Next Article in Journal
Potential of Biological Processes to Eliminate Antibiotics in Livestock Manure: An Overview
Next Article in Special Issue
Cows Come Down from the Mountains before the (Mw = 6.1) Earthquake Colfiorito in September 1997; A Single Case Study
Previous Article in Journal
Environmental and Anthropogenic Impacts on Avifaunal Assemblages in an Urban Parkland, 1976 to 2007
Previous Article in Special Issue
Frog Swarms: Earthquake Precursors or False Alarms?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Unusual Animal Behavior Preceding the 2011 Earthquake off the Pacific Coast of Tohoku, Japan: A Way to Predict the Approach of Large Earthquakes

1
Department of Animal Science and Biotechnology, Azabu University Graduate School of Veterinary Science, 1-17-71 Fuchinobe, Chuo-ku, Sagamihara, Kanagawa, 252-5201, Japan
2
Department of Human and Animal-Plant Relationships, Tokyo University of Agriculture, 1737 Funako, Atsugi, Kanagawa 243-0034, Japan
3
Department of Animal Science and Biotechnology, Azabu University School of Veterinary Science, 1-17-71 Fuchinobe, Chuo-ku, Sagamihara, Kanagawa, 252-5201, Japan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2014, 4(2), 131-145; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4020131
Submission received: 3 February 2014 / Revised: 26 March 2014 / Accepted: 28 March 2014 / Published: 3 April 2014
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Biological Anomalies Prior to Earthquakes)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Large earthquakes (EQs) cause severe damage to property and people. They occur abruptly, and it is difficult to predict their time, location, and magnitude. However, there are reports of abnormal changes occurring in various natural systems prior to EQs. Unusual animal behaviors (UABs) are important phenomena. These UABs could be useful for predicting EQs, although their reliability has remained uncertain yet. We report on changes in particular animal species preceding a large EQ to improve the research on predicting EQs.

Abstract

Unusual animal behaviors (UABs) have been observed before large earthquakes (EQs), however, their mechanisms are unclear. While information on UABs has been gathered after many EQs, few studies have focused on the ratio of emerged UABs or specific behaviors prior to EQs. On 11 March 2011, an EQ (Mw 9.0) occurred in Japan, which took about twenty thousand lives together with missing and killed persons. We surveyed UABs of pets preceding this EQ using a questionnaire. Additionally, we explored whether dairy cow milk yields varied before this EQ in particular locations. In the results, 236 of 1,259 dog owners and 115 of 703 cat owners observed UABs in their pets, with restless behavior being the most prominent change in both species. Most UABs occurred within one day of the EQ. The UABs showed a precursory relationship with epicentral distance. Interestingly, cow milk yields in a milking facility within 340 km of the epicenter decreased significantly about one week before the EQ. However, cows in facilities farther away showed no significant decreases. Since both the pets’ behavior and the dairy cows’ milk yields were affected prior to the EQ, with careful observation they could contribute to EQ predictions.

1. Introduction

Many countries including, most recently, Japan, have suffered extensive damage due to earthquakes (EQs). On 11 March 2011, the EQ (Mw 9.0) occurred in Japan, which took about twenty thousand lives together with missing and killed persons. There are several studies on the short-term prediction of EQ occurrence [1,2,3]. Those studies have mainly focused on pre-seismic unusual physical and/or chemical variations near the epicenters, such as electromagnetic signals, ionospheric propagations, and radon gasses, emerging prior to EQs [1,2,3]. These anomalies have often been observed within a few weeks before EQs.
Additionally, posteriori surveys concerning anomalous phenomena have been conducted. Macroscopic anomalies include unusual animal behaviors (UABs), abnormal sounds [4], EQ lights [5], EQ clouds [6], ground deformation [4], and abnormalities in the ground water [7]. Wadatsumi [8] investigated macroscopic anomalies preceding the Kobe EQ in Japan, on 17 January 1995, and found that the anomalous phenomena associated with animals constituted more than half of the total reported (872/1519). A similar ratio of UABs to the total macroscopic anomalies was reported after another EQ [9]. Many UABs had been observed within a week before the EQs [10]. The locomotive activities of mice drastically increased one day prior to the Kobe EQ [11], while the circadian rhythm of mice locomotion disappeared in the days before the Wenchuan EQ in China (12 May 2008, M = 8.0) [12]. Rikitake [13] noted that small animals and insects showed UABs first and then larger animals (birds, rodents and mammals) up to the hour before the EQ.
The UABs of dogs and cats were observed most frequently within the 24 hours prior to the Kobe EQ [8]. These behaviors included “barking loudly”, “being panicked”, or “biting owners” in dogs, and “hiding”, “being restless”, “meowing pathetically”, “taking the kitten outside”, “climbing a high tree”, or “disappearing” in cats [14].
The hearing range is 67 to 44,000 Hz in dogs, 55 to 79,000 Hz in cats, and 31 to 17,000 Hz in humans [15]. Thus, dogs and cats can hear ultrasounds that humans cannot. The number of smell receptors, olfactory cells, of dogs and cats are over an order of magnitude more numerous when compared with the 12 million of humans [16]. The olfactory bulb contains approximately 280 million cells in dogs, 67 million in cats, and 5 to 20 million in humans [17,18]. Humans can detect odor concentrations from 10−4.5 molar (M) to 10−5.0 M. Remarkably, dogs can detect a concentration of 10−17 M [19,20,21]. With these superior senses, dogs and cats show a greater sensitivity to small changes in smell and/or sound in their environment than humans. The occurrence of behaviors is incomplete without stimuli. For UABs prior to EQs, possible candidate stimuli include changes in atmospheric pressure, changes in gravity, ground deformation (ground uplift and tilt changes), acoustic signals and vibrations due to the generation of micro cracks, ground water level changes, and emanations of gases and chemical substances [22]. With their extraordinary sensory abilities, it is possible that animals sense and respond to such stimuli. Animals might show UABs as results of which they felt anxiety for EQ precursors as described by Lott et al. [23]. In addition, there are some considerable theoretical and experimental evidences that animals may be responding to some physical or chemical anomalies prior to EQs; (1) massive amounts of positive airborne ions which cause changes of the stress hormone concentration in animals and humans as described by Grant et al. [24], Freund [25], and Freund and Stolc [26]; (2) large amounts of toxic gasses such as the carbon monoxide, which is odorless but deadly [26,27]; (3) electromagnetic anomalies such as ultralow frequency and extremely low frequency field which cause physiological effects [22,26]. However, the mechanism by which these stimuli are sensed remains undetermined. Another controversial point is the anecdotal and retrospective nature of the reports regarding UABs prior to EQs. At present; however, the best course of action is to collect as much information on unusual physical and biological phenomena before EQs as possible.
The milk yield of dairy cows, although it is not a behavior, could be useful as an EQ predictor because it is often measured daily by instruments in the animal industry. In addition, milk yields are decreased by various stressors [28,29]. There are also reports that cows showed UABs prior to EQs [30,31]. If cows feel unusual physical or chemical variations prior to EQs, then milk yields could decrease. It may help to reveal the relationship between the seismic activities and UABs prior to EQs. The information on UABs prior to EQs could assist in predicting the next EQ occurrence. The aim of this study is to categorize the information on UABs prior to the massive 2011 Tohoku EQ in Japan and analyze the relationship between the information and seismic characteristics.

2. Material and Methods

2.1. Seismic Data

On 11 March 2011, at 14:46 JST (05:46 UTC), a megathrust EQ with Mw 9.0 occurred in the Northwestern Pacific Ocean (138.104°N, 142.861°E) at a shallow depth of 24 km, which is formally named as “the 2011 off the Pacific coast of Tohoku Earthquake” (Tohoku EQ, Figure 1). This EQ occurred on or near the subduction zone plate boundary between the Pacific and North America plates. There was a foreshock of Mw 7.3 on 9 March 2011, at 11:45 JST (02:45 UTC), two days before the main shock.

2.2. UABs of Pets

The web survey, in association with Iris Ohyama Inc. (Miyagi, Japan), collected a large amount of data from across the nation from 6 December 2011, to 19 January 2012. The survey’s campaign website was entitled “The survey associated with the EQ and the pet” (managed by Iris Ohyama Inc.) and was limited to registered members of the pets’ informational-website of Iris Ohyama Inc., that had dogs and/or cats. The questionnaire contained three sections: general information about the pets, their living environments, and the UABs. The general information consisted of species (dog or cat), sex, gonadal status, and age. The questions about the living environment included the owner’s address and where the pets had been reared. To protect personal information, only the post code of the owner’s address was used. This still enabled us to analyze the relationship between the distance from the epicenter and the UABs. The question regarding the pet’s place of rearing had four options: “only indoor”, “mainly indoor”, “only outdoor”, and “mainly outdoor”. Respondents were asked whether they observed UABs preceding the Tohoku EQ in their owned pets. The questions relevant to UABs were composed of 15 behavioral types based on previous studies of UABs [8,14] and emotional responses (stress, anxiety, and fearfulness) [32,33,34]. The detailed contents of these behavioral types are shown in Table 1. This question allowed multiple answers. If the respondents had observed particular types of UABs, then they were asked when they were observed. This question consisted of six terms: “from a few seconds to minutes”, “from 1 to a few hours”, “1 day”, “from 2 to 3 days”, “from 4 to 5 days”, and “6 or more days” before the EQ. If respondents owned more than one pet, then they were asked about only the pet with whom they had the closest relationship. All procedures pertaining to personal information in this study were in accordance with the guidelines supplied by the Human Research Ethics Committee of Azabu University.
Table 1. Types of unusual animal behaviors
Table 1. Types of unusual animal behaviors
AbbreviationsBehavior types
1EscapedAnimals escaped their normal home environment (home or yard)
2FrightenedAnimals were frightened by something
3Barked (dog)Dogs barked more than usual
4Howled (dog)Dogs howled more than usual
5Vocalized (cat)Cats vocalized more than usual
6RestlessAnimals were restless
7TremblingAnimal’s bodies were trembling
8Shaking offAnimals were shaking off a lot
9HidingAnimals were in hiding somewhere
10Different placeAnimals wanted to be in a different place than usual
11No appetiteAnimals had no appetite
12DiarrheaAnimals had diarrhea
13VomitedAnimals vomited
14StuckAnimals stuck close to the owner
15AggressiveAnimals became aggressive
16OtherOther

2.3. Milk Yields of Cows

With the aid of the institutes of animal industry in Kanagawa (Livestock Industry Technology Station; Kanagawa Agricultural Technology Center), in Shizuoka (Shizuoka Prefectural Research Institute of Animal Industry), and in Ibaraki Prefecture (National Agriculture and Food Research Organization) of Japan, we collected the daily milk yields of 86 Holstein cows from 1 January 2011, to 31 March 2011 (Figure 1). The population of each milking facility, which is a typical research center of the livestock industry in Japan, was 20 to 41 animals. The milking process was similar in all the facilities, with the cows being induced to the milking parlor and then milked by machines. They were individually identified by tags, and milk yields were counted using electronic milk meters. The milking frequency was twice a day in all of the facilities. The data measured were transferred to computers, and we used the total milk yield per cow per day. These measures are taken every day, not just in connection with this study. Milk yields from dairy cows increase for approximately four to eight weeks postpartum, and gradually decrease thereafter, with lactation being complete by approximately 40 weeks. Thus, it was necessary to remove the known factors affecting milk yields, such as the number of days after calving and environmental temperature and humidity, to verify the relevance of the EQ.
Figure 1. The epicenter of the 2011 earthquake off the Pacific coast of Tohoku, Japan. Black circles represent the approximate locations of three institutes with dairy cow milking facilities used in this study. The lower table shows the distances from the earthquake’s epicenter and approximate intensity on the Japan Meteorological Agency (JMA) seismic intensity scale of the three institutes.
Figure 1. The epicenter of the 2011 earthquake off the Pacific coast of Tohoku, Japan. Black circles represent the approximate locations of three institutes with dairy cow milking facilities used in this study. The lower table shows the distances from the earthquake’s epicenter and approximate intensity on the Japan Meteorological Agency (JMA) seismic intensity scale of the three institutes.
Animals 04 00131 g001

2.4. Statistical Analysis

The clear range of epicentral distance which animals show UABs is undetermined. Chen et al. [35] found out that there was surface displacement which covered entire Japan and agreed with the fault mechanism since about 80 days before the Tohoku EQ, by using the daily data from 1,243 Global Positioning System stations, although we do not know what triggers the UABs prior to EQs. Therefore, we used reports in all over Japan for analyses. The locations of respondents were defined as the center of their postcode area, which was established using latitude and longitude, and their distance from the EQ epicenter was calculated. The distance was transformed using a common logarithm as in earlier studies [36]. Logistic regression analyses using generalized linear models with a binomial error distribution and a logit link function (glm function in R Software version 2.15.0) were performed to analyze relationships between the distance from the epicenter and the presence of UABs according to each term. The presence or absence of UABs in each term was used as a dependent variable, and the distance from the epicenter was used as an independent variable. All analyses were conducted in dogs and cats separately.
We removed the effects of known factors that decrease milk yield before the analysis. First, we removed the effect of the number of days after calving by using WOOD’s lactation curve model [37]. Second, we removed the effect of the temperature-humidity index [38], which is used as the index of heat stress for cows [39], by using two-phased regression model. Third, final variations in milk yield were calculated by subtracting the average milk yield after removal of the factors one to seven days before the EQ from those of the current day, and these final variations were used for the next analysis. We used milk yields from the 1 to 31 January 2011, as the basal period, assuming that any EQ-related variation had not begun. Then, the differences between milk yields in the basal period and those in each day, from the 1 February to the 11 March 2011, were analyzed using a repeated measures two-way ANOVA and post hoc test. If there was an interaction between the milking facility and the days, then the differences between the milk yield in the basal period and those in each day were analyzed using a repeated measures one-way ANOVA and post hoc test for that facility. All statistical procedures were analyzed by using R Software version 2.15.0. The cutoff for statistical significance in all tests was established as a P ≤ 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. UABs of Pets

A total of 1,976 responses were collected in this study. Fourteen responses were omitted, one from abroad, and 13 that lacked reliability and/or contained contradictory information. Of the 1,962 usable responses, 1,259 were dog owners and 703 cat owners. The details of the general information on the pets and their living environments are shown in Table 2. The minimum and maximum distances of the dog owners from the epicenter were 140 km and 2,350 km, respectively. For cat owners, they were 140 km and 1,950 km, respectively.
Table 2. General characteristics of the pets.
Table 2. General characteristics of the pets.
DogsCats
n1259703
Sex
Male (intact)665 (329)373 (57)
Female (intact)589 (235)327 (41)
Unknown53
Age (years old)
<16934
1–3372249
4–6303144
7–926188
10–1216376
13–156344
>161346
Unknown1522
Rearing place
Indoor1159677
Outdoor10026
Distance from epicenter (km; mean ± S.E.)527.5 ± 7.8498.7 ± 9.7
A total of 236 (18.7%) dog owners and 115 (16.4%) cat owners observed UABs of their pets (Figure 2). The numbers of dog and cat owners that observed a single UAB were 55 and 39, respectively, and the numbers that observed more than one UAB were 181 and 76, respectively. In observed UABs, restless behavior was the most common, whereas diarrhea, vomited, no appetite, and aggressive behavior occurred only occasionally in dogs and cats. The timing of the UABs was different between dogs and cats. In dogs, 60.0% of the UABs occurred “from a few seconds to minutes” before the EQ. The distribution of the remaining UABs was as follows: 16.7% were “from 1 to a few hours”, 7.1% were “1 day”, 7.3% were “from 2 to 3 days”, 2.6% were “from 4 to 5 days”, and 6.3% were “6 or more days” before the EQ. In cats 44.6% of the UABs occurred “from a few seconds to minutes” and 30.4% occurred “from 1 to a few hours” before the EQ. The distribution of the remaining UABs was as follows: 9.0% were “1 day”, 11.5% were “from 2 to 3 days”, 1.6% were “from 4 to 5 days”, and 2.9% were “6 or more days” before the EQ.
Results of a regression analysis of each term are shown in Table 3 and Table 4. Dog UABs in the categories “from a few seconds to minutes (P < 0.001)”, “from 1 to a few hours (P = 0.020)”, “1 day (P = 0.019)”, and “6 or more days (P < 0.001)” before EQ were significantly closer to the epicenter. Also, cats UABs at 2 to 3 days (P = 0.014) increased as the distance to the epicenter decreased.
Figure 2. (a) Numbers of unusual animal behaviors (UABs) of dogs in each behavioral category and the time of occurrence prior to the 2011 earthquake off the Pacific coast of Tohoku, Japan; (b) Relative ratio normalized as % of the total; (c) Numbers of unusual animal behaviors (UABs) of cats in each behavioral category and the time of occurrence prior to the 2011 earthquake off the Pacific coast of Tohoku, Japan; (d) Relative ratio normalized as % of the total.
Figure 2. (a) Numbers of unusual animal behaviors (UABs) of dogs in each behavioral category and the time of occurrence prior to the 2011 earthquake off the Pacific coast of Tohoku, Japan; (b) Relative ratio normalized as % of the total; (c) Numbers of unusual animal behaviors (UABs) of cats in each behavioral category and the time of occurrence prior to the 2011 earthquake off the Pacific coast of Tohoku, Japan; (d) Relative ratio normalized as % of the total.
Animals 04 00131 g002
Table 3. Results of regression model to evaluate relationships between the epicentral distance and the presence of unusual behaviors of dogs during each time period; CI = confidence interval b = coefficient.
Table 3. Results of regression model to evaluate relationships between the epicentral distance and the presence of unusual behaviors of dogs during each time period; CI = confidence interval b = coefficient.
bP valueOdds ratio95% CI
lowerupper
From a few seconds to minutes−2.021<0.0010.1490.0670.326
From 1 to a few hours−1.6050.0200.2250.0620.824
1 day−2.3350.0190.1300.0200.836
From 2 to 3 days−1.6330.1060.1900.0291.277
From 4 to 5 days1.3560.4563.7420.116139.779
6 or more days−4.408<0.0010.0140.0010.128
Table 4. Results of regression model to evaluate relationships between the epicentral distance and the presence of unusual behaviors of cats during each time period; CI = confidence interval b = coefficient.
Table 4. Results of regression model to evaluate relationships between the epicentral distance and the presence of unusual behaviors of cats during each time period; CI = confidence interval b = coefficient.
bP valueOdds ratio95% CI
lowerupper
From a few seconds to minutes−0.8480.1550.4280.1321.373
From 1 to a few hours−1.2810.0710.2780.0681.111
1 day1.2980.3083.6640.30746.573
From 2 to 3 days−2.9740.0140.0510.0040.525
From 4 to 5 days−1.0820.6660.3390.00247.806
6 or more days 0.1740.9051.1900.06921.144

3.2. Milk Yields of Cows

The institutes of animal industry in Shizuoka, Kanagawa, and Ibaraki Prefecture of Japan are located at 490 km, 430 km, and 340 km, respectively, from the EQ’s epicenter (Figure 1). Table 5 is a list of EQs that satisfied the Dobrovolsky radius condition [40] recorded at least one of the three local institutes from 1 February 2011 to 11 March 2011 (14:46 JST).
Table 5. A list of earthquakes that satisfied the Dobrovolsky radius condition recorded at least one of the three local institutes from 1 February 2011, to 11 March 2011.
Table 5. A list of earthquakes that satisfied the Dobrovolsky radius condition recorded at least one of the three local institutes from 1 February 2011, to 11 March 2011.
DateTimeDistance from epicenters (km)Dobrovolsky radius (km)Depth (km)Magnitude
IbarakiKanagawaShizuoka
05/02/201110:56140130190170 64 5.2
10/02/201122:03170260320210 48 5.4
24/02/201114:36 3011017040 73 3.6
26/02/201104:12180140190140 56 5.0
27/02/201102:18240190140140 4 5.0
27/02/201105:38240200140230 4 5.5
07/03/201115:13 90 30 7040140 3.8
09/03/201111:453804705301380 8 7.3
09/03/201111:57390480540460 12 6.2
09/03/201111:58370460520380 21 6.0
09/03/201113:36400490550420 11 6.1
10/03/201103:16350440500570 29 6.4
10/03/201103:44400490560510 36 6.3
10/03/201106:22360460520510 18 6.3
10/03/201106:23350450510840 9 6.8
11/03/201114:464303404907410 24 9.0
Milk yields analyzed for this period showed a normal distribution. Standard deviations for milk yields at the three institutes from 1 January 2011, to 11 March 2011, were 0.621 at Ibaraki, 0.639 at Kanagawa and 0.601 at Shizuoka. Milk yields varied among the days (repeated measures two-way ANOVA, F = 1.730, P = 0.003), although the main effect among the facilities was not significant (Figure 3). An interaction between the days and the milking facilities was found (F = 1.778, P < 0.001). The result of a repeated measures one-way ANOVA analyzing the difference between milk yield during the basal period and during each day prior to the EQ at each institute revealed that only the Ibaraki Prefecture facility varied among the days (F = 8.875, P < 0.001). Post hoc tests indicated that milk yields on 10 February 2011 (P < 0.001), 5 March 2011 (P < 0.001), 6 March 2011 (P < 0.001), 7 March 2011 (P < 0.001), and 8 March 2011 (P = 0.001), were significantly decreased in comparison with the basal period in Ibaraki (Figure 3).
Figure 3. Changes in the dairy cow milk yields (mean ± S.E.) modified by days after calving and environmental factors at three institutes in Japan from 1 January 2011, to 11 March 2011. Darkened triangle (▼) indicates the occurrence of the earthquake. A double asterisks denote a significant difference at P < 0.001 compared with milk yields from 1 January 2011, to 31 January 2011, at the same institute.
Figure 3. Changes in the dairy cow milk yields (mean ± S.E.) modified by days after calving and environmental factors at three institutes in Japan from 1 January 2011, to 11 March 2011. Darkened triangle (▼) indicates the occurrence of the earthquake. A double asterisks denote a significant difference at P < 0.001 compared with milk yields from 1 January 2011, to 31 January 2011, at the same institute.
Animals 04 00131 g003

4. Discussion

The clear findings of this survey were that UABs of pets and decreased milk yields from cows occurred about one week before the EQ. Most of the UABs of dogs and cats were observed within one day before the EQ, and the decreased milk yield occurred in Ibaraki Prefecture, 340 km from the EQ’s epicenter.
The numbers of reports in which pet owners observed UABs of their pets were 18.6% for dogs and 16.6% for cats. The most frequently observed UAB in dogs and cats was restless behavior and “stuck close to the owner” was also indicated as a UAB in both species. Cats were described as “hiding” or “escaped” more often than dogs; however, this might be due to the natural difference between the species’ behaviors in which cats are more able to act three-dimensionally and go through narrower interspaces than dogs [41]. Although the options of diarrhea, vomited, and no appetite were used for visually apparent behavioral changes, they were rarely observed in either animal.
The ratio of total UABs increased in both animals as the time of the EQ occurrence neared. Many UABs prior to the Christchurch EQ in New Zealand (4 September 2010, Mw = 7.1) occurred within one hour of the EQ [42]. Although the exact time of the UABs were not asked for in this study, the variation in the UABs occurrence with time seemed similar to that of the Christchurch EQ. UABs were observed from a few seconds to minutes before the EQ, which could include responses to P-waves as described by Kirschvink [43]. In the Tohoku EQ, P-waves arrived at the closest area from the epicenter about 20 seconds before the arrival S-waves [44]. Thus, there was sufficient time to notice the behavioral changes of pets by their owners. In fact, three out of 84 respondents replied that “behavioral changes were shown before EQ sounds”. This suggests the presence of acoustic stimuli that could be discerned even by humans. No seismic swarms, as foreshocks of the Tohoku EQ, were observed before 9 March 2011; however, a number of large and small EQs occurred from 9 March 2011 (11:45 JST) to the Tohoku EQ on 11 March 2011 (14:46 JST), including 56 EQs of more than 3.5 M and 18 EQs of more than 5.0 M. Furthermore, 18 EQs satisfied the Dobrovolsky radius condition in the area nearest the epicenter. The UABs of dogs and cats after 9 March 2011, might include stress responses to shaking caused by these EQs. Therefore, the real ratio of UABs as precursor of the Tohoku EQ were observed after 9 March 2011, might be lower than indicated by the results of our survey. However, we consider that the UABs of pets occurred mostly in the few hours preceding the largest EQ (Mw = 9.0).
The UABs of dogs increased as their distance from the epicenter decreased, except from two to three days and from four to five days before the EQ, whereas cats showed the same correlation from only two to three days before the EQ. Previous research found the same relationship between the number of UABs and the distance from the epicenter for many EQs [4,36]. UABs prior to EQs are considered as stress or emotional responses to physical or chemical variations. Some physical or chemical variations prior to the Tohoku EQ emerged during various time periods, and the emerged anomalies did not necessarily remain until the EQ occurred [45,46,47]. The existence or non-existence of a relationship between UABs and the epicentral distance depended on precursory terms. These irregularities of physical or chemical states might cause the UABs identified in this study, although we do not know what triggers the UABs prior to EQs. However, significant relationships between epicentral distance and UABs in dogs “1 day”, “from 1 to a few hours” and “from a few seconds to minutes” before the Tohoku EQ, and those in cats “from 2 to 3 days” before the Tohoku EQ might include effects from stress responses to shaking caused by foreshocks after 9 March 2011.
There are some suggestions for possible stimuli, which cause UABs prior to EQs [22,24,26]. For example, Freund and Stolc have suggested that animals might respond to massive amounts of positive airborne ions, massive amounts of toxic gases and electromagnetic waves of ultralow and extremely low frequency [26]; however, the clear mechanism for UABs has remained unknown. At present time, to examine whether UABs are useful in EQ predictions, longitudinal observations of UABs are important and essential. It may be an effective prediction method since the objective behavior is observed everyday automatically. In fact, changes in the locomotive activities of mice before large EQs were reported by Yokoi et al. [11] and Li et al. [12]. Lott et al. [48] reported that ratios of UABs prior to EQs differed between some events, even if these occurred with similar distance, depth and magnitude. Longitudinal observations could enable to know that animals show UABs more frequently prior to what kind of EQs. In this study, restless behavior was the most frequently observed UAB in dogs and cats. It may be useful to quantify the frequency of this behavior. However, this result is based on only one case from an extremely large EQ, so further studies are required.
The milk yield from milking facilities at institutes in Shizuoka and Kanagawa Prefecture were not significantly affected by the coming EQ, but the facility in Ibaraki showed lowered milk production 6 days before the EQ. The decrease in the milk yield continued for four days. This might be because Ibaraki was the closest of the three institutes to the epicenter. If so, milk yield might be useful as an EQ precursor. Furthermore, these decreases of milk yields were probably not caused by fear responses to the EQ’s shaking, because no seismic swarms that satisfied the Dobrovolsky radius condition [40] occurred near the location of the institute in Ibaraki Prefecture from the 5 to 8 March 2011. The 7.3 M EQ on 9 March 2011, did not influence milk yields, because there were not significant decreases after 9 March 2011, at the three institutes. Milk yields on 10 February 2011, also decreased compared with the basal period. This decrease might have been a precursor for the EQ (M = 5.4) that occurred the same day, although the possibility that this decrease was a precursor of the Tohoku EQ cannot be excluded. As the milking work was already finished at the time of this EQ (22:03 JST), the decrease was not a response to the shaking. However, milk yields before other EQs that satisfied the Dobrovolsky radius condition did not show significant decreases. Tests for relationships between milk yields and more EQs using a longer data period could reveal differences between EQs that affect milk yields and those that do not.
Our study revealed the time periods in which the UABs of pets and changes in the milk yields of dairy cows occurred. About 80% of pet UABs were observed within one day of the EQ, whereas cow milk yields decreased six days before the EQ, which preceded the UABs of dogs and cats. Milk yields in each individual have been measured by many institutes in the animal husbandry industry to manage and improve productivity, and the UABs of pets have been observed directly by owners. These two phenomena could contribute to EQ predictions if people carefully observe whether pets show UABs, especially restless behavior, and when dairy cow milk yields decrease, as EQ precursors.

5. Conclusions

Our study revealed the characteristics of pet UABs and of changes in the milk yield of dairy cows associated with an extremely large EQ. The number of UABs decreased as the distance from the epicenter increased, as seen in previous research. The results suggest that the restless behavior is the most common UAB prior to EQs. Milk yield decreased from four to six days before the Tohoku EQ in the facility closest from the epicenter (340 km). Our study indicates that these two phenomena could contribute to the prediction of EQs, but the longitudinal and objective measurement of UABs and the verification of the relevance between milk yield levels and other EQs need to be performed.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Matching Fund Subsidy for Private Universities from The Promotion and Mutual Aid Corporation for Private Schools of Japan. The authors are grateful to Iris Ohyama Inc. for their interest in, and assistance with, the questionnaire survey. We also thank Livestock Industry Technology Station; Kanagawa Agricultural Technology Center, Shizuoka Prefectural Research Institute of Animal Industry, and National Agriculture and Food Research Organization for their cooperation and provision of the cows’ milk yield data.

Author Contributions

H. Yamauchi conceived and designed the study, analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. H. Uchiyama helped with the designation and implementation of the questionnaire survey. M. Ohta and N. Ohtani involved in interpreting results and editing the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Hayakawa, M.; Hattori, K.; Ohta, K. Monitoring of ULF (Ultra-Low-Frequency) Geomagnetic Variations Associated with Earthquakes. Sensors 2007, 7, 1108–1122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Maekawa, S.; Horie, T.; Yamauchi, T.; Sawaya, T.; Ishikawa, M.; Hayakawa, M.; Sasaki, H. A Statistical Study on the Effect of Earthquakes on the Ionosphere, Based on the Subionospheric LF Propagation Data in Japan. Ann. Geophys. (Germany) 2006, 24, 2219–2225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Wattananikorn, K.; Kanaree, M.; Wiboolsake, S. Soil Gas Radon as an Earthquake Precursor: Some Considerations on Data Improvement. Radiat. Measur. 1998, 29, 593–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Rikitake, T. Prediction and Precursors of Majour Earthquakes; Terra Scientific Publishing Company: Tokyo, Japan, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  5. Terada, T. On Luminous Phenomena Accompanying Earthquakes. Bull. Earthq. Res. Inst. Univ. Tokyo 1931, 9, 225–255. [Google Scholar]
  6. Tributsch, H. When the Snakes Awake: Animals and Earthquake Prediction; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
  7. Igarashi, G.; Wakita, H.; Sato, T. Precursory and Coseismic Anomalies in Well Water Levels Observed for the February 2, 1992 Tokyo Bay Earthquake. Geophys. Res. Lett. 1992, 19, 1583–1586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Wadatsumi, K. 1591 Witnesses Phenomena Prior to Earthquakes (in Japanese); Tokyo Publisher: Tokyo, Japan, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  9. Ulsoy, Ü.; Ikeya, M. Retrospective Statements of Earthquake Precursors by Eye-Witnesses. In Future Systems for Earthquake Early Warning; Ulsoy, Ü., Kundu, K.H., Eds.; Nova Science Publishers, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 3–53. [Google Scholar]
  10. Buskirk, R.E.; Frohlich, C.; Latham, G.V. Unusual Animal Behavior before Earthquakes: A Review of Possible Sensory Mechanisms. Rev. Geophys. 1981, 19, 247–270. [Google Scholar]
  11. Yokoi, S.; Ikeya, M.; Yagi, T.; Nagai, K. Mouse Circadian Rhythm before the Kobe Earthquake in 1995. Bioelectromagnetics 2003, 24, 289–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Li, Y.; Liu, Y.; Jiang, Z.; Guan, J.; Yi, G.; Cheng, S.; Yang, B.; Fu, T.; Wang, Z. Behavioral Change Related to Wenchuan Devastating Earthquake in Mice. Bioelectromagnetics 2009, 30, 613–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Rikitake, T. Biosystem Behaviour as an Earthquake Precursor. Tectonophysics 1978, 51, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Ikeya, M. Earthquakes and Animals: From Folks Legends to Science; World Scientific: Singapore, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  15. Heffner, H.E. Auditory Awareness. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1998, 57, 259–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Shier, D.; Butler, J.; Lewis, R. Hole's Human Anatomy and Physiology, 10th ed; McGraw-Hill: Boston, MA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  17. Hart, B.L. Olfaction and Feline Behavior. Feline Pract. 1977, 7, 8–10. [Google Scholar]
  18. Beadle, M. The Cat: History, Biology, and Behavior; Simon and Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  19. Halász, N. Vertebrate Olfactory System; Akaderniai Kiado: Budapest, Hungary, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  20. Krestel, D.; Passe, D.; Smith, J.; Jonsson, L. Behavioral Determination of Olfactory Thresholds to Amyl Acetate in Dogs. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1984, 8, 169–174. [Google Scholar]
  21. Passe, D.; Walker, J. Odor Psychophysics in Vertebrates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1985, 9, 431–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hayakawa, M. Possible Electromagnetic Effects on Abnormal Animal Behavior before an Earthquake. Animals 2013, 3, 19–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Lott, D.F.; Hart, B.L.; Verosub, K.L.; Howell, M.W. Is Unusual Animal Behavior Observed before Earthquakes? Yes and No. Geophys. Res. Lett. 1979, 6, 685–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Grant, R.A.; Halliday, T.; Balderer, W.P.; Leuenberger, F.; Newcomer, M.; Cyr, G.; Freund, F.T. Ground Water Chemistry Changes before Major Earthquakes and Possible Effects on Animals. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2011, 8, 1936–1956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Freund, F. Earthquake forewarning—A Multidisciplinary Challenge from the Ground Up to Space. Acta Geophys. 2013, 61, 775–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Freund, F.; Stolc, V. Nature of Pre-Earthquake Phenomena and their Effects on Living Organisms. Animals 2013, 3, 513–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Singh, R.P.; Kumar, J.S.; Zlotnicki, J.; Kafatos, M. Satellite Detection of Carbon Monoxide Emission Prior to the Gujarat Earthquake of 26 January 2001. Appl. Geochem. 2010, 25, 580–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Rushen, J.; Munksgaard, L.; Marnet, P.; DePassillé, A. Human Contact and the Effects of Acute Stress on Cows at Milking. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2001, 73, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Rigalma, K.; Duvaux-Ponter, C.; Barrier, A.; Charles, C.; Ponter, A.; Deschamps, F.; Roussel, S. Medium-Term Effects of Repeated Exposure to Stray Voltage on Activity, Stress Physiology, and Milk Production and Composition in Dairy Cows. J. Dairy Sci. 2010, 93, 3542–3552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Fidani, C. Biological Anomalies around the 2009 L’Aquila Earthquake. Animals 2013, 3, 693–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Nikonov, A.A. Abnormal Animal Behaviour as a Precursor of the 7 December 1988 Spitak, Armenia, Earthquake. Nat. Hazards 1992, 6, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Beaver, B.V. Feline Behavior; Elsevier Health Sciences: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  33. Beaver, B.V. Canine Behavior: Insights and Answers; Elsevier Health Sciences: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  34. Casey, R. Fear and Stress. In BSAVA Manual of Canine and Feline Behavioural Medicine; Horwitz, D., Mills, D., Eds.; British Small Animal Veterinary Association: Guarantee, UK, 2002; pp. 144–153. [Google Scholar]
  35. Chen, C.; Wen, S.; Liu, J.; Hattori, K.; Han, P.; Hobara, Y.; Wang, C.; Yeh, T.; Yen, H. Surface Displacements in Japan before the 11 March 2011 M9. 0 Tohoku-Oki Earthquake. J. Asian Earth Sci. 2014, 80, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Whitehead, N.E.; Ulusoy, Ü.; Asahara, H.; Ikeya, M. Are any Public-Reported Earthquake Precursors Valid? Nat. Hazard. Earth. Sys. 2004, 4, 463–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Wood, P.D.P. Algebraic Model of the Lactation Curve in Cattle. Nature 1967, 216, 164–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration. Livestock Hot Weather Stress.Operations Manual Letter C-31–76; Department of Commerce, NOAA, National Weather Service Central Region: Kansas City, MO, USA, 1976.
  39. Vitali, A.; Segnalini, M.; Bertocchi, L.; Bernabucci, U.; Nardone, A.; Lacetera, N. Seasonal Pattern of Mortality and Relationships between Mortality and Temperature-Humidity Index in Dairy Cows. J. Dairy Sci. 2009, 92, 3781–3790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Dobrovolsky, I.P.; Zubkov, S.I.; Miachkin, V.I. Estimation of the Size of Earthquake Preparation Zones. Pure Appl. Geophys. 1979, 117, 1025–1044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Liso, de G.; Fidani, C. Seismic Precursory Phenomenology in Unusual Animal Behaviour in Val Pellice, Western Piedmont, in Comparison with Anomalies of some Physical Parameters. Open J. Earthq. Res. 2014, 3, 30–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Whitehead, N.E.; Ulusoy, Ü. Macroscopic Anomalies before the September 2010 M = 7.1 Earthquake in Christchurch, New Zealand. Nat. Hazard. Earth. Syst. Sci. 2013, 13, 167–176. [Google Scholar]
  43. Kirschvink, J.L. Earthquake Prediction by Animals: Evolution and Sensory Perception. Acta Seismol. Sinica 2000, 90, 312–323. [Google Scholar]
  44. Hoshiba, M.; Iwakiri, K.; Hayashimoto, N.; Shimoyama, T.; Hirano, K.; Yamada, Y.; Ishigaki, Y.; Kikuta, H. Outline of the 2011 off the Pacific Coast of Tohoku Earthquake (Mw 9.0)-Earthquake Early Warning and Observed Seismic Intensity. Earth Planets Space 2011, 63, 547–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Hayakawa, M.; Hobara, Y.; Yasuda, Y.; Yamaguchi, H.; Ohta, K.; Izutsu, J.; Nakamura, T. Possible Precursor to the March 11, 2011, Japan Earthquake: Ionospheric Perturbations as seen by Subionospheric very Low frequency/low Frequency Propagation. Ann. Geophys. 2012, 55, 95–99. [Google Scholar]
  46. Heki, K. Ionospheric Electron Enhancement Preceding the 2011 Tohoku-Oki Earthquake. Geophys. Res. Lett. 2011, 38. Available online: http://hdl.handle.net/2115/48572 (accessed on 2 February 2014). [CrossRef]
  47. Ouzounov, D.; Pulinets, S.; Romanov, A.; Romanov, A.; Tsybulya, K.; Davidenko, D.; Kafatos, M.; Taylor, P. Atmosphere-Ionosphere Response to the M9 Tohoku Earthquake Revealed by Multi-Instrument Space-Borne and Ground Observations: Preliminary Results. Earthq. Sci. 2011, 24, 557–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Lott, D.F.; Hart, B.L.; Howell, M.W. Retrospective Studies of Unusual Animal Behavior as an Earthquake Predictor. Geophys. Res. Lett. 1981, 8, 1203–1206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yamauchi, H.; Uchiyama, H.; Ohtani, N.; Ohta, M. Unusual Animal Behavior Preceding the 2011 Earthquake off the Pacific Coast of Tohoku, Japan: A Way to Predict the Approach of Large Earthquakes. Animals 2014, 4, 131-145. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4020131

AMA Style

Yamauchi H, Uchiyama H, Ohtani N, Ohta M. Unusual Animal Behavior Preceding the 2011 Earthquake off the Pacific Coast of Tohoku, Japan: A Way to Predict the Approach of Large Earthquakes. Animals. 2014; 4(2):131-145. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4020131

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yamauchi, Hiroyuki, Hidehiko Uchiyama, Nobuyo Ohtani, and Mitsuaki Ohta. 2014. "Unusual Animal Behavior Preceding the 2011 Earthquake off the Pacific Coast of Tohoku, Japan: A Way to Predict the Approach of Large Earthquakes" Animals 4, no. 2: 131-145. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4020131

APA Style

Yamauchi, H., Uchiyama, H., Ohtani, N., & Ohta, M. (2014). Unusual Animal Behavior Preceding the 2011 Earthquake off the Pacific Coast of Tohoku, Japan: A Way to Predict the Approach of Large Earthquakes. Animals, 4(2), 131-145. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani4020131

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop