and Paola Perucchini. 2023. "The Predictive Role of Quantity and Quality Language-Exposure Measures for L1 and L2 Vocabulary Production among Immigrant Preschoolers in Italy" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 3: 1966. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20031966
APA StyleBello, A., Ferraresi, P., Caselli, M. C., & Perucchini, P. (2023). The Predictive Role of Quantity and Quality Language-Exposure Measures for L1 and L2 Vocabulary Production among Immigrant Preschoolers in Italy. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(3), 1966. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20031966