Machine Learning a Probabilistic Structural Equation Model to Explain the Impact of Climate Risk Perceptions on Policy Support
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Sample and Dataset
2.2. PSEM Machine Learning
2.3. Estimating a SEM Imputed from the PSEM
2.4. Goodness-of-Fit Statistics
3. Results
3.1. Data-Driven Machine Learning Models Can Account for Complex Interactions Among Measured and Latent Variables to Explain Climate Policy Support
3.2. Machine-Learned PSEM Enables Data-Driven Configuration of Measured Variables in Identification of Latent Variables and Their Class Sizes
3.3. Marginal and Conditional Probability Analysis of Policy Support Uncovers a Previously Unidentified Class of “Lukewarm Supporters”, Different from Strong Supporters and Opposers
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Zia, A. Post-Kyoto Climate Governance: Confronting the Politics of Scale, Ideology and Knowledge; Routledge: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Beckage, B.; Gross, L.J.; Lacasse, K.; Carr, E.; Metcalf, S.S.; Winter, J.M.; Howe, P.D.; Fefferman, N.; Franck, T.; Zia, A. Linking models of human behaviour and climate alters projected climate change. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2018, 8, 79–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beckage, B.; Lacasse, K.; Winter, J.M.; Gross, L.J.; Fefferman, N.; Hoffman, F.M.; Metcalf, S.S.; Franck, T.; Carr, E.; Zia, A. The Earth has humans, so why don’t our climate models? Clim. Chang. 2020, 163, 181–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zia, A. Synergies and Trade-Offs between Climate Change Adaptation and Mitigation across Multiple Scales of Governance. In Adaptiveness: Changing Earth System Governance; Siebenhuener, B., Djalante, R., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Rising, J.A.; Taylor, C.; Ives, M.C.; Ward, R.E. Challenges and innovations in the economic evaluation of the risks of climate change. Ecol. Econ. 2022, 197, 107437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, C.; Guivarch, C.; Kriegler, E.; Van Ruijven, B.; Van Vuuren, D.P.; Krey, V.; Schwanitz, V.J.; Thompson, E.L. Evaluating process-based integrated assessment models of climate change mitigation. Clim. Chang. 2021, 166, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Beek, L.; Oomen, J.; Hajer, M.; Pelzer, P.; van Vuuren, D. Navigating the political: An analysis of political calibration of integrated assessment modelling in light of the 1.5 C goal. Environ. Sci. Policy 2022, 133, 193–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burstein, P. The impact of public opinion on public policy: A review and an agenda. Political Res. Q. 2003, 56, 29–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shapiro, R.Y. Public opinion and American democracy. Public Opin. Q. 2011, 75, 982–1017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Drews, S.; Van den Bergh, J.C. What explains public support for climate policies? A review of empirical and experimental studies. Clim. Policy 2016, 16, 855–876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Attari, S.Z.; Schoen, M.; Davidson, C.I.; DeKay, M.L.; de Bruin, W.B.; Dawes, R.; Small, M.J. Preferences for change: Do individuals prefer voluntary actions, soft regulations, or hard regulations to decrease fossil fuel consumption? Ecol. Econ. 2009, 68, 1701–1710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dietz, T.; Dan, A.; Shwom, R. Support for climate change policy: Social psychological and social structural influences. Rural Sociol. 2007, 72, 185–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leiserowitz, A. Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role of affect, imagery, and values. Clim. Chang. 2006, 77, 45–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCright, A.M.; Dunlap, R.E.; Xiao, C. Perceived scientific agreement and support for government action on climate change in the USA. Clim. Chang. 2013, 119, 511–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steg, L.; Dreijerink, L.; Abrahamse, W. Factors influencing the acceptability of energy policies: A test of VBN theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 2005, 25, 415–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zia, A.; Todd, A.M. Evaluating the effects of ideology on public understanding of climate change science: How to improve communication across ideological divides? Public Underst. Sci. 2010, 19, 743–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bostrom, A.; O’Connor, R.E.; Böhm, G.; Hanss, D.; Bodi, O.; Ekström, F.; Halder, P.; Jeschke, S.; Mack, B.; Qu, M. Causal thinking and support for climate change policies: International survey findings. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2012, 22, 210–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patt, A.G.; Weber, E.U. Perceptions and communication strategies for the many uncertainties relevant for climate policy. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Clim. Chang. 2014, 5, 219–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steg, L.; Dreijerink, L.; Abrahamse, W. Why are energy policies acceptable and effective? Environ. Behav. 2006, 38, 92–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adaman, F.; Karalı, N.; Kumbaroğlu, G.; Or, İ.; Özkaynak, B.; Zenginobuz, Ü. What determines urban households’ willingness to pay for CO2 emission reductions in Turkey: A contingent valuation survey. Energy Policy 2011, 39, 689–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Franzen, A.; Vogl, D. Two decades of measuring environmental attitudes: A comparative analysis of 33 countries. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2013, 23, 1001–1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O‘Connor, R.E.; Bard, R.J.; Fisher, A. Risk perceptions, general environmental beliefs, and willingness to address climate change. Risk Anal. 1999, 19, 461–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Owen, A.L.; Conover, E.; Videras, J.; Wu, S. Heat waves, droughts, and preferences for environmental policy. J. Policy Anal. Manag. 2012, 31, 556–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petrovic, N.; Madrigano, J.; Zaval, L. Motivating mitigation: When health matters more than climate change. Clim. Chang. 2014, 126, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marcot, B.G.; Penman, T.D. Advances in Bayesian network modelling: Integration of modelling technologies. Environ. Model. Softw. 2019, 111, 386–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pearl, J. Probabilistic Reasoning in Intelligent Systems: Networks of Plausible Inference; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Kullback, S. Information Theory and Statistics; Courier Corporation: North Chelmsford, MA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Conrady, S.; Jouffe, L. Bayesian Networks and BayesiaLab: A Practical Introduction for Researchers; Bayesia USA: Nashville, TN, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Bouman, T.; Verschoor, M.; Albers, C.J.; Böhm, G.; Fisher, S.D.; Poortinga, W.; Whitmarsh, L.; Steg, L. When worry about climate change leads to climate action: How values, worry and personal responsibility relate to various climate actions. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2020, 62, 102061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ding, D.; Maibach, E.W.; Zhao, X.; Roser-Renouf, C.; Leiserowitz, A. Support for climate policy and societal action are linked to perceptions about scientific agreement. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2011, 1, 462–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldberg, M.H.; Gustafson, A.; Ballew, M.T.; Rosenthal, S.A.; Leiserowitz, A. Identifying the most important predictors of support for climate policy in the United States. Behav. Public Policy 2021, 5, 480–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, N.; Leiserowitz, A. The role of emotion in global warming policy support and opposition. Risk Anal. 2014, 34, 937–948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marquart-Pyatt, S.T.; Qian, H.; Houser, M.K.; McCright, A.M. Climate change views, energy policy preferences, and intended actions across welfare state regimes: Evidence from the European Social Survey. Int. J. Sociol. 2019, 49, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bamberg, S.; Möser, G. Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: A new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 14–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klöckner, C.A. A comprehensive model of the psychology of environmental behaviour—A meta-analysis. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2013, 23, 1028–1038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barber, D. Bayesian Reasoning and Machine Learning; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Binder, J.; Koller, D.; Russell, S.; Kanazawa, K. Adaptive probabilistic networks with hidden variables. Mach. Learn. 1997, 29, 213–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cui, G.; Wong, M.L.; Lui, H.-K. Machine learning for direct marketing response models: Bayesian networks with evolutionary programming. Manag. Sci. 2006, 52, 597–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frey, B.J.; Brendan, J.F.; Frey, B.J. Graphical Models for Machine Learning and Digital Communication; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Kallbekken, S. Research on public support for climate policy instruments must broaden its scope. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2023, 13, 206–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hasanaj, V.; Stadelmann-Steffen, I. Is the problem or the solution riskier? Predictors of carbon tax policy support. Environ. Res. Commun. 2022, 4, 105001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levi, S. Why hate carbon taxes? Machine learning evidence on the roles of personal responsibility, trust, revenue recycling, and other factors across 23 European countries. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 2021, 73, 101883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Povitkina, M.; Jagers, S.C.; Matti, S.; Martinsson, J. Why are carbon taxes unfair? Disentangling public perceptions of fairness. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2021, 70, 102356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yates, J. Risk-Taking Behavior; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Visschers, V.; Wiedemann, P.M.; Gutscher, H.; Kurzenhäuser, S.; Seidl, R.; Jardine, C.; Timmermans, D. Affect-inducing risk communication: Current knowledge and future directions. J. Risk Res. 2012, 15, 257–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slovic, P.; Finucane, M.L.; Peters, E.; MacGregor, D.G. The affect heuristic. Eur. J. Oper. Res. 2007, 177, 1333–1352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loewenstein, G.F.; Weber, E.U.; Hsee, C.K.; Welch, N. Risk as feelings. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 127, 267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber, E.U. “Risk as feelings” and “perception matters”: Psychological contributions on risk, risk taking and risk management. In Future Risk Risk Management; University of Pennsylvania Press: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2018; pp. 30–47. [Google Scholar]
- Kasperson, R.E.; Renn, O.; Slovic, P.; Brown, H.S.; Emel, J.; Goble, R.; Kasperson, J.X.; Ratick, S. The social amplification of risk: A conceptual framework. Risk Anal. 1988, 8, 177–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van der Linden, S. The social-psychological determinants of climate change risk perceptions: Towards a comprehensive model. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 41, 112–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van der Linden, S. Determinants and measurement of climate change risk perception, worry, and concern. In The Oxford Encyclopedia of Climate Change Communication; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Ballew, M.T.; Leiserowitz, A.; Roser-Renouf, C.; Rosenthal, S.A.; Kotcher, J.E.; Marlon, J.R.; Lyon, E.; Goldberg, M.H.; Maibach, E.W. Climate change in the American mind: Data, tools, and trends. Environ. Sci. Policy Sustain. Dev. 2019, 61, 4–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Acock, A. Discovering Structural Equation Modeling Using Stata; Stata Press: College Station, TX, USA, 2013; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
- Ullman, J.B.; Bentler, P.M. Structural equation modeling. In Handbook of Psychology, 2nd ed.; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Wright, S. Correlation and causation. J. Agric. Res. 1921, 20, 557. [Google Scholar]
- Haavelmo, T. The statistical implications of a system of simultaneous equations. Econometrica 1943, 11, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simon, H.A. Notes on the observation and measurement of political power. J. Politics 1953, 15, 500–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pearl, J. Causality; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Epstein, S. Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. Am. Psychol. 1994, 49, 709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, D. Thinking, Fast and Slow; Macmillan: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Lagerkvist, C.J.; Okello, J.; Karanja, N. Consumers’ evaluation of volition, control, anticipated regret, and perceived food health risk. Evidence from a field experiment in a traditional vegetable market in Kenya. Food Control 2015, 47, 359–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tiedens, L.Z.; Linton, S. Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information processing. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 81, 973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber, M.; Weber, E.U.; Nosić, A. Who takes risks when and why: Determinants of changes in investor risk taking. Rev. Financ. 2013, 17, 847–883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferrer, R.A.; Klein, W.M. Risk perceptions and health behavior. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2015, 5, 85–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hornsey, M.J.; Harris, E.A.; Bain, P.G.; Fielding, K.S. Meta-analyses of the determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2016, 6, 622–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rickard, L.N.; Yang, Z.J.; Schuldt, J.P. Here and now, there and then: How “departure dates” influence climate change engagement. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2016, 38, 97–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, A.; Poortinga, W.; Pidgeon, N. The psychological distance of climate change. Risk Anal. Int. J. 2012, 32, 957–972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stokes, B.; Eike, R.; Carle, J. Global Concern about Climate Change, Broad Support for Limiting Emissions. Pew Research Centers Global Attitudes Project. 2015. Available online: https://www.pewresearch.org/global/2015/11/05/global-concern-about-climate-change-broad-support-for-limiting-emissions/ (accessed on 17 August 2024).
- Lerner, J.S.; Keltner, D. Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cogn. Emot. 2000, 14, 473–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellers, B.A.; McGraw, A.P. Anticipated emotions as guides to choice. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2001, 10, 210–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bechara, A.; Damasio, H.; Tranel, D.; Damasio, A.R. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 1997, 275, 1293–1295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strick, M.; Dijksterhuis, A.; Bos, M.W.; Sjoerdsma, A.; Van Baaren, R.B.; Nordgren, L.F. A meta-analysis on unconscious thought effects. Soc. Cogn. 2011, 29, 738–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Acker, F. New findings on unconscious versus conscious thought in decision making: Additional empirical data and meta-analysis. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2008, 3, 292–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slovic, P. What’s fear got to do with it-It’s affect we need to worry about. Mo. L. Rev. 2004, 69, 971. [Google Scholar]
Variable Name | Survey Question | Response Options | Obs | Mean | S.D. |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Public Opinion Statements | |||||
1. happening | Recently, you may have noticed that global warming has been getting some attention in the news. Global warming refers to the idea that the world’s average temperature has been increasing over the past 150 years, may be increasing more in the future, and that the world’s climate may change as a result. What do you think: Do you think that global warming is happening? | −1. Refused 1. No 2. Don’t know 3. Yes | 22,416 | 2.49 | 0.78 |
2. cause_recoded | Assuming global warming is happening, do you think it is... (Recoded to include open ends) | −1. Refused 1. Don’t know 2. Other 3. Neither because global warming isn’t happening 4. Caused mostly by natural changes in the environment 5. Caused by human activities and natural changes 6. Caused mostly by human activities | 22,416 | 4.97 | 1.21 |
3. sci_consensus | Which comes closest to your own view? | −1. Refused 1. Don’t know enough to say 2. There is a lot of disagreement among scientists about whether or not global warming is happening 3. Most scientists think global warming is not happening 4. Most scientists think global warming is happening | 21,086 | 2.74 | 1.21 |
4. worry | How worried are you about global warming? | −1. Refused 1. Not at all worried 2. Not very worried 3. Somewhat worried 4. Very worried | 22,416 | 2.53 | 0.97 |
5. harm_personally | How much do you think global warming will harm: You personally | −1. Refused 1. Don’t know 2. Not at all 3. Only a little 4. A moderate amount 5. A great deal | 22,416 | 1.99 | 1.20 |
6. harm_US | How much do you think global warming will harm: People in the United States | −1. Refused 1. Don’t know 2. Not at all 3. Only a little 4. A moderate amount 5. A great deal | 22,416 | 2.35 | 1.32 |
7. harm_dev_countries | How much do you think global warming will harm: People in developing countries | −1. Refused 1. Don’t know 2. Not at all 3. Only a little 4. A moderate amount 5. A great deal | 22,416 | 2.47 | 1.43 |
8. harm_future_gen | How much do you think global warming will harm: Future generations of people | −1. Refused 1. Don’t know 2. Not at all 3. Only a little 4. A moderate amount 5. A great deal | 22,416 | 2.75 | 1.46 |
9. harm_plants_animals | How much do you think global warming will harm: Plant and animal species | −1. Refused 1. Don’t know 2. Not at all 3. Only a little 4. A moderate amount 5. A great deal | 21,086 | 2.75 | 1.43 |
10. when_harm_US | When do you think global warming will start to harm people in the United States? | −1. Refused 1. Don’t know 2. Not at all 3. Only a little 4. A moderate amount 5. A great deal | 22,416 | 3.87 | 1.96 |
11. reg_CO2_pollutant | How much do you support or oppose the following policies? Regulate carbon dioxide (the primary greenhouse gas) as a pollutant. | −1. Refused 1. Strongly oppose 2. Somewhat oppose 3. Somewhat support 4. Strongly support | 21,406 | 2.84 | 1.09 |
12. reg_utilities | How much do you support or oppose the following policies? Require electric utilities to produce at least 20% of their electricity from wind, solar, or other renewable energy sources, even if it costs the average household an extra $100 a year. | −1. Refused 1. Strongly oppose 2. Somewhat oppose 3. Somewhat support 4. Strongly support | 17,390 | 2.61 | 1.16 |
13. fund_research | How much do you support or oppose the following policies? Fund more research into renewable energy sources, such as solar and wind power. | −1. Refused 1. Strongly oppose 2. Somewhat oppose 3. Somewhat support 4. Strongly support | 22,416 | 3.09 | 1.06 |
14. discuss_GW | How often do you discuss global warming with your family and friends? | −1. Refused 1. Never 2. Rarely 3. Occasionally 4. Often | 22,416 | 2.11 | 0.89 |
Sociodemographic variables | |||||
1. gender | Are you…? | 1. Male 2. Female | 22,416 | 1.51 | 0.49 |
2. age_category | How old are you? [recoded] | 1. 18–34 years 2. 35–54 years 3. 55+ years | 22,416 | 2.23 | 0.78 |
3. educ_category | What is the highest level of school you have completed? [recoded] | 1. Less than high school 2. High school 3. Some college 4. Bachelor’s degree or higher | 22,416 | 2.90 | 0.96 |
4. income_category | Responses to “income” were categorized into the following three groups. | 1. Less than $50,000 2. $50,000 to $99,999 3. $100,000 or more | 22,416 | 1.87 | 0.80 |
5. race | Responses to “race” were categorized into the following four groups. | 1. White, non-Hispanic 2. Black, non-Hispanic 3. Other, non-Hispanic 4. Hispanic | 22,416 | 1.51 | 0.98 |
6. ideology | In general, do you think of yourself as... | −1. Refused 1. Very liberal 2. Somewhat liberal 3. Moderate, middle of the road 4. Somewhat conservative 5. Very conservative | 22,416 | 3.04 | 1.20 |
7. party | Generally speaking, do you think of yourself as a... | −1. Refused 1. Republican 2. Democrat 3. Independent 4. Other; please specify: 5. No party/not interested in politics | 22,416 | 2.32 | 1.26 |
8. registered_voter | Are you currently registered to vote, or not | −1. Refused 1. Registered 2. Not registered 3. Not sure 4. Don’t know 5. Prefer not to answer | 22,416 | 1.24 | 0.82 |
9. region9 | Computed based on state of residence | 1. New England 2. Mid-Atlantic 3. East-North Central 4. West-North Central 5. South Atlantic 6. East-South Central 7. West-South Central 8. Mountain 9. Pacific | 22,416 | 6.06 | 5.25 |
10. religion | What is your religion? | −1. Refused 1. Baptist–any denomination 2. Protestant 3. Catholic 4. Mormon 5. Jewish 6. Muslim 7. Hindu 8. Buddhist 9. Pentecostal 10. Eastern Orthodox 11. Other Christian 12. Other–non-Christian 13. Agnostic 14. Atheist 15. None of the above | 22,416 | 6.06 | 5.25 |
11. evangelical | Would you describe yourself as “born-again” or evangelical? | −1. Refused 1. Yes 2. No 3. Don’t know | 22,416 | 1.79 | 0.64 |
12. service_attendance | How often do you attend religious services? | −1. Refused 1. Never 2. Once a year or less 3. A few times a year 4. Once or twice a month 5. Once a week 6. More than once a week | 22,416 | 3.08 | 1.80 |
13. marit_status | Are you now…? | 1. Married 2. Widowed 3. Divorced 4. Separated 5. Never married 6. Living with partner | 22,416 | 2.36 | 1.80 |
14. employment | Do any of the following currently describe you? | 1. Working—as a paid employee 2. Working—self-employed 3. Not working—on temporary layoff from a job 4. Not working—looking for work 5. Not working—retired 6. Not working—disabled 7. Not working—other | 22,416 | 2.93 | 2.18 |
15. house_head | Respondents were asked “Is your residence in…” with response options “Your name only”, “Your name with someone else’s name (jointly owned or rented)”, or “Someone else’s name only”. Respondents who said “Someone else’s name only” were coded as 0 = “Not head of household;” the other two responses were coded as 1 = “Head of household” | 1. Not head of household 2. Head of household | 22,416 | 1.83 | 0.38 |
16. house_size | How many people live in your household [recoded] | Open ended | 22,416 | 2.67 | 1.47 |
17. house_type | Which best describes the building where you live? | 1. One-family house detached from any other house 2. One-family house attached to one or more houses (such as a condo or townhouse) 3. Building with 2 or more apartments 4. Mobile home 5. Boat, RV, van, etc. | 22,416 | 1.53 | 0.92 |
18. house_own | Are your living quarters… | 1. Owned by you or someone in your household 2. Rented 3. Occupied without payment of rent | 22,416 | 1.27 | 0.49 |
19. year | Year of survey data collection | 1. 2008, 2. 2010, …. 10. 2018 | 22,416 | 5.72 | 2.88 |
weight_wave | Sampling weight specific to each wave | -- | 22,416 | 0.99 | 0.66 |
weight_aggregate | Sampling weight if aggregating multiple waves | -- | 22,416 | 0.99 | 0.71 |
Step Number | Description |
---|---|
Step 1 | Estimation of latent variables through unsupervised hierarchical Bayesian network clustering of respondent beliefs. |
Step 2 | Estimation of Bayesian network of latent variables that minimizes the description length. |
Step 3 | Linking latent variable PSEM with sociodemographic measured variables. |
Step 4 | Calibration and k-fold validation of PSEM with target variable as policy support. |
Recommended Goodness of the Fit (GoF) Value [47] | SEM#1 Standard SEM with ML Method & No Sampling Weights | SEM#2 Standard SEM with ML Method & Sampling Weights | SEM#3 Standard SEM with MLMV Method & No Sampling Weights | SEM#4 Standard SEM with MLMV Method & Sampling Weights | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sample Size | 16,380 | 16,380 | 22,416 | 22,416 | |
(1) Population error [Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation, RMSEA] | Less than 0.1 | 0.08 | RMSEA not reported due to model fit with vce (robust) | 0.08 | RMSEA not reported due to model fit with vce (robust) |
(2A) Baseline comparison [Comparative Fit Index, CFI] | Closer to 1 | 0.92 | CFI not reported due to model adding sampling weights | 0.92 | CFI not reported due to model adding sampling weights |
(2B) Baseline comparison [Tucker Lewis Index, TLI] | Closer to 1 | 0.90 | TLI not reported due to model adding sampling weights | 0.90 | TLI not reported due to model adding sampling weights |
(3) Size of residuals [Standardized Root Mean Squared Residual, SRMR] | Less than 0.08 | 0.06 | 0.06 | SRMR is not reported due to missing value treatment. | SRMR is not reported due to missing value treatment. |
(4) Size of residuals [Coefficient of determination, CD] | Less than 0.08 | 0.10 | 0.07 | 0.10 | 0.08 |
PSEM (N = 22,416) | SEM (N = 22,416) | |
---|---|---|
Affective Risk Perception | 0.41 (4221.64) | 0.53 (24.35) |
Analytical Risk Perception | 0.36 (4188.44) | 0.11 (11.64) |
Beliefs | 0.42 (6431.69) | 0.35 (37.76) |
Ideology | −0.19 (3361.94) | −0.05 (−4.39) |
Party | 0.06 (2142.29) | 0.02 (7.06) |
Race | 0.05 (56.49) | 0.02 (7.60) |
Variables and Their Categorical Class Values | Marginal (a Priori) Probability (%) | Conditional (Posterior) Probability for Strong Opposers (%) | Conditional (Posterior) Probability for Lukewarm Supporters (%) | Conditional (Posterior) Probability for Strong Supporters (%) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Policy Support | ||||
| 13.15 | 100.00 | ||
| 59.46 | 100.00 | ||
| 27.38 | 100.00 | ||
Beliefs | ||||
| 10.48 | 40.78 | 7.92 | 1.49 |
| 5.21 | 8.22 | 4.88 | 4.46 |
| 7.04 | 12.79 | 8.01 | 2.19 |
| 11.37 | 15.99 | 14.25 | 2.89 |
| 19.41 | 11.03 | 25.12 | 11.02 |
| 46.50 | 11.19 | 39.82 | 77.95 |
Affective Risk Perception | ||||
| 41.09 | 81.04 | 44.79 | 13.86 |
| 58.91 | 18.96 | 55.21 | 86.14 |
Analytical Risk Perception | ||||
| 12.77 | 19.70 | 13.54 | 7.77 |
| 15.94 | 47.13 | 14.71 | 3.62 |
| 18.73 | 15.95 | 22.06 | 12.82 |
| 30.46 | 11.63 | 30.92 | 38.51 |
| 22.11 | 5.59 | 18.77 | 37.28 |
Ideology | ||||
| 2.54 | 9.94 | 1.66 | 0.92 |
| 7.28 | 3.36 | 4.28 | 15.67 |
| 17.78 | 5.95 | 16.02 | 27.30 |
| 41.14 | 25.03 | 45.37 | 39.69 |
| 21.03 | 27.87 | 23.79 | 11.76 |
| 10.22 | 27.86 | 8.89 | 4.66 |
Party | ||||
| 1.34 | 4.63 | 0.96 | 0.57 |
| 24.56 | 38.23 | 25.61 | 15.72 |
| 34.13 | 18.22 | 32.15 | 46.07 |
| 23.35 | 20.41 | 24.13 | 23.05 |
| 2.48 | 3.86 | 2.42 | 1.94 |
| 14.15 | 14.66 | 14.73 | 12.65 |
Race | ||||
| 66.05 | 71.81 | 66.48 | 62.36 |
| 11.72 | 9.73 | 11.65 | 12.83 |
| 7.43 | 6.19 | 7.32 | 8.25 |
| 14.80 | 12.26 | 14.56 | 16.55 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zia, A.; Lacasse, K.; Fefferman, N.H.; Gross, L.J.; Beckage, B. Machine Learning a Probabilistic Structural Equation Model to Explain the Impact of Climate Risk Perceptions on Policy Support. Sustainability 2024, 16, 10292. https://doi.org/10.3390/su162310292
Zia A, Lacasse K, Fefferman NH, Gross LJ, Beckage B. Machine Learning a Probabilistic Structural Equation Model to Explain the Impact of Climate Risk Perceptions on Policy Support. Sustainability. 2024; 16(23):10292. https://doi.org/10.3390/su162310292
Chicago/Turabian StyleZia, Asim, Katherine Lacasse, Nina H. Fefferman, Louis J. Gross, and Brian Beckage. 2024. "Machine Learning a Probabilistic Structural Equation Model to Explain the Impact of Climate Risk Perceptions on Policy Support" Sustainability 16, no. 23: 10292. https://doi.org/10.3390/su162310292
APA StyleZia, A., Lacasse, K., Fefferman, N. H., Gross, L. J., & Beckage, B. (2024). Machine Learning a Probabilistic Structural Equation Model to Explain the Impact of Climate Risk Perceptions on Policy Support. Sustainability, 16(23), 10292. https://doi.org/10.3390/su162310292