Characterizing Human–Dog Attachment Relationships in Foster and Shelter Environments as a Potential Mechanism for Achieving Mutual Wellbeing and Success
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Animal Subjects
2.2. Human Participants
2.3. Behavioral Tests and Surveys
2.3.1. Secure Base Test (SBT)
- Phase one (Baseline, two minutes): The familiar volunteer was asked to sit neutrally in a chair within the 1 m radius circle. The volunteers were permitted to interact freely (petting, talking, etc.) with the dog (without restraining it) each time the dog placed at least two paws in the circle, but were instructed to sit neutrally if the dog exited the circle. Dogs were able to freely explore the room. The volunteer could play with toys with the dog if the dog brought them to the volunteer inside the circle.
- Phase two (Alone, two minutes): The familiar volunteer or adopter exited the testing room so that the dog was left alone.
- Phase three (Return, two minutes): The familiar volunteer or adopter re-entered the testing room and the instructions were identical to phase 1 (baseline).
2.3.2. Video Analysis of SBT
2.3.3. Paired Attachment Test
- Phase 1 (two minutes): A two-minute alone period immediately following the SBT.
- Phase 2 (Passive, two-minute phase): The dog’s caretaker and a stranger sat neutrally for two minutes in chairs opposite each other surrounded by a 1m radius circle. Each individual was instructed to pet the dog twice each time it entered the circle with at least two paws, but were instructed to otherwise remain neutral.
- Phase 3 (Active, two minutes): Both humans were asked to call the dog and provide continuous petting and attention if the dog entered their circle with at least two paws.
2.3.4. Disinhibited Attachment Coding
2.4. Statistical Methods
2.4.1. Attachment Analysis
2.4.2. Paired Attachment Analysis
2.4.3. Disinhibited Attachment Analysis
2.4.4. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS) Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Attachment
3.2. Paired Attachment
3.3. Disinhibited Attachment
3.4. Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (LAPS)
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Dog’s Name | Group | Age | Mode of Intake | LOS | Breed |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Whisper | Foster | 8 years | Transfer | 32 | Mixed breed, medium |
Tilly | Foster | 5 years | Transfer | 46 | Mixed breed, small |
Jazzy | Foster | 1 year | Unknown | Unknown | Greyhound |
Jupiter | Foster | 2 years | Transfer | 16 | Mixed breed, medium |
Maddie | Foster | 4 months | Surrender | 43 | Mixed breed, small |
Opal | Foster | 2 years | Transfer | 154 | Mixed breed, medium |
Ginger | Foster | 2 years | Transfer | 57 | Mixed breed, medium |
Hera | Foster | 5 years | Transfer | 70 | Mixed breed, medium |
Helios | Foster | 2 months | Transfer | 62 | Mixed breed, medium |
Mackenna | Foster | Unknown | Unknown | Unknown | Mixed breed, medium |
Sierra | Foster | Unknown | Unknown | Unknown | Mixed breed, medium |
Brisa | Foster | 8 years | Surrender | Unknown | Mixed breed, large |
Skipper | Foster | 5 months | Transfer | 52 | Mixed breed, medium |
Remy | Foster | 1 year | Unknown | 120+ (still available for adoption) | Mixed breed, medium |
Lux | Foster | 2 years | Unknown | Unknown | Greyhound |
Lillie | Foster | 13 years | Unknown | Unknown | Mixed breed, small |
Dr. Zeuss | Foster | 4 years | Stray | 107 | Mixed breed, large |
Panda | Foster | 4 years | Unknown | Unknown (still available) | Mixed breed, medium |
Bella | Foster | 8 years | Unknown | Unknown (still available) | Pomeranian |
Nellie | Foster | 9 years | Stray | 23 | Shih Tzu |
Stormy | Foster | Unknown | Unknown | unknown | Mixed breed, medium |
Bandit | Shelter | 2 years | Surrender | 87 | Mixed breed, large |
Biscay | Shelter | 2 years | Transfer | 147 | Mixed breed, medium |
Brownie | Shelter | 5 years | Stray | 30 | Mixed breed, large |
Charlie | Shelter | 5 years | Surrender | 134 | Mixed breed, large |
Cheyenne | Shelter | 8 months | Transfer | 8 | Mixed breed, medium |
Chico | Shelter | 5 years | Surrender | 31 | Mixed breed, large |
Clooney | Shelter | 1.5 years | Stray | 19 | Mixed breed, large |
Dakota | Shelter | 5 years | Surrender | 83 | Mixed breed, large |
Floki | Shelter | 1 year | Surrender | 69 | Mixed breed, large |
Gemma | Shelter | 2 years | Surrender | 35 | Mixed breed, large |
Hoagie | Shelter | 3.5 years | Transfer | 427 | Mixed breed, large |
Hunny | Shelter | 7 years | Surrender | 32 | Mixed breed, small |
Jordy | Shelter | 1 year | Transfer | 27 | Mixed breed, small |
Luna | Shelter | 3 years | Surrender | 49 | Mixed breed, large |
Maizie | Shelter | 5 years | Transfer | 6 | Mixed breed, small |
Lincoln | Shelter | 1 year | Transfer | 200 | Mixed breed, large |
Mari | Shelter | 2 years | Surrender | 159 | Mixed breed, large |
Numair | Shelter | 7 years | Surrender | 64 | Mixed breed, small |
Smallz | Shelter | 10 years | Transfer | 23 | Mixed breed, small |
Jacob | Shelter | 6.5 years | Surrender | 48 | Mixed breed, small |
Elfie | Shelter | 4 years | Surrender | 141 | Mixed breed, large |
Champ | Shelter | 5 years | Surrender | 85 | Mixed breed, large |
Daisy | Shelter | 7 years | Surrender | 39 | Mixed breed, medium |
Molly | Shelter | 3 years | Surrender | 69 | Mixed breed, medium |
Hans | Shelter | 3.5 years | Surrender | 40 | Mixed breed, large |
Heidi | Shelter | 1.5 years | Transfer | 126 | Mixed breed, large |
Charlie Boy | Shelter | 3.5 years | Surrender | 57 | Mixed breed, small |
Brinx | Shelter | 4.5 years | Surrender | 177 | Mixed breed, medium |
Peaches | Shelter | 1.5 years | Surrender | 38 | Mixed breed, large |
Carly | Shelter | 4 years | Transfer | 200 | Retriever mix |
Starlord | Shelter | 1 year | Stray | 39 | Mixed breed, small |
References
- Parthasarathy, V.; Crowell-Davis, S.L. Relationship between attachment to owners and separation anxiety in pet dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2006, 1, 109–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Prato-Previde, E.; Custance, D.M.; Spiezio, C.; Sabatini, F. Is the dog-human relationship an attachment bond? An observational study using Ainsworth & aposs strange situation. Behaviour 2003, 140, 225–254. [Google Scholar]
- Topál, J.; Miklósi, A.; Csányi, V.; Dóka, A. Attachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): A new application of Ainsworth’s (1969) Strange Situation Test. J. Comp. Psychol. 1998, 112, 219–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gácsi, M.; Topál, J.; Miklósi, Á.; Dóka, A.; Csányi, V. Attachment behavior of adult dogs (Canis familiaris) living at rescue centers: Forming new bonds. J. Comp. Psychol. 2001, 115, 423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schöberl, I.; Beetz, A.; Solomon, J.; Wedl, M.; Gee, N.; Kotrschal, K. Social factors influencing cortisol modulation in dogs during a strange situation procedure. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2016, 11, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Thielke, L.E.; Rosenlicht, G.; Saturn, S.R.; Udell, M.A.R. Nasally-Administered Oxytocin Has Limited Effects on Owner-Directed Attachment Behavior in Pet Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Front. Psychol. 2017, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wanser, S.H.; Udell, M.A. Does attachment security to a human handler influence the behavior of dogs who engage in animal assisted activities? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 210, 88–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ainsworth, M.D.S.; Bell, S.M. Attachment, Exploration, and Separation: Illustrated by the Behavior of One-Year-Olds in a Strange Situation. Child. Dev. 1970, 41, 49–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rutter, M.; Colvert, E.; Kreppner, J.; Beckett, C.; Castle, J.; Groothues, C.; Hawkins, A.; O’Connor, T.G.; Stevens, S.E.; Sonuga-Barke, E.J.S. Early adolescent outcomes for institutionally-deprived and non-deprived adoptees. I: Disinhibited attachment. J. Child. Psychol Psychiatry 2007, 48, 17–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mertens, P.A.; Unshelm, J. Effects of Group and Individual Housing on the Behavior of Kennelled Dogs in Animal Shelters. Anthrozoös 1996, 9, 40–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mohan-Gibbons, H.; Weiss, E.; Slater, M. Preliminary Investigation of Food Guarding Behavior in Shelter Dogs in the United States. Animals 2012, 2, 331–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Coppola, C.L.; Grandin, T.; Enns, R.M. Human interaction and cortisol: can human contact reduce stress for shelter dogs? Physiol. Behav. 2006, 87, 537–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Udell, M.A.R.; Dorey, N.R.; Wynne, C.D.L. The performance of stray dogs (Canis familiaris) living in a shelter on human-guided object-choice tasks. Anim. Behav. 2010, 79, 717–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Protopopova, A. Effects of sheltering on physiology, immune function, behavior, and the welfare of dogs. Physiol. Behav. 2016, 159, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bergamasco, L.; Osella, M.C.; Savarino, P.; Larosa, G.; Ozella, L.; Manassero, M.; Badino, P.; Odore, R.; Barbero, R.; Re, G. Heart rate variability and saliva cortisol assessment in shelter dog: Human–animal interaction effects. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 125, 56–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hennessy, M.B.; Morris, A.; Linden, F. Evaluation of the effects of a socialization program in a prison on behavior and pituitary–adrenal hormone levels of shelter dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2006, 99, 157–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shiverdecker, M.D.; Schiml, P.A.; Hennessy, M.B. Human interaction moderates plasma cortisol and behavioral responses of dogs to shelter housing. Physiol. Behav. 2013, 109, 75–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willen, R.M.; Mutwill, A.; MacDonald, L.J.; Schiml, P.A.; Hennessy, M.B. Factors determining the effects of human interaction on the cortisol levels of shelter dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 186, 41–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Horn, L.; Huber, L.; Range, F. The importance of the secure base effect for domestic dogs—Evidence from a manipulative problem-solving task. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e65296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bernier, A.; Beauchamp, M.H.; Carlson, S.M.; Lalonde, G. A secure base from which to regulate: Attachment security in toddlerhood as a predictor of executive functioning at school entry. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 51, 1177–1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smyke, A.T.; Zeanah, C.H.; Fox, N.A.; Nelson, C.A.; Guthrie, D. Placement in Foster Care Enhances Quality of Attachment Among Young Institutionalized Children. Child Dev. 2010, 81, 212–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, T.P.; Garrity, T.F.; Stallones, L. Psychometric Evaluation of the Lexington Attachment to Pets Scale (Laps). Anthrozoös 1992, 5, 160–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stovall, K.C.; Dozier, M. Infants in Foster Care. Adopt. Q. 1998, 2, 55–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harlow, H.F. The nature of love. Am. Psychol. 1958, 13, 673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Waters, E. The Reliability and Stability of Individual Differences in Infant-Mother Attachment. Child Dev. 1978, 49, 483–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rehn, T.; McGowan, R.T.S.; Keeling, L.J. Evaluating the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) to Assess the Bond between Dogs and Humans. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e56938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Protopopova, A.; Wynne, C.D.L. Adopter-dog interactions at the shelter: Behavioral and contextual predictors of adoption. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 157, 109–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Konok, V.; Kosztolányi, A.; Rainer, W.; Mutschler, B.; Halsband, U.; Miklósi, Á. Influence of Owners’ Attachment Style and Personality on Their Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) Separation-Related Disorder. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0118375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Voith, V.L.; Ingram, E.; Mitsouras, K.; Irizarry, K. Comparison of Adoption Agency Breed Identification and DNA Breed Identification of Dogs. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2009, 12, 253–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Attachment Style | Definition |
---|---|
Secure | Little or no resistance to contact or interaction. Greeting behavior is active, open and positive. Seeks proximity and is comforted upon reunion, returning to exploration or play. |
Insecure ambivalent | Shows exaggerated proximity-seeking and clinging behavior, but may struggle if held by familiar volunteer. Mixed persistent distress with efforts to maintain physical contact and/or physically intrusive behavior directed toward the familiar volunteer. (Dogs who the judges agreed seemed essentially secure but with insecure ambivalent tendencies, were included in the secure group). |
Insecure avoidant | May show little/no distress on departure. Little/no visible response to return, ignores/turns away but may not resist interaction altogether (e.g., rests or stands without bodily contact, out of reach or at a distance). |
Insecure disorganized | Evidence of strong approach avoidance conflict or fear on reunion, for example, circling familiar volunteer, hiding from sight, rapidly dashing away on reunion, “aimless” wandering around the room. May show stereotypies on return (e.g., freezing or compulsive grooming). Lack of coherent strategy shown by contradictory behavior. “Dissociation” may be observed, that is, staring into space without apparent cause; still or frozen posture for at least 20 s (in the nonresting, nonsleeping dog). |
Behavior | Definition |
---|---|
Proximity seeking | Proportion of the episode in which the dog had at least 2 paws (or half their body) within the 1 m radius circle the human was sitting in. |
Dog-human contact | Proportion of the episode in which the dog or human engaged in physical contact with the other individual. Contact must be in circle to count. Sniffing and body touches count as contact. |
Measure | Percent Agreement between Two Coders |
---|---|
Familiar human: proximity seeking (duration) | 95.3% |
Familiar human: contact (duration) | 87.5% |
Unfamiliar human: proximity seeking (duration) | 100% |
Unfamiliar human: contact (duration) | 92.2% |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Thielke, L.E.; Udell, M.A.R. Characterizing Human–Dog Attachment Relationships in Foster and Shelter Environments as a Potential Mechanism for Achieving Mutual Wellbeing and Success. Animals 2020, 10, 67. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10010067
Thielke LE, Udell MAR. Characterizing Human–Dog Attachment Relationships in Foster and Shelter Environments as a Potential Mechanism for Achieving Mutual Wellbeing and Success. Animals. 2020; 10(1):67. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10010067
Chicago/Turabian StyleThielke, Lauren E., and Monique A.R. Udell. 2020. "Characterizing Human–Dog Attachment Relationships in Foster and Shelter Environments as a Potential Mechanism for Achieving Mutual Wellbeing and Success" Animals 10, no. 1: 67. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10010067
APA StyleThielke, L. E., & Udell, M. A. R. (2020). Characterizing Human–Dog Attachment Relationships in Foster and Shelter Environments as a Potential Mechanism for Achieving Mutual Wellbeing and Success. Animals, 10(1), 67. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10010067