3.1. Descriptive Statistics
In the test group, results show a predominance of neutral or negative attitudes towards the conservation of the building (around 52% of the responses) and low levels of perception of control (pointed out by about 55% of the respondents), as presented in
Table 3. The low average values are primarily due to the layer “services” that concentrates only on negative beliefs. This layer has the lowest values: attitudes regarding its conservation are shallower (with only 13% of positive responses) than the conservation of other layers, and intentions and self-reported behaviours are not positive for more than 80% of participants. On the other extreme, the layer “structure” presents high average replies, with positive attitudes (86%) and perception of high expectations, but also good levels of control (for 67% of respondents). Moreover, the layer “skin” has consistently positive responses, with 74% of the respondents expressing positive attitudes and 72% expressing positive intentions towards its conservation. Finally, in the layer “Spirit of the Place”, there is a positive attitude towards conservation, but it presents the lowest value on the perceived levels of control.
At the attribute level, the results allow identifying the most valued attributes of the building and the priorities in the design (
Table 4). The conservation of the structural system, for instance, is seen for 86% of the respondents in the test group as valuable, and 100% feel social pressure to conserve this element, despite the low levels of perceived behavioural control (57% positive responses). As a result, 89% of the respondents self-report high percentages of conservation of the structural system. Other indicators with similar positive reactions are the structural materials, the façade, and the building shape. In the opposite direction, the conservation of the layer “services” concentrates more negative reactions, with the indicators energy and heating, ventilation, and water presenting the lowest attitudes, intentions, perceived control, and self-reported conservation behaviours. At the same time, more than half of the respondents do not feel social pressure for the conservation of this layer. These results show that the services layer is the least conserved by the design students in the case study.
In the control group, the descriptive statistics show results very similar to the test group, with slightly lower perception of control (40% positive responses, instead of 45%) and social pressure (41% instead of 43%). On average, levels of attitude, intention, and behaviour do not vary significantly between the two groups. As in the first group, the layer “structure” consistently presents positive replies but is surpassed in the control group by the layer “space plan”, with 87% positive attitudes, 68% high perceived norms, and 63% of perceived control (
Table 5). This is a significant difference from the test group, where positive attitudes towards the conservation of the layer “space plan” are expressed by only 55% of participants. The layer “services” has even lower results in the control group, with only 4% positive attitudes. Despite this, frequencies of intention and behaviour towards “services” are similar in both groups. In the control group, participants express a very low level of perceived behavioural control in relation to the layer “services” (14% positive responses).
At the attribute level, differences emerge between the control and test groups, mainly in the more positive indicators. The relation of the building with the surroundings, the building shape, and the layout of the space plan are considered more valuable by the majority of the respondents in the control group. The control group coincides with the test group in identifying the indicators energy and heating, ventilation, and water as the most negative ones. However, despite the similar frequency of negative attitudes towards the layer “services”, the control group feels less social pressure to preserve this layer than the test group and shows lower behavioural control (
Table 6).
3.2. Comparative Analysis of Intentions and Behaviours on Test and Control Groups
Nonparametric Mann–Whitney tests were used to measure the difference in the intentions and behaviours of the test and the control group. The null hypothesis was that “the two groups have equal means on attitudes, subjective norms, perceptions of control intentions and behaviours regarding the conservation of the building attributes”. The tests were performed at the broader and detailed levels. The results at the broader level of the psychological constructs are insufficient to reject the null hypothesis—the test group presents lower mean values (in a scale from 1 to 5, where 1 is the most positive value) on subjective norms, perceived behavioural control, and intention, than the control group, though results are not statistically significant (
p > 0.1). The Mann–Whitney test also suggests that attitudes are, on average, more negative in the test group.
Table 7 presents the results of the Mann–Whitney test at the psychological construct level, where “T.mean rank” refers to the test group and “C.mean rank” refers to the control group, the U-value compares the differences between the two groups, and Sig. refers to the statistical significance or probability value (
p).
However, at the building layer and attribute levels, the analysis of frequencies shows that the test group presents more positive results than the control group in 53% of the indicators, even if not always statistically significant. Further, the Mann–Whitney tests evidence statistically significant differences between the two groups, particularly in the layers “skin” and “space plan”. While in the layer “skin”, the test group concentrates more positive responses, in the layer “space plan”, the control group expresses stronger positive attitudes and conservation behaviours. On the one hand, attitudes towards the conservation of the skin, particularly the façade, the materials, and the detailing, are significantly more positive in the test group. Additionally, this group manifests a more robust perception of social pressure (subjective norms) to preserve the abovementioned indicators of the “skin” and more positive intentions towards the conservation of the materials. However, no significant differences were found in the self-reported behaviours towards this layer.
On the other hand, the control group (that did not use the building passport) shows more positive attitudes towards the conservation of the space plan, both of the layout and the interior–exterior relationships. Thus, even though no significant differences were found in the intentions towards the conservation of the space plan, the control group self-reports to have conserved more of the space plan layout.
Table 8 summarises the attributes in which statistically significant differences were found between the control and the test group.
3.3. Measuring the Intention-Behaviour Gap in the Test and Control Groups
To further understand the differences identified through the Mann–Whitney tests, bivariate correlation analysis was performed to observe differences in the correlation between the psychological constructs in the test and control groups. This analysis supports the correlations predicted by the theoretical model [
19,
20,
43,
49,
50,
51], demonstrating the role of attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioural control in shaping conservation intentions. However, while the theoretical model strongly supports the prediction of intention, the same does not happen with the prediction of behaviour, which is not statistically significant (see
Table 9). Although in the scope of this research, conservation behaviours are not directly correlated with expressed intentions, the results show a positive correlation between attitudes and behaviours, both in the test (
p = 0.039) and in the control group (
p = 0.069).
Some differences emerge between the two groups, namely regarding the strength of moderating beliefs in the formation of intentions. In the test group, attitudes have a stronger positive correlation with intentions (r = 0.880, p < 0.001) than in the control group (r = 0.653, p < 0.001), evidencing that an increase in positive attitudes increases positive intentions after implementing the passport. In the test group, subjective norms and perceived behavioural control present positive correlations with intentions, too, but to a lesser degree than attitudes. In the control group, however, subjective norms (r = 0.825, p < 0.001) and perceived behavioural control (r = 0.664, p = 0.002) are stronger predictors of intention than attitudes (r = 0.639, p < 0.001). Interestingly, subjective norms present the same correlation with intentions in the test and the control groups, suggesting that tutors’ expectations have an important moderation effect on personal evaluations.
Since no correlation was found between intentions and behaviours, the effect of applying the building passport was analysed by comparing the most determinant variables for conservation intentions through single linear regression.
Table 10 presents the results of the single linear regressions, on which “B” stands for beta coefficient (the degree of change in the outcome variable for every unit of change in the predictor variable), “R
2” refers to R-squared (goodness-of-fit measure for the model), and “Sig.” refers to the statistical significance, through the probability value (
p).
The results showed a significant relationship between the attitudes towards the “services” (R2 = 0.294; p = 0.013) and the “space plan” (R2 = 0.648, p < 0.001) and overall conservation intentions in the test group. Subjective norms also have a significant contribution in this group, moderating the conservation intentions in the layer “services” (R2 = 0.582; p < 0.001). With an R2 value of 0.648, the attitudes towards the “space plan” have the most substantial effect on the overall expressed conservation intentions. In the opposite direction, the structure layer is the least significant in predicting the conservation intentions of the participants who used the building passport, followed by the layer “skin”. This result suggests that general conservation intentions do not reflect the high attitudes towards structure and skin. However, they tend to be moderated by the lower valued layers, such as the services and the space plan.
Almost symmetrically, in the control group, the attitudes towards the layer “skin” have the most significant correlation with conservation intentions (R2 = 0.511; p = 0.001), while the attitudes towards the “services” (R2 = 0.000; p = 0.955) and the “space plan” (R2 = 0.038; p = 0.425) have the lowest one.
Considering the single linear regression results, multiple regression with backwards elimination was performed to find out the models that better explain the conservation intentions in the test and the control groups. The results, presented in
Table 11, confirm that expressed intentions on the control group tend to be normative controlled. The model of intentions in the control group, explaining until 92.5% of the variance on intentions (R
2 = 0.925), suggests that the most positive intentions towards conservation are found in the students with higher perceptions of social pressure towards the conservation of the site, the skin, the services, and the spirit of place. It also suggests that highly positive attitudes towards the façade do not reflect overall positive conservation intentions.
In the test group, the results of the multiple regression suggest attitudinally controlled intentions (
Table 11). In this case, stronger conservation intentions were found in students who report positive attitudes towards the services, space, and spirit, but, again, not necessarily towards the conservation of the façade. The norms towards the structure and the spirit of place present a negative correlation with intention, meaning that the higher the perception of social pressure to conserve, the lower the conservation intentions. At the same time, the attitudes towards the conservation of the spirit of place contributed significantly to the model (B = 0.298,
p < 0.001), while the attitudes towards the conservation of the skin did not (B = −0.095,
p = 0.094).
The results show that both intentions and behaviours are strongly led by normative and attitudinal beliefs, with perceived behavioural control having a minor influence on conservation decisions in the case study. While intentions do not significantly correlate with reported behaviours, the Pearson correlation showed relevant correlations of behaviour with attitudes and subjective norms that were further investigated through multiple linear regression to identify the main differences between expressed intentions and reported behaviours. In the analysis of behaviour, attitudes overtake norms in the control group, with a significant contribution of the attitudes towards the services (
p = 0.083) and the spirit of place (
p = 0.031). Together with the norms towards the conservation of the space (
p < 0.001), the model explains around 68% of the variance in behaviour (R
2 = 0.675) in the control group (
Table 11).
Regarding the conservation behaviours in the control group, the perception of the norms towards the space and the attitudes towards the spirit of the place highly influence positive conservation behaviours. Negative attitudes towards the services, on the contrary, do not translate into negative conservation behaviours.
In the test group, attitudes remain the most influential psychological construct to predict conservation behaviours. Attitudes towards the conservation of the services layer have a negative correlation with behaviour (B = −0.431, p = 0.004), suggesting that even if participants show a negative attitude than can end up preserving this layer due to intervening factors, the fact that norms towards this layer have a significant positive correlation (B = 0.292, p = 0.010) with behaviour may help to explain this difference. Compared with the model explaining conservation intentions in the test group, attitudes towards the skin change to a positive correlation (B = 0.270, p = 0.052), while the norms towards the spirit have a significantly more negative influence on conservation behaviours (B = −0.801, p = 0.006) than in conservation intentions.
The predictive model for conservation behaviour of the test group (R
2 = 0.937) suggests that more positive conservation behaviours happen with students who have positive attitudes towards the conservation of site, space, and spirit, despite negative attitudes towards the conservation of the services (
Table 11). However, the higher subjective norms towards the conservation of the spirit do not translate into general positive conservation behaviours. As suggested by the descriptive statistics, this may happen because of the moderation effect of the low perceived behavioural control.
3.4. Correlations between Behaviour and the Building Passport
The regression analysis of intentions and behaviours suggests differences between the test and the control groups, with the models of the test group expressing more complex decision processes, with more factors affecting the reported conservation behaviours. To analyse the causal effect of the passport and the differences found between the test and control groups (
Table 10), single linear regression was applied for each indicator of the building passport. The indicators for which significant relationships were found are presented in
Figure 2, including the average sustainability assessment from the test group, the directionality of the relation (positive or negative), and if there were significant differences with the control group in the Mann-Whitney test.
The results show significant correlations, mainly between the passport and the attitudes (22% of the indicators) and subjective norms (25%). In addition, perceived behavioural control (17%), intentions and behaviours (both 14% of the indicators) seem to be affected by the building passport, but to a lesser extent. While some relations are positive—with positive assessments increasing the likelihood of positive conservation behaviours—some relations are negative, suggesting that despite negative assessments, participants can still engage in positive conservation behaviours and vice versa.
In general, attitudes present a positive correlation with the passport. Interestingly, these correlations emerge dominantly in indicators with lower assessment ratings (C, D, or B-), such as “sufficient thermal insulation”, “windows avoid thermal losses”, or “presence of energy autonomy strategies”. This suggests that the passport has a role in reinforcing pre-existing negative beliefs about specific building attributes. Significant correlations, however, also emerge in the most positive indicators, rated with A, such as “presence of transitional spaces” or structures that are “simple to build and maintain”.
Moreover, the correlation with subjective norms is mostly positive, with higher sustainability assessment relating to higher perceptions of social pressure to conserve the building. However, as happened with the attitudes, this correlation emerges more clearly when assessments are low (C, D), indicating that the building passport might justify decisions not to conserve the services and the envelope attributes related to energy needs (such as openings, façade, and roof).
In analysing the perceived behavioural control, an essential number of negative correlations emerge in the layer “structure”. This result suggests that despite the positive contributions for sustainability (ratings A and B), participants do not perceive behavioural control over the conservation of this layer, influencing their intention to preserve it. Together with the negative correlations found between the sustainability assessment and attitudes and subjective norms in this layer, the model explains the negative correlation between the intention to conserve the structure and the positive assessments in the building passport.
The assessment of the indicator “positive impact on biodiversity” affects attitudes, subjective norms, and intentions towards the site’s conservation. The assessment on the indicator “energy needs”, in the skin layer, and on the indicators related with water and energy and heating, in the services layer, consistently affect attitudes, norms, perception of control and intentions, shaping results significantly different from the control group, according to the Mann–Whitney test. The fact that the Mann–Whitney tests show significant differences in indicators on which no direct correlation with the passport was found suggests an indirect multi-effect of the sustainability assessment—for instance, while the assessment of the “relation of the building with climate” affects attitudes towards the conservation of this attribute, the most significant differences between the test and the control group emerge on the subjective norms and intentions. Thus, a possible interpretation is that the sustainability assessment may indirectly affect participants’ perceptions of norms and expressed intentions.