Next Article in Journal
Use of Online Dietary Recalls among Older UK Adults: A Feasibility Study of an Online Dietary Assessment Tool
Previous Article in Journal
Considering Gender Differences in Portion Sizes to Improve the Accuracy of Nutrient Intakes from A Food Frequency Questionnaire
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Decreased Conflict Control in Overweight Chinese Females: Behavioral and Event-Related Potentials Evidence

1
Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality (Ministry of Education), Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
2
School of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing 400715, China
3
Department of Psychology, Wichita State University, Wichita, KS 67260, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2019, 11(7), 1450; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11071450
Submission received: 21 May 2019 / Revised: 19 June 2019 / Accepted: 23 June 2019 / Published: 27 June 2019

Abstract

:
Overweight or obesity is related to a decrease in cognitive control, especially conflict control. However, research on conflict control in overweight/obese individuals are still controversial. This study was conducted to explore general and food-related conflict control in overweight Chinese females (OWs) with a color–word Stroop task and a food-related conflict task. Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded during the food-related conflict task. Behavioral results showed that, OWs had a longer reaction time (RT) than normal-weight Chinese females (NWs), in both tasks. ERP results in the food-related conflict task showed that there was a reduction of N2 and N450 response strength in OWs, and the P3 and late positive component (LPC) response strength was enhanced. Results indicated that OWs might be less efficient in monitoring and resolving conflict, and OWs tended to have a higher motivational or emotional involvement in processing food-related stimuli, which was likely to contribute to their difficulty in losing weight.

1. Introduction

Obesity has become one of the most prevalent problems around the world and has posed a serious threat to public health [1,2]. In China, the rate of overweight and obese individuals has been increasing rapidly. China’s obese population (89.6 million) has surpassed that of the United States since 2014, ranking first in the word [3]. Obesity not only increases the risk of physical illnesses, such as type II diabetes, hypertension, hyperlipidemia, coronary heart disease, cancer, gallbladder disease, and stroke [4], but also the risk of mental health problems, such as age-related cognitive decline [5], and even neurodegenerative diseases, such as dementia [6]. Obesity is also likely to impair the normal functions of organs, such as the heart, the skeletal muscle, and the liver [7].
It is widely acknowledged that overeating plays an important role in leading to an imbalance between motivation and control/inhibition systems [8,9]. Therefore, overweight and obese individuals might be characterized by an enhanced motivation system and a decreased control system. Cognitive control, such as conflict control, has become a hot topic of research in overweight and obese individuals. Cognitive conflicts appear with exposure to different simultaneous characterizations that interfere with each other. Conflict control refers to monitoring and controlling conflicts that occur during the processing of information [10]. Color–word Stroop task uses the incongruence of color and word to produce conflict (e.g., “red” written in blue), which is usually used to measure the general conflict control. Research using color–word Stroop task found that overweight and obese individuals had poorer general conflict control performance than normal-weight individuals [11,12,13,14]. In addition, similar findings were found in other studies that applied Go/No-Go, Flanker, and Stop signal tasks to examine the general conflict control in overweight and obese individuals [15,16,17,18]. Go/No-Go task consists of Go stimuli and No-Go stimuli. Participants are asked to press a button as soon as possible during Go stimuli but to inhibit responses during No-Go stimuli. The procedure of Stop signal task is similar to Go/No-Go task, which consists of Go stimuli and Stop stimuli. Participants are asked to press a button as soon as possible during Go stimuli but to inhibit responses during Stop stimuli. The Flanker task is similar to the Stroop task, which consists of congruent and incongruent stimuli. A previous study using food-related Go/No-Go task found that overweight adolescents had a poorer food-related conflict control performance than normal-weight controls [19]. Moreover, a meta-analysis study found that overweight and obese individuals showed a decrease in conflict control, which did not differ in age and gender [20]. However, there are still some studies which failed to reveal an association between overweight/obesity and poorer conflict control. For instance, many studies using the Stroop task did not find the group difference between overweight/obesity and normal-weight individuals [21,22,23,24]. Therefore, it is necessary to provide further evidence for the relationship between overweight/obesity and conflict control.
Moreover, the neural mechanisms underlying the relationship between conflict control and overweight remains poorly understood. Chen et al. (2018) [17] explored the neural markers of general conflict control in both obese and normal-weight adolescents, and found that the obese adolescents had greater No-Go N2 amplitude compared to the Go N2 amplitude. Kamijo et al. (2012) [18] found that obese participants had a smaller error-related negativity (ERN) amplitude than normal weight participants, during a general Flanker task, which indicated that obese participants were characterized by a decreased ability of cognitive control. Nijs et al. (2010) [25] used food-related words Stroop task to explore the food-related conflict control in individuals with obesity, where participants were asked to ignore the content of the words and respond as quickly as possible to the font color of the words. They found that there was an enhanced P2 in obese participants, which might indicate that obese individuals displayed an enhanced, automatic, and preconscious attentional processing of food-related stimuli. Carbine et al. (2018) [26] examined the neural correlates of food-related inhibition among normal-weight, overweight, and obese adults in Go/No-Go tasks. The findings showed that No-Go trials elicited greater N2 amplitudes than Go trials in all participants. N2 is a negative potential occurring at about 200–350 ms after the onset of stimuli [26,27], reflecting sensory processing, conflict monitoring, and response inhibition, based on stimuli and tasks [27]. N450, which could be elicited by conflict information, is also a commonly used indicator in the study of conflict control. N450 is a negative component that appears 400–600 ms after stimuli presentation, reflecting conflict detection [28], conflict processing, and conflict adaptation [29]. N450 is likely to reflect activity of neural generators localized in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, which is involved in detection of conflict in performance or environment, as well as subsequent behavioral adjustment [30].
P3 is a positive potential occurring about 300–600 ms after the onset of stimuli [31]. P3 also reflects different cognitive processes based on tasks, such as working memory and attention allocation [32]. Overweight/obese children showed decreased P3 amplitudes during general Go/No-Go tasks, compared to normal-weight children, which suggested that overweight children had greater difficulties in processing cognitive demands [14]. Recently, some studies found in a Go/No-Go task, No-Go trials elicited a greater P3 than Go trials in both overweight/obese and normal-weight individuals [17,26]. Therefore, the group difference of P3 between overweight/obese and normal-weight individuals is still controversial and further investigation is needed. Meanwhile, a late positive component (LPC) is sensitive to the engagement of controlled cognitive resources, and it reflects higher-order cognitive processes, such as finer evaluation of stimulus meaning and response decisional processing [33]. Increased LPC reflects more resources of attention allocated to the stimuli processing, which involves motivation or emotion [34]. To our knowledge, LPC is less often applied to conflict control study in the overweight/obese. The present study aimed to examine both early and late processing of stimuli in overweight and normal-weight adults. Therefore, we explored not only the early event-related potentials (ERPs), but also the late ERPs.
The aim of the current study was to extend previous work on general and food-related conflict control by comparing both behavioral and neurocognitive correlates in the overweight and normal-weight Chinese females. We used a color–word Stroop and a food-related conflict task to explore general conflict control and food-related conflict control, respectively, and ERPs were recorded during the food-related conflict task. Behavioral and electrophysiological methods were used to assess aspects of conflict control in overweight Chinese females (OWs) and normal-weight Chinese females (NWs). Based on previous studies, we hypothesized that OWs might have a lower level of ability to detect conflicts, which is manifested by a longer reaction time (RT), lower accuracy (ACC), and decreased N2, N450; OWs might also allocate more cognitive resource to food-related stimuli, which is reflected by an enhanced P3, LPC.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Chinese female participants (N = 43) were recruited through campus advertisements at Southwest University in China. Only female adults were recruited because men and women differ in how and why they gain and lose their weight [35]. Participants with a BMI equal to or greater than 25 kg/m2 (M = 28.84, SD = 3.86) were included in the overweight group (N = 22), and those with a BMI between 18 and 22 kg/m2 (M = 20.81, SD = 1.49) were assigned to the normal weight group (N = 21). None of them reported having physical or psychiatric problems that might interfere with the current study. Before starting the experiment, all participants read the instructions and could ask questions about the experiment before signing the consent to participate. For standardization purposes, all participants were asked to abstain from consuming any food except for water, for at least four hours before the experiment [36,37]. The current study was approved by the Southwest University Ethics Committee.

2.2. Self-Report Measures

2.2.1. Hunger, Desire to Eat, and Body Satisfaction

Participants rated their hunger, desire to eat, and body satisfaction on a 100 mm visual-analog scale (VAS), ranging from “not at all” to “very much.”

2.2.2. Negative Physical Self Scale—Fatness Concern (NPS–F)

The NPS–F [38] is an 11-item questionnaire that assesses physical self-perception regarding fatness. Participants were asked to rate their responses to certain statements (e.g., “My weight has always been a pain in my heart”) on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 (never) to 4 (always). The original scale has a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.88; in the current study, NPS–F had a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.93.

2.2.3. Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire—Restraint Scale (DEBQ–R)

The DEBQ–R [39] is a 10-item scale that assesses dietary behaviors related to the loss and maintenance of weight (e.g., “Do you take into account your weight with what you eat?”). For each item, respondents rated their degree of restrained-eating on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 (never) to 5 (always). Higher DEBQ–R scores indicates more restrained-eating. The scale had a good internal consistency in general, with alphas coefficient ranging from 0.93 to 0.95. Its two-week test-retest reliability was 0.82. In the current study, the DEBQ–R had an alpha coefficient of 0.89.

2.3. Experimental Tasks

2.3.1. Color–Word Stroop Task

In the color–word Stroop task, stimulus was presented on a computer screen. Participants responded with the computer keyboard. The targets were the Chinese words ‘red’, ‘green’, ‘yellow’, and ‘blue’ displayed in the red, green, yellow, and blue color. Congruent (e.g., the Chinese word ‘red’ displayed in red) and incongruent (e.g., the Chinese word ‘red’ displayed in green, yellow, or blue) trials were produced by a combination of color words and color. Four keys on the computer keyboard were covered with different color patches; participants were told to press the key that corresponded to the color of the target as soon as possible. The task consisted of two runs. One run asked participants to response to the word, and the other one asked participants to response to color. Run order was counterbalanced across participants. Each run consisted of a practice block of 20 trials, followed by an experimental block of 60 trials. Each trial began with a fixation point presented for 500 ms in the center of the screen, after which the stimulus was presented on the screen until participants gave response. If participants did not respond, stimuli would automatically disappear after 2,000 ms. In order to eliminate the interaction between stimuli, as soon as possible, an inter-stimuli interval of 1,000 ms was presented (Figure 1).

2.3.2. Food-Related Conflict Task

The food-related conflict control task used in the current study was newly designed. In the task, the stimuli consisted of food pictures (including high- and low-calorie food) and the words ‘high-calorie’ or ‘low-calorie’. Congruent and incongruent trials were produced by a combination of food pictures and calorie information. For example, we combined high-calorie food pictures with the words ‘high-calorie’ to form the congruent trials. Food pictures used in the task were taken from the ones previously used in our studies [40,41]. Each trial began with a fixation point presented for 500 ms in the center of the screen, after which the stimuli were presented on the monitor until the participants gave a response. If participants did not respond, the stimuli would automatically disappear after 1,000 ms, followed by an inter-stimuli interval of 1,000 ms (Figure 2). The task consisted of a practice block of 20 trials, followed by an experimental block of 200 trials. During the task, participants were asked to press ‘F’ (or ‘J’) for the congruent trials and ‘J’ (or ‘F’) for the incongruent trials, and responses were counterbalanced across participants.
During the food-related conflict task, event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded from 64 scalp sites, using tin electrodes mounted on an elastic cap (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany), with the reference electrodes placed on FCz (frontocentral aspect) and a ground electrode on AFz (medial frontal aspect). The vertical electrooculogram (IO) was recorded with an electrode placed infra-orbitally at the right eye. All inter-electrode impedance was maintained below 5 KΩ.

2.4. Behavioral Analyses

Independent sample t-tests were conducted to identify between-group differences in age, BMI, hunger, desire to eat, body satisfaction, NPS–F, and DEBQ–R.
For the color–word Stroop task, 2 (group—overweight and normal weight) × 2 (condition—congruent and incongruent trials) × 2 (run—response to word and color) three-way ANOVAs was conducted on reaction time (RT) for correct responses and ACC, with group as a between-group factor, and condition and run as the within-group factors.
For the food-related conflict task, 2 (group—overweight and normal weight) × 2 (condition—congruent and incongruent trials) two-way ANOVAs was conducted on RT for correct responses and ACC, with group as a between-group factor and condition as a within-group factor.

2.5. ERPs Recording and Analyses

ERPs data processing was performed with EEGLAB [42], an open source toolbox running on the Matlab software. Individual and grand ERP averages were created for the food-related conflict task. We first downsampled the data from 1,000 Hz to 256 Hz and performed high-pass filtering at 0.1 Hz and low-pass filtering at 45 Hz. We selected the mean values of the left and right mastoids as the re-reference. Data were epoched from 200 ms prior to stimuli onset to 800 ms after presentation, and were baseline corrected to the pre-stimuli interval. Trials with large fluctuations in amplitudes were removed before independent component analysis (ICA). Then, the components including EOG artifacts (ocular movements and eye blinks) and head movement were removed from the results of ICA. Based on the topographical distribution of the grand-averaged ERP activities, the ERPs and their time epochs were as follows—N2 (300–360 ms), P3 (360–400 ms), N450 (400–450 ms), and LPC (500–800 ms). The following electrode sites were selected, frontal (F3, Fz, F4), frontal–central (FC3, FCz, FC4), central (C3, Cz, C4), central–parietal (CP3, CPz, CP4), and parietal (P3, Pz, P4). Four 2 (group—NWs, OWs) × 2 (condition—congruent and incongruent trials) × 5 (electrode site—frontal, frontocentral, central, central-parietal, and parietal) repeated measures ANOVAs were conducted on the mean amplitudes of N2, P3, N450, and LPC. All analyses were conducted by SPSS 22.0. p-value was computed for deviation in all analyses, based on the Greenhouse–Geisser method. Post-hoc t-tests were conducted with Bonferroni correction for multiple pairwise comparisons.

3. Results

3.1. Self-Report Results

Participants’ demographic information and self-report results are shown in Table 1. OWs had a significantly higher BMI than NWs, p < 0.01. The score of NPS–F in OWs was significantly higher than that in NWs, t = −2.27, p = 0.03, Cohen’s d = 0.69. The score of body satisfaction in OWs was significantly smaller than that in NWs, t = 4.46, p < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 1.36. There was no significant difference between groups in hunger, desire to eat, and DEBQ–R, all p > 0.05.

3.2. Behavioral Results

3.2.1. Color–Word Stroop Task

Repeated-measures ANOVAs on RT (Figure 3) found a main effect of condition, F (1, 41) = 280.19, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.87, with the post-hoc test indicating that RT of incongruent trials was longer than that of congruent trials; a main effect of run, F (1, 41) = 50.03, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.55, with post-hoc test suggesting that RT of the run where the correct response was to the word was smaller than the run where the correct response was to color; there was also a significant interaction between group and condition, F (1, 41) = 6.168, p = 0.017, η2 = 0.131, and a simple effect analysis showed that the RT of OWs was significantly longer than that of NWs in incongruent trials, p = 0.05.
Repeated-measures ANOVAs on ACC (Figure 4) found a main effect of condition, F (1, 41) = 86.74, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.68, and post-hoc test found that ACC of incongruent trials was smaller than that of congruent trials. However, we did not find the group difference between OWs and NWs.

3.2.2. Food-Related Conflict Task

Repeated-measures ANOVAs on RT (Figure 5) found a main effect of condition, F (1, 41) = 5.24, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.113, and a post-hoc test on condition found that the RT of incongruent trials was longer than that of congruent trials. Post-hoc test on group found that RT in OWs was significantly longer than that in NWs, p = 0.03.

3.3. ERPs Results

Grand average ERPs for N2, P3, N450, and LPC at Fz are shown in Figure 6.

3.3.1. N2

Results of repeated-measures ANOVAs on N2 showed a marginally significant main effect of condition, F (1, 41) = 3.83, p = 0.057, η2 = 0.06, and the post-hoc test on condition found that the N2 amplitude of incongruent trials was significantly greater than that of the congruent trials; Post-hoc test on group found that N2 amplitude in OWs was significantly lower than that in NWs, p = 0.01.

3.3.2. P3

Results of repeated-measures ANOVAs on P3 showed a significant interaction effect between group and electrode site, F (1, 41) = 4.09, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.09, and the simple effect analysis showed that the OWs elicited greater P3 in the frontal, frontocentral, and central regions than the NWs, all p < 0.05. Post-hoc test on group found that P3 amplitude of OWs was significantly greater than that of NWs, p = 0.02.

3.3.3. N450

Results on N450 found a main effect of condition, F (1, 41) = 10.04, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.20, and the post-hoc test found that the incongruent trials elicited a greater N450 than the congruent trials; an interaction effect of the group and electrode site was significant, F (1, 41) = 4.51, p = 0.028, η2 = 0.10, and a simple effect analysis found that the OWs elicited lower N450 than the NWs in the frontal and frontocentral regions.

3.3.4. LPC

Results on LPC found an interaction effect of group and electrode site, F (1, 41) = 4.92, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.11. A simple effect analysis found that LPC amplitude of OWs was significantly greater than that of NWs in the frontal, frontocentral, and central regions.

4. Discussion

In our novel examination of general and food-related conflict control in OWs and NWs, the findings partially supported our hypotheses. There was no difference in hunger, desire to eat, or DEBQ–R between OWs and NWs, which indicated that the difference in weight between the two groups was not influenced by these factors. Both in the general and food-related tasks, RT difference between the OWs and the NWs was found in the study. More importantly, evidence from ERPs also supported the behavioral results, which showed that N2 and N450 of OWs were significantly lower than those of NWs, but P3 and LPC of OWs were significantly greater than those of NWs.
Previous studies have shown that overweight/obese individuals have a longer RT to food-related words, which suggests that there is attentional bias towards food-related words in overweight/obese individuals [25,43]. In addition, researchers used a verbal and a spatial version of the Stroop task and found that there was group difference in the verbal Stroop task between the overweight and normal-weight individuals [44], which might suggest that the OWs’ ability to monitor verbal conflict is more easily damaged. In our study, RT in OWs was significantly longer than that in NWs, which suggested that conflict control was decreased in the OWs, compared to the NWs.
There was a reduction of N2 neural response strength among OWs. Folstein and Van (2008) [27] believed that N2 can be divided into three subcomponents—a frontocentral (anterior) N2 related to the detection of novelty or mismatch with a perceptual template when the eliciting stimuli are presented, a second frontocentral N2 related to cognitive control (including response inhibition, response conflict, and error monitoring), and a posterior N2 related to visual attention. N2 has been thought to reflect the detection and monitoring of response conflict between erroneous and correct responses [27,45]. The reduced N2 effect has also been attributed to a poor recruitment of neural resources [46]. Therefore, a speculative interpretation of N2 might suggest that OWs are less capable of recruiting attentional and neural resources to detect conflict over general and food-related stimuli or have less efficient conflict monitoring than NWs. In the conflict control studies, the function of N450 is similar to N2. N450 reflects conflict processing and conflict adaptation [28], which also represents the ability to inhibit and resolve conflict information [27]. In our task, when food stimuli arose, participants were asked to monitor and adapt food stimuli as soon as possible, and then respond differently to food stimuli. The reduction of N450 in OWs suggested that OWs were less capable of processing and resolving conflict stimuli.
P3 increases with task difficulty and effort [47], and it is closely related to the intensity of processing [48]. In addition, salient and self-relevant environmental cues also led to P3 enhancements [49]. Moreover, compared with neutral stimuli, food stimuli would elicit greater P3 [50], and high-calorie food would elicit greater P3 than low-calorie food [26], indicating that participants needed to allocate more resources to food stimuli, especially high-calorie food. Our results showed an enhanced P3 in OWs compared to NWs, which was inconsistent with a previous study [14], which found that overweight/obese children showed decreased P3 amplitudes. We believed that the difference might have resulted from experimental stimuli. OWs should allocate more cognitive resources to food-related stimuli. LPC is also related to the allocation of cognitive resources, but it reflects higher-order cognitive processes, such as evaluation of stimuli meaning and response decision processing [33]. Increased LPC reflects the allocation of more attentional resources in the processing of stimuli involving motivation or emotion [34]. Therefore, enhanced P3 and LPC in OWs could be interpreted in terms of the relative salience of the food stimuli that demand more cognitive resources, and OWs’ processing of food stimuli involves motivation or emotion.
In the current study, time windows of N2, P3, N450, and LPC were 300–800 ms. When food-related conflict stimuli appeared, N2 was responsible for monitoring the conflict stimuli, P3 was responsible for allocating cognitive resources for processing conflict stimuli, and N450 was responsible for adapting and resolving the conflict stimuli. Finally, LPC reflected late higher-order processing of stimuli involving motivation or emotion. To our knowledge, this was the first study investigating the general and food-related conflict control from the perspective of the course of time. It explored the dynamics of conflict control in OWs and NWs, over time. Both behavioral and ERP evidence indicated a decreased conflict control in OWs.
Conflict control is the first phase of cognitive control, and it is necessary for successful self-control [51,52]. The goal conflict model of eating suggests that the food intake of dieters is characterized by a conflict between two chronically accessible incentives or goals—eating enjoyment and weight control. Their difficulty in weight control is due to their behavioral sensitivity to eating enjoyment and its incompatibility with the eating control goal [53]. In the face of food temptation, successful dieters realize that food enjoyment is in conflict with the long-term weight control, and thus, they would show a weight-control goal. Therefore, the explorations of conflict control in OWs could provide a theoretical basis for intervention strategies targeted on conflict control. In addition, the explorations of ERPs in OWs can provide objective indicators of cognitive function improvement, after successful weight loss. For example, a previous study showed that after acute exercise, P3 amplitudes to food stimuli was significantly reduced, compared to those for non-food stimuli, in adolescents with obesity [54].
However, some limitations of the current study should be acknowledged. First, although the sample size was suitable for an ERP study, it was relatively small for a behavioral study, and we only recruited female adults. Future studies should recruit a larger sample (both females and males) to explore the conflict control so that the research conclusions could be better generalized. Second, the present study did not show a causal relationship between overweight and conflict control. Longitudinal studies could be designed to explore the causal relationship between overweight and conflict control. Third, we did not evaluate mental illness in the current study. Future studies could control the impact that mental illness might have on research conclusion. Moreover, the ERP method is not sufficient to understand the spatial dynamics of conflict control. Future studies can use simultaneous EEG–fMRI, which has the advantages of both EEG and fMRI, to explore both temporal and spatial neural responses.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the present study showed that OWs might be less efficient in monitoring and resolving conflict over food-related and food-unrelated stimuli and might allocate more cognitive resources to food-related stimuli. Moreover, OWs tend to have higher-order processing of food-related stimuli involving motivation or emotion, which is likely to contribute to their difficulty in losing weight. Potential directions for future research will be to explore the causal relationship between overweight and conflict control and to develop intervention strategies targeted on conflict control to address the issue of obesity.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Y.L. and H.C.; Methodology, Y.L. and H.C.; Software, Y.L.; Validation, Y.L., H.Q., S.S., X.Z., C.Y., and H.C.; Formal Analysis, Y.L. and S.S.; Investigation, Y.L.; Resources, H.C.; Data Curation, Y.L. and H.C.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, Y.L.; Writing—Review & Editing, H.Q., and H.C.; Visualization, X.Z., and C.Y.; Supervision, H.C.; Project Administration, H.C.; Funding Acquisition, H.C.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Chinese National Natural Science Foundation (NO. 31771237) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (SWU1709106).

Conflicts of Interest

All authors claim that there are no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Hesse, S.; Rullmann, M.; Luthardt, J.; Winter, K.; Hankir, M.K.; Becker, G.A.; Schinke, C. Central serotonin transporter availability in highly obese individuals compared with non-obese controls: A [11 C] DASB positron emission tomography study. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 2016, 43, 1096–1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Pi-Sunyer, F.X. The obesity epidemic: Pathophysiology and consequences of obesity. Obes. Res. 2012, 10, 97S–104S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. NCD Risk Factor Collaboration (NCD-RisC). Trends in adult body-mass index in 200 countries from 1975 to 2014: A pooled analysis of 1698 population-based measurement studies with 19.2 million participants. Lancet 2016, 387, 1377–1396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Friedemann, C.; Heneghan, C.; Mahtani, K.; Thompson, M.; Perera, R.; Ward, A.M. Cardiovascular disease risk in healthy children and its association with body mass index: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 2012, 345, e4759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Cournot, M.C.M.J.; Marquie, J.C.; Ansiau, D.; Martinaud, C.; Fonds, H.; Ferrieres, J.; Ruidavets, J.B. Relation between body mass index and cognitive function in healthy middle-aged men and women. Neurology 2006, 67, 1208–1214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Fitzpatrick, A.L.; Kuller, L.H.; Lopez, O.L.; Diehr, P.; O’Meara, E.S.; Longstreth, W.T.; Luchsinger, J.A. Midlife and late-life obesity and the risk of dementia: Cardiovascular health study. Arch. Neurol. 2009, 66, 336–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Karlsson, H.K.; Tuulari, J.J.; Hirvonen, J.; Lepomäki, V.; Parkkola, R.; Hiltunen, J.; Nuutila, P. Obesity is associated with white matter atrophy: A combined diffusion tensor imaging and voxel-based morphometric study. Obesity 2013, 21, 2530–2537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Volkow, N.; Wang, G.J.; Fowler, J.S.; Tomasi, D.; Baler, R. Food and Drug Reward: Overlapping Circuits in Human Obesity and Addiction. Brain Imaging in Behavioral Neuroscience; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2011; pp. 1–24. [Google Scholar]
  9. Volkow, N.D.; Wang, G.J.; Tomasi, D.; Baler, R.D. Obesity and addiction: Neurobiological overlaps. Obes. Rev. 2013, 14, 2–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Botvinick, M.M.; Braver, T.S.; Barch, D.M.; Carter, C.S.; Cohen, J.D. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 2001, 108, 624–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kittel, R.; Schmidt, R.; Hilbert, A. Executive functions in adolescents with binge-eating disorder and obesity. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2017, 50, 933–941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Restivo, M.R.; McKinnon, M.C.; Frey, B.N.; Hall, G.B.; Syed, W.; Taylor, V.H. The impact of obesity on neuropsychological functioning in adults with and without major depressive disorder. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0176898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Sweat, V.; Yates, K.F.; Migliaccio, R.; Convit, A. Obese adolescents show reduced cognitive processing speed compared with healthy weight peers. Child. Obes. 2017, 13, 190–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Reyes, S.; Peirano, P.; Peigneux, P.; Lozoff, B.; Algarin, C. Inhibitory control in otherwise healthy overweight 10-year-old children. Int. J. Obes. 2015, 39, 1230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Calvo, D.; Galioto, R.; Gunstad, J.; Spitznagel, M.B. Uncontrolled eating is associated with reduced executive functioning. Clin. Obes. 2014, 4, 172–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Chamberlain, S.R.; Derbyshire, K.L.; Leppink, E.; Grant, J.E. Obesity and dissociable forms of impulsivity in young adults. CNS Spectrums 2015, 20, 500–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Chen, S.; Jia, Y.; Woltering, S. Neural differences of inhibitory control between adolescents with obesity and their peers. Int. J. Obes. 2018, 42, 1753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kamijo, K.; Pontifex, M.B.; Khan, N.A.; Raine, L.B.; Scudder, M.R.; Drollette, E.S.; Hillman, C.H. The negative association of childhood obesity to cognitive control of action monitoring. Cereb. Cortex 2012, 24, 654–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Deux, N.; Schlarb, A.A.; Martin, F.; Holtmann, M.; Hebebrand, J.; Legenbauer, T. Overweight in adolescent, psychiatric inpatients: A problem of general or food-specific impulsivity? Appetite 2017, 112, 157–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Yang, Y.; Shields, G.S.; Guo, C.; Liu, Y. Executive function performance in obesity and overweight individuals: A meta-analysis and review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 84, 225–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Moreno-López, L.; Soriano-Mas, C.; Delgado-Rico, E.; Rio-Valle, J.S.; Verdejo-García, A. Brain structural correlates of reward sensitivity and impulsivity in adolescents with normal and excess weight. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e49185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Verdejo-García, A.; Pérez-Expósito, M.; Schmidt-Río-Valle, J.; Fernández-Serrano, M.J.; Cruz, F.; Pérez-García, M.; Campoy, C. Selective alterations within executive functions in adolescents with excess weight. Obesity 2010, 18, 1572–1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Wu, X.; Nussbaum, M.A.; Madigan, M.L. Executive function and measures of fall risk among people with obesity. Percept. Motor Skill. 2016, 122, 825–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Yau, P.L.; Kang, E.H.; Javier, D.C.; Convit, A. Preliminary evidence of cognitive and brain abnormalities in uncomplicated adolescent obesity. Obesity 2014, 22, 1865–1871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Nijs, I.M.; Franken, I.H.; Muris, P. Food-related Stroop interference in obese and normal-weight individuals: Behavioral and electrophysiological indices. Eat. Behav. 2010, 11, 258–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Carbine, K.A.; Duraccio, K.M.; Kirwan, C.B.; Muncy, N.M.; LeCheminant, J.D.; Larson, M.J. A direct comparison between ERP and fMRI measurements of food-related inhibitory control: Implications for BMI status and dietary intake. NeuroImage 2018, 166, 335–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Folstein, J.R.; Van Petten, C. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: A review. Psychophysiology 2008, 45, 152–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Qiu, J.; Luo, Y.; Wang, Q.; Zhang, F.; Zhang, Q. Brain mechanism of stroop interference effect in chinese characters. Brain Res. 2006, 1072, 186–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Xue, S.; Ren, G.; Kong, X.; Liu, J.; Qiu, J. Electrophysiological correlates related to the conflict adaptation effect in an emotional conflict task. Neurosci. Lett. 2015, 584, 219–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Silton, R.L.; Heller, W.; Towers, D.N.; Engels, A.S.; Spielberg, J.M.; Edgar, J.C.; Miller, G.A. The time course of activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex during top-down attentional control. NeuroImage 2010, 50, 1292–1302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Albert, J.; López-Martín, S.; Hinojosa, J.A.; Carretié, L. Spatiotemporal characterization of response inhibition. NeuroImage 2013, 76, 272–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Polich, J. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007, 118, 2128–2148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Hajcak, G.; Nieuwenhuis, S. Reappraisal modulates the electrocortical response to unpleasant pictures. Cognitive, Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2006, 6, 291–297. [Google Scholar]
  34. Hajcak, G.; MacNamara, A.; Foti, D.; Ferri, J.; Keil, A. The dynamic allocation of attention to emotion: Simultaneous and independent evidence from the late positive potential and steady state visual evoked potentials. Biol. Psychol. 2013, 92, 447–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Holm-Denoma, J.M.; Joiner, T.E., Jr.; Vohs, K.D.; Heatherton, T.F. The “freshman fifteen” (the “freshman five” actually): Predictors and possible explanations. Health Psychol. 2008, 27, S3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Wang, Y.; Dong, D.; Todd, J.; Du, J.; Yang, Z.; Lu, H.; Chen, H. Neural correlates of restrained eaters’ high susceptibility to food cues: An fMRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 2016, 631, 56–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Zhang, B.; Tian, D.; Yu, C.; Zhang, J.; Tian, X.; von Deneen, K.M.; Liu, Y. Altered baseline brain activities before food intake in obese men: A resting state fMRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 2015, 584, 156–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Chen, H.; Jackson, T.; Huang, X. The negative physical self scale: Initial development and validation in samples of Chinese adolescents and young adults. Body Image 2006, 3, 401–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Van Strien, T.; Frijters, J.E.; Van Staveren, W.A.; Defares, P.B.; Deurenberg, P. The predictive validity of the Dutch restrained eating scale. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 1986, 5, 747–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Zhou, Y.; Liu, Y.; Du, J.; Chen, H. Effects of food exposure on food-related inhibitory control in restrained eaters: An ERP study. Neurosci. Lett. 2018, 672, 130–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Kong, F.; Zhang, Y.; Chen, H. Inhibition ability of food cues between successful and unsuccessful restrained eaters: A two-choice oddball task. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0120522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Delorme, A.; Makeig, S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Meth. 2004, 134, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Janssen, L.K.; Duif, I.; van Loon, I.; Wegman, J.; de Vries, J.H.; Cools, R.; Aarts, E. Loss of lateral prefrontal cortex control in food-directed attention and goal-directed food choice in obesity. NeuroImage 2017, 146, 148–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Tarantino, V.; Vindigni, V.; Bassetto, F.; Pavan, C.; Vallesi, A. Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of cognitive control in ex-obese adults. Biol. Psychol. 2017, 127, 198–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Yeung, N.; Botvinick, M.M.; Cohen, J.D. The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 111, 931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Senderecka, M.; Grabowska, A.; Szewczyk, J.; Gerc, K.; Chmylak, R. Response inhibition of children with ADHD in the stop-signal task: An event-related potential study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2012, 85, 93–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Kok, A. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. Psychophysiology 2001, 38, 557–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Polich, J.; Kok, A. Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: An integrative review. Biol. Psychol. 1995, 41, 103–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Heinze, M.; Wölfling, K.; Grüsser, S.M. Cue-induced auditory evoked potentials in alcoholism. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007, 118, 856–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Watson, T.D.; Garvey, K.T. Neurocognitive correlates of processing food-related stimuli in a Go/No-go paradigm. Appetite 2013, 71, 40–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Matsumoto, K.; Tanaka, K. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in achieving goals. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2004, 14, 178–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Myrseth, K.O.R.; Fishbach, A. Self-control: A function of knowing when and how to exercise restraint. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 18, 247–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Stroebe, W.; Mensink, W.; Aarts, H.; Schut, H.; Kruglanski, A.W. Why dieters fail: Testing the goal conflict model of eating. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 26–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Fearnbach, S.N.; Silvert, L.; Pereira, B.; Boirie, Y.; Duclos, M.; Keller, K.L.; Thivel, D. Reduced neural responses to food cues might contribute to the anorexigenic effect of acute exercise observed in obese but not lean adolescents. Nutr. Res. 2017, 44, 76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Depiction of a congruent and an incongruent trial in the color–word Stroop task.
Figure 1. Depiction of a congruent and an incongruent trial in the color–word Stroop task.
Nutrients 11 01450 g001
Figure 2. Depicts examples of stimuli from the food-related conflict task.
Figure 2. Depicts examples of stimuli from the food-related conflict task.
Nutrients 11 01450 g002
Figure 3. Reaction Time (RT) difference between overweight Chinese females (OWs) and normal-weight Chinese females (NWs) in the color–word Stroop task (* p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001).
Figure 3. Reaction Time (RT) difference between overweight Chinese females (OWs) and normal-weight Chinese females (NWs) in the color–word Stroop task (* p < 0.05; *** p < 0.001).
Nutrients 11 01450 g003
Figure 4. Accuracy (ACC) difference between congruent and incongruent trials in the color–word Stroop task (*** p < 0.001).
Figure 4. Accuracy (ACC) difference between congruent and incongruent trials in the color–word Stroop task (*** p < 0.001).
Nutrients 11 01450 g004
Figure 5. RT difference between OWs and NWs in food-related conflict task (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01).
Figure 5. RT difference between OWs and NWs in food-related conflict task (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01).
Nutrients 11 01450 g005
Figure 6. Stimuli-locked, grand average waveforms of N2, P3, N450, and late positive component (LPC) at site Fz.
Figure 6. Stimuli-locked, grand average waveforms of N2, P3, N450, and late positive component (LPC) at site Fz.
Nutrients 11 01450 g006
Table 1. Demographic information and self-report results.
Table 1. Demographic information and self-report results.
VariableOWs (M ± SD)NWs (M ± SD)
n = 22n = 21
Age20.55 (2.06)20.00 (1.48)
BMI **28.84 (3.86)20.81 (1.49)
NPS-F *2.32 (1.04)1.56 (0.85)
DEBQ-R35.47 (6.5)34.07 (10.13)
Hunger53.64 (28.21)37.62 (24.01)
Desire49.77 (26.07)43.33 (26.52)
Body satisfaction **22.27 (18.56)50.95 (23.43)
Note: NPS–F: Negative Physical Self Scale–Fatness concern; DEBQ–R: Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire–Restraint Scale; NWs: Normal-weight adults; OWs: Overweight adults (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01).

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Liu, Y.; Quan, H.; Song, S.; Zhang, X.; Yang, C.; Chen, H. Decreased Conflict Control in Overweight Chinese Females: Behavioral and Event-Related Potentials Evidence. Nutrients 2019, 11, 1450. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11071450

AMA Style

Liu Y, Quan H, Song S, Zhang X, Yang C, Chen H. Decreased Conflict Control in Overweight Chinese Females: Behavioral and Event-Related Potentials Evidence. Nutrients. 2019; 11(7):1450. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11071450

Chicago/Turabian Style

Liu, Yong, Huan Quan, Shiqing Song, Xuemeng Zhang, Chao Yang, and Hong Chen. 2019. "Decreased Conflict Control in Overweight Chinese Females: Behavioral and Event-Related Potentials Evidence" Nutrients 11, no. 7: 1450. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11071450

APA Style

Liu, Y., Quan, H., Song, S., Zhang, X., Yang, C., & Chen, H. (2019). Decreased Conflict Control in Overweight Chinese Females: Behavioral and Event-Related Potentials Evidence. Nutrients, 11(7), 1450. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu11071450

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop