Next Article in Journal
Efficiency of Harvester with the Debarking Head at Logging in Spruce Stands Affected by Bark Beetle Outbreak
Next Article in Special Issue
Winter Roosting by Eastern Red Bats in Ozark Mountain Forests of Missouri
Previous Article in Journal
Tree Regeneration by Seeds in Natural Forests
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Eastern Red Bat Responses to Fire during Winter Torpor

1
Department of Biology, Missouri State University, 901 S, National Avenue, Springfield, MO 65897, USA
2
Stantec Consulting Services Inc., 6800 College Boulevard, Suite 750, Overland Park, KS 66211, USA
3
Department of Biology, Laboratory Building LB109, University of Regina, 3737 Wascana Parkway, Regina, SK S4S 0A2, Canada
4
U.S. Department of Agriculture Wildlife Services, 140 Locust Grove Rd, Pittstown, NJ 08867, USA
5
Environmental Solutions & Innovations Inc., 3851 S Jefferson Avenue, Springfield, MO 65807, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Forests 2021, 12(10), 1347; https://doi.org/10.3390/f12101347
Submission received: 25 August 2021 / Revised: 17 September 2021 / Accepted: 26 September 2021 / Published: 2 October 2021

Abstract

:
Prescribed fires are a forest management tool used to improve natural areas for a variety of benefits including increased plant diversity, reduced competition for desired species, decreased fuel loads, and improved wildlife habitat. The post-fire results in landscapes have shown positive benefits for bat populations. However, prescribed fires set in the winter may cause direct mortality of eastern red bat (Lasiurus borealis) populations that use leaf litter for roosting during periods of colder (<10 °C) temperatures. Therefore, we used controlled laboratory techniques to explore if eastern red bats arouse from torpor when exposed to cues associated with fire (i.e., smoke and the sound of fire). Through subsequent field trials, we confirmed latencies of first response (i.e., movement or increased respiration), arousal, and flight behaviors to the stimuli of fire. We provide evidence of smoke influencing eastern red bat first response and arousal through laboratory and field trial results. Latencies of all behaviors were negatively correlated with temperatures and wind speeds prior to and during field trials. We recommend prescribing winter fires on days when temperatures are >10 °C to provide eastern red bats with a better chance to passively rewarm and react to an approaching fire.

1. Introduction

Forests across North America are managed using a variety of different silvicultural strategies, including clear cutting, tree thinning, and prescribed fire [1]. Forest landscapes in the eastern U.S. have relied on fires for shaping their natural history, and several species evolved in response to direct and indirect effects from fire. The silvicultural practice of prescribed burning reduces the conditions leading to severe wildfires and improves overall forest productivity [2]. However, improper use of prescribed burning can have detrimental effects such as crown scorch in Pinus species (e.g., loblolly (P. taeda), slash (P. elliottii), and longleaf (P. palustris)) and can also remove nutrients via high intensity burns and sediment loss through increased erosion exposure to rainfall [3,4]. Research involving “burn vs. non-burn” treatments demonstrated an increase in diversity over time of both plants and animals [5,6]. While prescribed fires may be overall beneficial for the plant and animal community, they may induce energetically costly behaviors from wildlife to avoid an active fire, or death.
‘Escape responses’ describe the behaviors of wildlife to avoid potential threats, including examples of wildlife avoiding fire [7,8]. Using sensory systems and basic survival instincts, many animals demonstrate a realization of immediate danger and avoid it altogether [9,10,11]. Animals are particularly susceptible to fire when they are impaired or non-mobile, as with altricial young (i.e., mouse pups, bird nestlings). Winter habits of bats are not normally considered regarding winter fire management in forests, but eastern red bats (Lasiurus borealis) have attracted interest because of their unique habit of roosting under the leaf litter and utilizing torpor [12,13,14,15,16,17,18]. Considering torpor is a state of reduction in activity and metabolism, eastern red bats under the leaf litter are at a unique risk of experiencing the adverse effects of natural or prescribed fires.
Unlike many North American bat species that colonially roost in caves and manmade structures (e.g., barns, houses, and mines), eastern red bats roost solitarily within the foliage in forests and small patches of trees throughout the year [19]. During the warmer months, eastern red bats are found throughout the eastern and central United States, but in winter are common south of the Ohio and Missouri River Valleys, predominantly in the states surrounding the Gulf of Mexico [20]. The winter roosting behavior of eastern red bats is thermally dependent, and when temperatures approach or go below 0 °C, they abandon tree roosts in favor of leaf litter on south-facing slopes, which provides a more suitable and stable microclimate [21,22,23,24,25,26]. They remain under the leaf litter until ambient temperatures (Ta) return to approximately 10 °C [23,26].
Due to the prevalence of prescribed fires performed by land managers in late fall through early spring, eastern red bats are thought to be a species that may be harmed because of their leaf roosting habits [25]. Historically, fires have shaped the landscape during the summer months [27] when eastern red bats are not known to exhibit torpor under leaf litter. Prescribed fires often occur during the months when eastern red bats are torpid in the leaf litter or on leaves or branches of trees [21]. Low-intensity summer fires can have positive effects on bats [28,29], but it is less clear how fires affect bats in winter. While the U.S. Department of Agriculture (USDA) issues state recommendations for a temperature range for setting prescribed fires of 4–21 °C for controlling woody vegetation, it is unclear how bats under leaf litter will respond [30]. Considering that eastern red bats are experiencing population declines and are a species of conservation concern [31], it is important to understand if management practices have the potential to cause mortality or detrimental energy consumption in levels harmful to local area populations. Therefore, measurement of eastern red bat ‘escape responses’ to fire conditions at varying weather conditions can inform best management practices.
The ability for bats to detect fire during torpor has been documented [11]. Gould’s long-eared bat (Nyctophilus gouldi) detected smoke in a lab setting during deep torpor and initiated an arousal response [11]. While torpid bats have reduced activity because of lower metabolism, we considered hearing and smell to be the primary senses available to alert eastern red bats to the presence of fire. Eastern red bats have an acute sense of hearing used for echolocation, mainly in the range between 35 and 56 kHz [32] and capable of responding to noises audible to humans (0.02 to 20 kHz, personal observations). Greater mouse-eared bats (Myotis myotis) exhibit variable responses to audio-recorded stimuli during shallow torpor through the maternity season [33]. Other species, such as little brown bats (Myotis lucifugus), exhibited a decrease in sensitivity to high frequency sounds with decreasing body temperature [34]. In addition, higher frequency sounds attenuate (lose strength) as a function of distance [35,36]. Olfactory communication is used in other species of bats for kin-recognition, as territorial markers, determine reproductive status, and detecting food resources [37,38].
With the prevalence of prescribed fires during the winter in areas where eastern red bats occur, the importance of determining physical limitations of behavioral responses to the threat of fire based on ambient weather conditions to assist with improving proper utilization of the forest management tool. Our objective was to determine (1) whether environmental cues associated with fire (i.e., sound, smoke) cause eastern red bats to arouse from torpor and (2) how the behavior (i.e., first response, arousal, flight) of torpid bats is influenced by stimuli associated with fire under controlled-laboratory and experimental field conditions. We provide insight into the sensory abilities of eastern red bats, particularly while torpid, and guidelines for fire management as it relates to reducing direct negative impacts to the species.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Site

We obtained eastern red bats during the winter months between November through March of 2005–2006 and 2007–2008 in Carter County, Missouri at Peck Ranch Conservation Area (PRCA), a Missouri Department of Conservation managed property. PRCA had a hilly topography and a mixture of pine (Pinus spp.) and hardwood forests managed with prescribed fires implemented during the winter and early spring to enhance woodland and glade communities by suppressing invading understory plants and reducing litter depths [39]. Bats were active at PRCA during the winter when daily temperatures were above 10 °C [26], and were documented to successfully forage during the winter months in southern Missouri [18].

2.2. Animal Care

Protocols followed guidelines for animal use in research and were approved by the Missouri State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol # 2009A). We captured eastern red bats for lab trials and transported them to the Missouri State University (Springfield, MO, USA) physiology lab during the winter of 2005–2006. We kept bats in plastic cages (31 × 19 × 17 cm3) with a Styrofoam substrate (29 × 17 × 2 cm3) covered with leaves. Mealworms and water were provided ad libitum. Bats were kept at room temperature (20–25 °C) when they were not being tested and were allowed to fly in a confined space (10.8 × 7.5 × 3 m3) once a week. We released bats at the point of capture after they completed the sequence of trials.
We captured and transported bats to the field lab located in PRCA on the night of capture for field trials performed in 2007–2008. Bats were subjected to trials the following morning, during/after which they were allowed to fly away.

2.3. Laboratory

To determine if torpid eastern red bats would react and arouse from torpor when exposed to smoke, sound, and/or combination of both, each bat was subjected to five laboratory trials. Trials consisted of (1) smoke only, (2) audio recording of fire, (3) combination smoke and audio recording of fire, (4) control air, and (5) control audio recording. Bats were randomly assigned 1 of 5 different trial treatment sequences to avoid any order-effects (i.e., 12345, 54132, 45123, 34251, and 23514). A bat was allowed a minimum of 48 h between trial treatments.
Bats were placed in a test cage that consisted of a plastic base and metal wires for structure and internal plastic mesh that bats could climb and perch. The cage floor was covered with oak (Quercus sp.) leaves. We placed cages into an environmental chamber to acclimate the bat to the reduced temperature 24 h prior to a trial. Ta was monitored with a digital probe thermometer (RadioShack, Boston, MA, USA) and maintained at 5 ± 0.5 °C throughout all trials. Eastern red bats were assumed to be in a state of “optimum” torpor when Ta was 5 ± 0.5 °C, since the frequency of arousals was less at this parameter when metabolic rates were compared to lower/higher Ta [16]. Glass-front environmental chambers (n = 2; Avanti model WC492D 47 × 43 × 83 cm3) were used for trials. One was used for the control air and sound treatments, and another was used for the smoke and smoke/sound combination trials; this protocol insured that one chamber was not contaminated with residual smoke. Each environmental chamber was constructed to facilitate the movement of air/smoke for the bat arousal trials. We drilled one 4 cm opening into each side of the environmental chambers. On the left side, the opening was centered 17 cm from the top, and on the right side, it was centered 7 cm from the bottom to allow air flow throughout the chambers.
We timed each trial from the onset of an introduction of a stimulus/control to time of first response and arousal as observed by a witness. First response in a trial bat was defined as any movement or increased respiration. Arousal was defined as movement from one location to another (>0.5 cm). To control for the base rate for arousal, we used 60 min as the time limit for all fire stimuli arousal trials based on physical disturbance tests [17]. We terminated a trial after the bat aroused or the 60 min time limit was reached.
We placed dead oak leaves (approx. 8 g) inside a metal can (3.2 L) with a metal lid outfitted with two openings for smoke trials. A 2.5 cm diameter plastic tube approximately 1 m in length connected one lid opening to the left opening of the environmental chamber (Figure 1). A hand pump was connected to the other opening in the metal lid, which forced air through the can and pushed smoke through the tube into the environmental chamber. We connected a second tube (same dimensions) on the right side of the environmental chamber to a separate hand pump. At the beginning of each trial, we ignited a fire inside the can, closed the lid and pumped smoke into the test chamber. After 30 s, we closed off the smoke input tube and pumped from the right side of the environmental chamber to remove smoke. We repeated the process every 5 min until trial termination conditions were met. The introduction of smoke/air into the environmental chambers did not influence temperature, as observed via the digital thermometer.
We audio recorded a fire of dead oak leaves and twigs (approximately 10 × 20 cm2) with a power module microphone (Audio-technica, Tokyo, Japan) and duplicated the soundtrack 5 times. Each track was off-set to one another and then joined together into one track, resulting in a final track (the recording of multiple tracks together). We played that recording using a 16-bit mono sound through a speaker that was able to emit sounds of up to 20 kHz (Altec Lansing Multimedia model ACS41) inside the test chamber positioned 17 cm away from a bat until termination conditions were met.
To act as the control for smoke, we used a different hand pump and tubing system (Figure 1) to force air (approximately 1200 mL) into and out of the environmental chamber every 5 min. To act as a control for the sound of fire, we generated white noise using the Cool Edit Pro Version 2.00 software (Syntrillium Software Corp., Scottsdale, AZ, USA). We set the white noise to play for 60 min using the same procedure as in the audio fire sound test.

2.4. Field

We cleared and leveled an area (2.5 × 1.5 m2) in a field at PRCA for our field trials. We bordered the area with plastic edging, spread sand over the bare dirt, and used leaf litter to emulate forest ground cover. We placed three circular cone cages (approximately 80 cm in height) 0.2 m apart in a line, held in place by stakes. Outside of the arena, at a distance of approximately 3 m, we positioned two troughs (2.5 × 1.2 × 0.25 m3; 2 × 1 × 0.25 m3) composed of expanded metal and square metal tubing (Figure 2). During trials, we filled the troughs with leaves and covered them with a sheet of expanded metal to contain flaming debris.
An onsite weather logger (Kestrel 4500 Pocket Weather Tracker) placed on a 1.5 m wind vane within 10 m of the burn arena recorded Ta, wind speed/direction, and relative humidity data. We separated weather data for each trial into 3 time periods: (1) 2200 at night of capture through one min before civil sunrise, (2) civil sunrise through one min before the onset of the trial, and (3) onset of the trial until termination.
We monitored and recorded bats with closed-circuit surveillance cameras connected to a video monitor and a recorder stationed 10 m from the fire. Prior to each field trial, roosting positions were classified as either ‘perched’ (hanging from the cage) or ‘prone’ (underneath leaf litter). Perched bats were left undisturbed, with video cameras placed outside of the cages to record their movements. We removed cages over the prone bats and located bats by carefully removing leaf litter to allow for proper camera placement (i.e., head visible).
We used a drip torch to ignite the trough of leaves, emulating a heading fire (flames and smoke going toward the fuel being consumed, and thus toward the bat(s)). Our method provided the greatest amount of smoke exposure going toward a focal bat, and we maintained the fire until the end of the trial. If the wind direction changed during a trial, we ignited the second trough to maintain smoke going in the direction of the bat(s), but only after the first trough’s fire had stopped burning. We defined first response and arousal in prone bats during field trials as we did during laboratory trials. We recorded the latencies to first response and arousal behaviors to the onset of the fire. When we observed a behavior on a recording, the time was marked and subtracted from the onset of the fire to produce latency durations. Additionally, we measured latency to flight, herein defined as the time from the onset of the fire until a bat flew away. Because bats regularly aroused and crawled out of the video frame, we recorded flight latency times by visual observations of bats flying from the ground (Figure 3). We did not consider perched bats to be in deep states of torpor based on field observations (e.g., head movement during camera setup, etc.), so the first response latencies were not measured. However, we defined arousal in perched bats as any visible head movement or wing movement. Flight response was defined as when wings were unfolded, since bats were still in cages and could not freely fly away. We would carefully tip cages onto their side after a bat was observed to exhibit a flight response to allow free movement away from the burn arena. Trials were terminated after all bats had flown or 60 min after the fire was started.

2.5. Smoke Analyses

We used a carbon monoxide (CO) monitor with two sensors (Sixth Sense, Inc. (Roma, Italy) Eco-Sense 2e electrochemical sensors with a custom electronics signal conditioning board) that sampled every 10 s to compare our results to actual prescribed fires, and to measure for differences between the laboratory and field trial smoke (e.g., Dickinson et al., 2010). For the lab trials, we placed sensors in the same position as bats to replicate the conditions an individual would experience and ran trials to measure CO concentrations. In the field, we placed sensors on the ground between bat cages.

2.6. Analyses

For our laboratory study, we used the Friedman nonparametric test to compare arousal times for the five treatments. Following a significant Friedman’s test, Wilcoxon signed rank tests were used for pairwise comparisons. We used α ≤ 0.05 for indicating significance in all statistical tests.
For our field study, latency values of all three observed behaviors were viewed against each separate time periods weather data and examined for associations among variables. We used Pearson’s product moment correlations (r) to ascertain the strength of relationships. We used α ≤ 0.05 for indicating significance in correlation values. We completed all analyses through Minitab statistical software (Minitab Inc., State College, PA, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Laboratory

We tested 15 male eastern red bats in optimum torpor at Ta 5 ± 0.5 °C for first response and arousal behaviors to fire stimuli and controls through randomized laboratory behavioral trial sequences. Each bat was tested individually through all 5 exposure trials, totaling 75 trials.

3.1.1. Sound Response

No bats responded to the control audio recording. When exposed to the sound of fire, only one individual elicited a first response at 35 min, and we observed no arousals.

3.1.2. Smoke Response

No bats elicited any response during control air trials. In contrast, all bats exposed to fire generated smoke had a first response, with response times varying from 4 s to 5.5 min (Figure 4). Arousal times (n = 10) ranged from 11 to 40.5 min (mean ± SE = 36.55 ± 4.98), with five bats not responding within the 60 min time allotment (Figure 5).

3.1.3. Fire Sound and Smoke Response

All bats had a relatively rapid first response between 4 and 30 s when exposed to both the sound of fire and smoke simultaneously (Figure 4). Arousal times ranged from 10.5 to 42 min (mean ± SE = 21.92 ± 1.87) (Figure 5).

3.1.4. Statistical Comparisons

The Friedman nonparametric test indicated a difference in latency to arouse among the five treatments (S = 48.7, p < 0.05) (Figure 5). The sound of fire had no effect on arousal times when compared to the sound control (Wilcoxon: z = 3.4, p = 1). In pairwise comparisons, arousal latencies were faster for the smoke versus smoke control (Wilcoxon: z = 2.8, p < 0.05). The combination of fire sound and smoke had shorter arousal latencies when compared to smoke alone (Wilcoxon: z = −2.5, p < 0.05).

3.2. Field

We placed 27 eastern red bats (26 males, 1 female) within the field trial arena. We observed bat behavioral responses to fire stimuli through 10 separate field trials, ranging from 1 to 3 bats per trial. Out of the male bats, 22 were in the prone position, and 5 were in the perched position on the side of the cage. Two prone males flew during the setup of cameras prior to fire ignition and were not considered to be in torpor. Due to technical difficulties with recording equipment, two trials were not recorded for first response or arousal (first trial (12/2/07) and sixth trial (3/1/08)), resulting in five bats (four prone, one perched) not being observed for these behaviors.
Of the 15 prone bats monitored for first response and arousal, 13 exhibited a first response during trials. First responses occurred as soon as 51 s after the ignition of a fire to as late as 40 min after, with an average of 12.5 min (SE ± 2.49) for all individuals that exhibited the behavior. Arousals occurred in 12 individuals, occurring as soon as 3 min 42 s after ignition to 29 min 20 s, with an average of 17.5 min (SE ± 2.18) for all individuals that exhibited the behavior.
Since flight was observed manually, all bats tested (n = 25) were monitored for flight response. Of those, 15 prone individuals (68.2%) exhibited a flight response ranging from as soon as 10 min after ignition to 46 min, with an average of 21.8 min (SE ± 4.20) for all prone bats that exhibited the behavior.
We did not monitor perched bats for first response due to observed shallower states of torpor. Of the five perched bats, only four were video recorded for arousal. Perched bats exhibited arousal behavior between 45 s and 7 min 9 s, with an average of 3.8 min (SE ± 1.37) after fire ignition. Flight occurred from 3 to 26 min, with an average flight latency of 14.4 min (SE ± 4.20).

3.2.1. Smoke Response

Latency to first response, arousal, and flight response were significantly negatively correlated with temperature during all three time periods (Table 1). First response and arousal latency at time periods 2 and 3 (prior to field trials, during field trials, respectively) were significantly negatively correlated with wind speed, whereas flight response latency was significantly negatively correlated with wind speed during all three time periods. The only significant association between relative humidity and any behavioral response (first) was observed in time period 3 (Table 1).

3.2.2. Smoke Chemical Concentration

The concentration of CO measured in the environmental chambers fluctuated among trials (n = 3). The average amount of CO was 141.17 (SE ± 4.29), 103.20 (SE ± 3.41), and 60.66 (SE ± 1.54) parts per million (ppm). Maximum exposure at each trial was 375, 291, and 184 ppm, respectively. During the one field trial where CO was monitored, it averaged 1.51 ppm (SE ± 0.14), with a maximum exposure of 40 ppm.

4. Discussion

We determined that eastern red bats are able to arouse from optimum torpor bouts when exposed to cues associated with fire (e.g., smoke and sounds). Eastern red bats are capable of arousing from torpor when exposed to smoke and the additional sound of fire decreases the response time. For our second objective, we described eastern red bats exhibiting decreased latency responses (i.e., first response, arousal, and flight) to increased Ta and wind speeds during the simulated prescribed fire stimuli. Although literature references on escape responses of torpid mammals are limited, there are a variety of species that are capable of this behavior [11,40,41], including other species of bat [11,42]. Similar to our findings, torpid Gould’s long-eared bats are capable of arousing from torpor when exposed to smoke stimuli [11].
Laboratory trial bats had a significantly shorter latency to first response and arousal time compared to controls (Figure 4 and Figure 5, respectively). First response latency to smoke alone was <1 min compared to no response within 60 min when exposed to sound alone. Due to the limitations of audio equipment, the sound recording of fire that was emitted from the speaker maxed out at 20 kHz, whereas actual fires have frequencies that range up to 60 kHz [17]. It is likely that eastern red bats have lower sensitivity to sound during the winter months when temperatures are lower, as seen in other species. For example, little brown bat hearing frequency sensitivity was negatively correlated with temperature, to the point of an inability to detect sounds when Ta and body temperature (Tb) were below 12 °C [34]. It is unknown how decreasing temperatures affect hearing or smell in eastern red bats. However, when smoke and sound are combined, time to first response decreased slightly. We suggest that the fire sound may act as a catalyst, speeding up the reaction time once smoke is detected. Similarly, exposure to smoke alone significantly decreased the arousal latency to approximately 30 min. Trials that included both smoke and sound resulted in shorter arousal times (Figure 4 and Figure 5). This study emphasized the importance of smoke for torpid bats’ awareness to fire. However, lab conditions did not fully encompass the whole range of parameters that are associated with actual fires. Smoke is more variable in nature than the conditions presented in the environmental chambers as measured by the CO monitor [43]. During all field trials, bats only exhibited a first response after wind blew smoke over the bats. CO monitored during one field trial peaked at 40 ppm, which is low compared to the amount measured by Dickinson et al. (2009) during an early-spring prescribed burn in Ohio (350 to <400 ppm). The wind speed in our field trial when CO was measured as 3.9 km per hour (km/h), which was near the average of all field burn trials (3.6 km/h), and smoke exposure was sporadic, usually lasting less than 1 min. In comparison, peaks and durations of exposure observed in the lab were greater than field measurements, with levels of over 70 ppm lasting longer than 12 min. A higher concentration of CO presumably leads to higher detection probability of fire, and therefore would shorten the latency of behavioral responses. However, since all laboratory trials exhibited longer latencies to first response and arousals it lends credibility to the finding that higher ambient temperatures decreased latency even when CO levels were lower. Our baseline arousal time of 60 min determined during lab trials is related to eastern red bat winter roosting ecology [23]. Unlike cave-dwelling bats, eastern red bats are constantly exposed to changes in their immediate environment. Considering that arousal is energetically expensive [44], we postulate that it is more efficient to delay exhibiting an escape response until the potential threat is more imminent. However, an innate response that triggers other senses would provide a selective advantage to a species that, if exposed to the threat of a fire, could escape before local population level mortality would occur. Although eastern red bats face wide fluctuations of temperatures compared to cave roosting species, they are able to remain in torpor until ambient conditions are appropriate for insect activity [16,45]. Eastern red bats in PRCA and southwestern Missouri were observed to roost in trees during periods when temperatures exceeded 10 °C [23,26]. This same temperature-dependent roosting behavior during the late fall/winter seasons was also observed in Seminole bats (Lasiurus seminolus) [24]. Perched bats in this study all flew during field trials compared to the 68.2% of prone bats that flew. This may be explained by bats that roost in trees possibly able to have access and/or assess variables in their surroundings more effectively, resulting in quicker responses than bats located under leaf litter [46]. However, a bat hidden in leaf litter may feel safer from the threat of fire than an exposed perched bat. Radio-tracked lasurine species roosting in trees in areas that were subsequently managed with prescribed fire were able to arouse and fly to safety [47]. More studies could be performed to determine response times of other species that may occur in the same forest type as eastern red bats during the winter, but utilize different roosting strategies (e.g., silver-haired bats (Lasionycteris noctivagans)).
In our field trials, increased temperature and wind speed were correlated with decreased latencies of response behavior in torpid eastern red bats. Bats only exhibited a first response after wind blew smoke over the bats. Wind during the night prior to a trial had a significant, but unexplained correlation with latency behaviors. The occurrence of wind propelling smoke over bats during fires confirmed lab findings that smoke greatly reduces latency time to first response. Latencies of bats that exhibited flight response from prone leaf litter roosts are too long (21.8 min on average) to allow for successful escape from a fire in close proximity. If bats were in natural roosting positions with the chance to passively rewarm before the onset of fire, and because it is likely that CO concentrations are higher during prescribed woodland burns [34], responses may be quicker and more effectual. The authors have observed bats flying away from leaf litter during backing fires (i.e., smoke blowing away from the direction of fire). Bats in shallower states of torpor or in states of activity due to warm Ta can be assumed not to need smoke to elicit an escape response. The location of roosts that maximize solar exposure [23,26] is a factor that would decrease latency times to arousal and thus increase survivorship. As an example, one bat that did not exhibit a flight response during a field trial was left at the burn arena. During the mid-afternoon (14:30), the bat was observed on top of the leaf litter, breathing heavily, with its back towards the sun. It responded to our approach and took flight when we were less than 2 m away. This observation suggests that the bat may have been in a state closer to arousal from torpor while passively rewarming relative to during the fire trial.
Forest managers typically do not start prescribed burns with temperatures below 10 °C, but USDA recommendations [30] do not preclude starting prescribed fires at lower temperatures. Personal observations by the authors and numerous anecdotal reports indicate that eastern red bats can successfully evade approaching fires even if previous nighttime temperatures were below freezing. For example, temperatures during the night prior to a field trial burn in our study reached 0.8 °C (2 March 2008), with all three bats flying within 18 min when the temperature before onset of the fire averaged 13.6 °C, suggesting that temperatures before the onset of the burn may have a more significant effect on arousals than temperatures during the previous night.
Bats were occasionally observed to awaken and crawl away from their roost locations during burn trials. The only female included in our study was tested for behavioral responses during a field trial. The bat exhibited a first response 1 min 19 s after the fire started, and arousal latency was <8 min, but it exhibited no flight response. The bat crawled approximately 0.5 m away from its prone position to another pile of leaves and remained there throughout the trial. If a bat is aroused due to cues from an oncoming fire, it may be able to move to an area where the fire would not cause injury or death. Burrowing behaviors were never observed in burn trials, but this behavior may be futile based on model results where lethal heat levels are able to penetrate into leaf litter [25].

5. Conclusions

Our study demonstrates that a migratory, tree-roosting bat species is capable of detecting potential fire threats during winter torpor when in precarious roost locations. Additionally, we experimentally confirmed anecdotal evidence of red bats emerging from leaf litter when in the presence of fire. Considering that all migratory bat species are of conservatory concern, more study into the winter ecology, responses to management practices, and baseline biological knowledge is needed.
Successful management planning requires both researchers and managers to overcome gaps in understanding of how bat assemblages respond to landscape management patterns [29,48]. Requirements for bat habitat throughout the year need to be quantified, and then interpreted by managers who can satisfy these and other forest management objectives [49]. To this end, an extrapolation to explain multi-species bat latency in response to fire across a range of temperatures based on these and other data was made and published (see [43]) that can be implemented in effective burn management plans.

Author Contributions

Study design, data collection, analyses, and primary writing were carried out by J.T.L., A.S. and L.W.R.; figure preparation and manuscript revision and review were handled by D.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was possible through the funding and cooperation of Missouri Department of Conservation, Missouri State University, and Dickerson Park Zoo.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Protocols followed guidelines for animal use in research and were approved by the Missouri State University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (Protocol # 2009A).

Acknowledgments

We thank the several faculty members and students at Missouri State University and the Missouri Department of Conservation staff who dedicated time to assist these lab and field efforts via physical and moral support, especially: Josh Flinn, Shelly and Ron Colatskie, Brandon Carney, Katie Baker, Christin Dzurick Byrd, Brad Moorman, Tom Tomasi, Dan Drees, and Robert (Bob) Holmes.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Loeb, S.C. Qualitative synthesis of temperate bat responses to silvicultural treatments-where do we go from here? J. Mammal. 2020, 101, 1513–1525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Monroe, M.E.; Converse, S.J. The effects of early season and late season prescribed fires on small mammals in a Sierra Nevada mixed conifer forest. For. Ecol. Manag. 2006, 236, 229–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Klimas, K.; Hiesl, P.; Hagan, D.; Park, D. Prescribed fire effects on sediment and nutrient exports in forested environments: A review. J. Environ. Qual. 2020, 49, 793–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Warziniack, T.; Sham, C.H.; Morgan, R.; Feferholtz, Y. Effects of Forest Cover on Drinking Water Treatment Costs; American Water Works Association: Denver, CO, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  5. Wright, H.A.; Bailey, A.W. Wildlife, in Fire Ecology: United States and Southern Canada; John Wiley and Sons, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
  6. Kelly, L.T.; Brotons, L. Using fire to promote biodiversity. Science 2017, 355, 1264–1265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Howard, W.E.; Fenner, R.L.; Childs, H.E. Wildlife Survival in Brush Burns. J. Range Manag. 1959, 12, 230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Grafe, T.U.; Döbler, S.; Linsenmair, K.E. Frogs flee from the sound of fire. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2002, 269, 999–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Geluso, K.N.; Bragg, T.B. Fire-Avoidance Behavior of Meadow Voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Am. Midl. Nat. 1986, 116, 202–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Withgott, J.H.; Amlaner, C.J. Elaphe obsoleta obsoleta. Herpetol. Rev. 1996, 27, 145–146. [Google Scholar]
  11. Doty, A.C.; Currie, S.E.; Stawski, C.; Geiser, F. Can bats sense smoke during deep torpor? Physiol. Behav. 2018, 185, 31–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Whitaker, J.O.; Rose, R.K.; Padgett, T.M. Food of the red bat (Lasiurus borealis) in winter in the Great Dismal Swamp, North Carolina and Virginia. Am. Midl. Nat. 1997, 137, 408–411. [Google Scholar]
  13. Saugey, D.A.; Crump, B.G.; Vaughn, R.L.; Heidt, G.A. Notes on the Natural History of Lasiurus borealis in Arkansas. J. Arkansas Acad. Sci. 1998, 52, 92–98. [Google Scholar]
  14. Moormann, C.E.; Russell, K.R.; Menzel, M.A.; Lohr, S.M.; Ellenberger, J.E.; Van Lear, D.H. Bats roosting in deciduous leaf litter. Bat Res. News 1999, 40, 74–75. [Google Scholar]
  15. Boyles, J.G.; Aubrey, D.P. Managing forests with prescribed fire: Implications for a cavity-dwelling bat species. For. Ecol. Manag. 2006, 222, 108–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Dunbar, M.B.; Tomasi, T.E. Arousal patterns, metabolic rate, and an energy budget of eastern red bats (Lasiurus borealis) in winter. J. Mammal. 2006, 87, 1096–1102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Scesny, A.A. Detection of Fire by Eastern Red Bats (Lasiurus borealis) Arousal from Torpor. Master’s Thesis, Missouri State University, Springfield, MO, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  18. Dunbar, M.B.; Whitaker, J.O.; Robbins, L.W. Winter feeding by bats in Missouri. Acta Chiropterol. 2007, 9, 305–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Shump, K.A.; Shump, A.U. Lasiurus borealis. Mamm. Species 1982, 183, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Cryan, P.M. Seasonal Distribution of Migratory Tree Bats (Lasiurus and Lasionycteris) in North America. J. Mammal. 2003, 84, 579–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Hutchinson, J.T.; Lacki, M.J. Selection of Day Roosts by Red Bats in Mixed Mesophytic Forests. J. Wildl. Manag. 2000, 64, 87–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Mager, K.J.; Nelson, T.A. Roost-site selection by eastern red bats (Lasiurus borealis). Am. Midl. Nat. 2001, 145, 120–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Mormann, B.; Robbins, L.W. Winter Roosting Ecology of Eastern Red Bats in Southwest Missouri. J. Wildl. Manag. 2007, 71, 213–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Hein, C.D.; Castleberry, S.B.; Miller, K.V. Male seminole bat winter roost-site selection in a managed forest. J. Wildl. Manag. 2008, 72, 1756–1764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Perry, R.W.; McDaniel, V.L. Temperatures below leaf litter during winter prescribed burns: Implications for litter-roosting bats. Int. J. Wildl. Fire 2015, 24, 544–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Flinn, J.R. Winter Roosting Behavior of Red Bats (Lasiurus borealis): Habitat Use, Microclimate, and Effects of Ambient Temperature on Roost Choice. Master’s Thesis, Missouri State University, Springfield, MO, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  27. Ryan, K.C.; Knapp, E.E.; Varner, J.M. Prescribed fire in North American forests and woodlands: History, current practice, and challenges. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2013, 11, e15–e24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Austin, L.V.; Silvis, A.; Ford, W.M.; Muthersbaugh, M.; Powers, K.E. Bat Activity Following Restoration Prescribed Burning in the Central Appalachian Upland and Riparian Habitats. Nat. Areas J. 2018, 38, 183–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Burns, L.K.L.; Loeb, S.C.; Bridges, W.C. Effects of fire and its severity on occupancy of bats in mixed pine-oak forests. For. Ecol. Manag. 2019, 446, 151–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. United States Department of Agriculture Prescribed Burning. Iowa Job Sheet Conserv. Pract. 338; Natural Resources Conservation Serive: Des Moines, IA, USA, 2009.
  31. Winhold, L.; Kurta, A.; Foster, R. Long-term change in an assemblage of North American bats: Are eastern red bats declining? Acta Chiropterol. 2008, 10, 359–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Murray, K.L.; Britzke, E.R.; Robbins, L.W. Variation in search-phase calls of bats. J. Mammal. 2001, 82, 728–737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Luo, J.; Clarin, B.M.; Borissov, I.M.; Siemers, B.M. Are torpid bats immune to anthropogenic noise? J. Exp. Biol. 2013, 217, 1072–1078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Harrison, J.B. Temperature effects on responses in the auditory system of the little brown bat Myotis l. lucifugus. Physiol. Zool. 1965, 38, 34–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Griffin, D.R. The importance of atmospheric attenuation for the echolocation of bats (Chiroptera). Anim. Behav. 1971, 19, 55–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Lawrence, B.D.; Simmons, J.A. Measurements of atmospheric attenuation at ultrasonic frequencies and the significance for echolocation by bats. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1982, 71, 585–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Bloss, J.; Acree, T.E.; Bloss, J.M.; Hood, W.R.; Kunz, T.H. Potential use of chemical cues for colony-mate recognition in the big brown bat, Eptesicus fuscus. J. Chem. Ecol. 2002, 28, 819–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Brokaw, A.F.; Smotherman, M. Olfactory tracking strategies in a Neotropical fruit bat. J. Exp. Biol. 2021, 224, jeb.231829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Stevenson, A.P. Effects of Prescribed Burning in Missouri Ozark Upland Forests. Master’s Thesis, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  40. Geiser, F.; Stawski, C.; Doty, A.C.; Cooper, C.E.; Nowack, J. A burning question: What are the risks and benefits of mammalian torpor during and after fires? Conserv. Physiol. 2018, 6, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Geiser, F. Ontogeny and phylogeny of endothermy and torpor in mammals and birds. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 2008, 150, 176–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Doty, A.C.; Stawski, C.; Law, B.S.; Geiser, F. Post-wildfire physiological ecology of an Australian microbat. J. Comp. Physiol. B Biochem. Syst. Environ. Physiol. 2016, 186, 937–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Dickinson, M.B.; Norris, J.C.; Bova, A.S.; Kremens, R.L.; Young, V.; Lacki, M.J. Effects of wildland fire smoke on a tree-roosting bat: Integrating a plume model, field measurements, and mammalian dose-response relationships. Can. J. For. Res. 2010, 40, 2187–2203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Geiser, F. Metabolic Rate and Body Temperature Reduction During Hibernation and Daily Torpor. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 2004, 66, 239–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Davis, W.H.; Reite, O.B. Responses of bats from temperate regions to changes in ambient temperature. Biol. Bull. 1967, 132, 320–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Carter, T.C.; Ford, W.M.; Menzel, M.A. Fire and bats in the Southeast and Mid-Atlantic: More questions than answers? In The Role of Fire for Nongame Wildlife Management and Community Restoration: Traditional Uses and New Directions; Ford, W.M., Russell, K.R., Moormann, C.E., Eds.; USFS General Technical Report NE-288; U.S. Department of Agriculture: Forest Service, Northern Research Station: Newtown Square, PA, USA, 2002; pp. 139–143. [Google Scholar]
  47. Jorge, M.H.; Ford, W.M.; Sweeten, S.E.; Freeze, S.R.; True, M.C.; St. Germain, M.J.; Taylor, H.; Gorman, K.M.; Garrison, E.P.; Cherry, M.J. Winter roost selection of Lasiurine tree bats in a pyric landscape. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0245695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Cryan, P.M.; Veilleux, J.P. Migration and use of autumn, winter, and spring roosts by tree bats. Bats For. Conserv. Manag. 2007, 153–176. [Google Scholar]
  49. Guldin, J.M.; Emmingham, W.H.; Carter, S.A.; Saugey, D.A. Silvicultural practices and management of habitat for bats. Bats For. Conserv. Manag. 2007, 177–206. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Experimental set up for lab-based trials with directionality of smoke indicated. The bat (A) was placed in a cage with no barriers blocking the scent or sound of stimuli on the top shelf of the environmental chamber (B). Depending on the experimental trial, air/smoke was forced through the chamber using the bellows (C) and removed on the opposite side (D), passing through tubing, and sound was emitted through the speaker (E) in the bottom of the chamber. Bats were subjected to these stimuli individually and concurrently during trials.
Figure 1. Experimental set up for lab-based trials with directionality of smoke indicated. The bat (A) was placed in a cage with no barriers blocking the scent or sound of stimuli on the top shelf of the environmental chamber (B). Depending on the experimental trial, air/smoke was forced through the chamber using the bellows (C) and removed on the opposite side (D), passing through tubing, and sound was emitted through the speaker (E) in the bottom of the chamber. Bats were subjected to these stimuli individually and concurrently during trials.
Forests 12 01347 g001
Figure 2. Schematic for field-based trials showing the fabricated forest floor and position of troughs containing leaf litter that was burned to simulate prescribed fire. Individual bats were placed into individual enclosures with leaf litter the night prior to field trials.
Figure 2. Schematic for field-based trials showing the fabricated forest floor and position of troughs containing leaf litter that was burned to simulate prescribed fire. Individual bats were placed into individual enclosures with leaf litter the night prior to field trials.
Forests 12 01347 g002
Figure 3. Eastern red bat taking flight during field trial.
Figure 3. Eastern red bat taking flight during field trial.
Forests 12 01347 g003
Figure 4. First response (i.e., movement or increased respiration) latencies of eastern red bats (n = 15) exposed to different lab-based treatments. Bats that failed to have a first response were given a value of 60 min. There was a difference between treatments (Friedman p < 0.05). Latency to first response was shorter in the smoke treatment than the controls and fire sound treatments (Wilcoxon p < 0.05) and showed no difference when compared to smoke and fire sound combination and the smoke treatment (Wilcoxon p > 0.05).
Figure 4. First response (i.e., movement or increased respiration) latencies of eastern red bats (n = 15) exposed to different lab-based treatments. Bats that failed to have a first response were given a value of 60 min. There was a difference between treatments (Friedman p < 0.05). Latency to first response was shorter in the smoke treatment than the controls and fire sound treatments (Wilcoxon p < 0.05) and showed no difference when compared to smoke and fire sound combination and the smoke treatment (Wilcoxon p > 0.05).
Forests 12 01347 g004
Figure 5. Arousal times of red bats (n = 15) exposed to the different lab-based treatments. Bats that failed to arouse were given a value of 60 min. There was a difference between treatments (Friedman p < 0.05). Latency to arouse was shorter in the smoke treatment than the smoke control, and shorter in the smoke and fire sound combination than in the smoke treatment (Wilcoxon p < 0.05).
Figure 5. Arousal times of red bats (n = 15) exposed to the different lab-based treatments. Bats that failed to arouse were given a value of 60 min. There was a difference between treatments (Friedman p < 0.05). Latency to arouse was shorter in the smoke treatment than the smoke control, and shorter in the smoke and fire sound combination than in the smoke treatment (Wilcoxon p < 0.05).
Forests 12 01347 g005
Table 1. Correlation coefficients (r) and associated p values of latency to behaviors vs. ambient weather parameters. Values are for each time period: 1 = night before burn trial (2200) to one min before sunrise, 2 = sunrise to one min before onset of trial, 3 = onset of burn trial to end of burn trial. For first response (i.e., movement or increased respiration) and arousal, n = 15 (except time period 1 wind speed (n = 14)). For flight, n = 17 (except time period 1 wind speed (n = 16)). <indicates significance at p < 0.05, d.f. = (n − 2).
Table 1. Correlation coefficients (r) and associated p values of latency to behaviors vs. ambient weather parameters. Values are for each time period: 1 = night before burn trial (2200) to one min before sunrise, 2 = sunrise to one min before onset of trial, 3 = onset of burn trial to end of burn trial. For first response (i.e., movement or increased respiration) and arousal, n = 15 (except time period 1 wind speed (n = 14)). For flight, n = 17 (except time period 1 wind speed (n = 16)). <indicates significance at p < 0.05, d.f. = (n − 2).
BehaviorTime PeriodTemperatureWindRelative Humidity
rp Valuerp Valuerp Value
First Response1−0.53<0.05 −0.36>0.050.36>0.05
2−0.76<0.05−0.84<0.050.45>0.05
3−0.78<0.05−0.91<0.050.60<0.05
Arousal1−0.62<0.05−0.48>0.050.25>0.05
2−0.84<0.05−0.88<0.050.38>0.05
3−0.86<0.05−0.96<0.050.51>0.05
Flight1−0.79<0.05−0.61<0.050.29>0.05
2−0.88<0.05−0.83<0.050.16>0.05
3−0.86<0.05−0.80<0.050.06>0.05
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Layne, J.T.; Green, D.; Scesny, A.; Robbins, L.W. Eastern Red Bat Responses to Fire during Winter Torpor. Forests 2021, 12, 1347. https://doi.org/10.3390/f12101347

AMA Style

Layne JT, Green D, Scesny A, Robbins LW. Eastern Red Bat Responses to Fire during Winter Torpor. Forests. 2021; 12(10):1347. https://doi.org/10.3390/f12101347

Chicago/Turabian Style

Layne, Jason T., Dana Green, Anna Scesny, and Lynn W. Robbins. 2021. "Eastern Red Bat Responses to Fire during Winter Torpor" Forests 12, no. 10: 1347. https://doi.org/10.3390/f12101347

APA Style

Layne, J. T., Green, D., Scesny, A., & Robbins, L. W. (2021). Eastern Red Bat Responses to Fire during Winter Torpor. Forests, 12(10), 1347. https://doi.org/10.3390/f12101347

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop