Inter- and Intra-Species Communication of Emotion: Chemosignals as the Neglected Medium
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
1.1. Prelude: Human Chemosignaling
1.2. Emotion Transfer from Humans to Humans via Chemosignals
1.3. Emotion Transfer from Humans to Other Animals
2. Discussion and Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Semin, G.R. Agenda 2000: Communication: Language as an Implementational Device for Cognition. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 30, 595–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Semin, G.R. Grounding communication: Synchrony. In Social psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 630–649. ISBN 1-57230-918-0. [Google Scholar]
- Von Frisch, K. The Dances of the Honey Bee. Bull. Anim. Behav. 1947, 5, 1–32. [Google Scholar]
- Von Frisch, K. The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees; Oxford Univeristy Press: London, UK, 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Sandoz, J.C.; Laloi, D.; Odoux, J.F.; Pham-Delègue, M.H. Olfactory information transfer in the honeybee: Compared efficiency of classical conditioning and early exposure. Anim. Behav. 2000, 59, 1025–1034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wenner, A.M.; Wells, P.H.; Johnson, D.L. Honey bee recruitment to food sources: Olfaction or language? Science 1969, 164, 84–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bordereau, C.; Pasteels, J. Pheromones and Chemical Ecology of Dispersal and Foraging in Termites. In Biology of Termites: A Modern Synthesis; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2010; pp. 279–320. [Google Scholar]
- Tinebergen, N. The curious behavior of the stickleback. Sci. Am. 1952, 187, 22–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGann, J.P. Poor human olfaction is a 19th-century myth. Science 2017, 356, 7263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Majid, A.; Burenhult, N. Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language. Cognition 2014, 130, 266–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Majid, A.; Roberts, S.G.; Cilissen, L.; Emmorey, K.; Nicodemus, B.; O’Grady, L.; Woll, B.; LeLan, B.; de Sousa, H.; Cansler, B.L.; et al. Differential coding of perception in the world’s languages. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 11369–11376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mori, K.; Yoshihara, Y. Molecular recognition and olfactory processing in the mammalian olfactory system. Prog. Neurobiol. 1995, 45, 585–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bushdid, C.; Magnasco, M.O.; Vosshall, L.B.; Keller, A. Humans can discriminate more than 1 trillion olfactory stimuli. Science 2014, 343, 1370–1372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Porter, J.; Craven, B.; Khan, R.M.; Chang, S.-J.; Kang, I.; Judkewitz, B.; Volpe, J.; Settles, G.; Sobel, N. Mechanisms of scent-tracking in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 2007, 10, 27–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gerkin, R.C.; Castro, J.B. The number of olfactory stimuli that humans can discriminate is still unknown. eLife 2015, 4, e08127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meister, M. On the dimensionality of odor space. eLife 2015, 4, e07865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ruddigkeit, L.; van Deursen, R.; Blum, L.C.; Reymond, J.-L. Enumeration of 166 billion organic small molecules in the chemical universe database GDB-17. J. Chem. Inf. Model. 2012, 52, 2864–2875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, D.; Haviland-Jones, J. Human olfactory communication of emotion. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2000, 91, 771–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhou, W.; Chen, D. Fear-related chemosignals modulate recognition of fear in ambiguous facial expressions. Psychol. Sci. 2009, 20, 177–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Groot, J.H.B.; Semin, G.R.; Smeets, M.A.M. I can see, hear, and smell your fear: Comparing olfactory and audiovisual media in fear communication. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2014, 143, 825–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mutic, S.; Parma, V.; Brunner, Y.F.; Freiherr, J. You Smell Dangerous: Communicating Fight Responses Through Human Chemosignals of Aggression. Chem. Senses 2016, 41, 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olsson, M.J.; Lundstrom, J.N.; Kimball, B.A.; Gordon, A.R.; Karshikoff, B.; Hosseini, N.; Sorjonen, K.; Olgart Hoglund, C.; Solares, C.; Soop, A.; et al. The scent of disease: Human body odor contains an early chemosensory cue of sickness. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 25, 817–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Groot, J.H.B.; Semin, G.R.; Smeets, M.A.M. Chemical communication of fear: A case of male-female asymmetry. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2014, 143, 1515–1525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bensafi, M.; Brown, W.M.; Tsutsui, T.; Mainland, J.D.; Johnson, B.N.; Bremner, E.A.; Young, N.; Mauss, I.; Ray, B.; Gross, J.; et al. Sex-steroid derived compounds induce sex-specific effects on autonomic nervous system function in humans. Behav. Neurosci. 2003, 117, 1125–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parma, V.; Tirindelli, R.; Bisazza, A.; Massaccesi, S.; Castiello, U. Subliminally perceived odours modulate female intrasexual competition: An eye movement study. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e30645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martins, Y.; Preti, G.; Crabtree, C.R.; Runyan, T.; Vainius, A.A.; Wysocki, C.J. Preference for human body odors is influenced by gender and sexual orientation. Psychol. Sci. 2005, 16, 694–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lubke, K.T.; Hoenen, M.; Pause, B.M. Differential processing of social chemosignals obtained from potential partners in regards to gender and sexual orientation. Behav. Brain Res. 2012, 228, 375–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gelstein, S.; Yeshurun, Y.; Rozenkrantz, L.; Shushan, S.; Frumin, I.; Roth, Y.; Sobel, N. Human tears contain a chemosignal. Science 2011, 331, 226–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Porter, R.H. Olfaction and human kin recognition. Genetica 1998, 104, 259–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Havlicek, J.; Roberts, S.C.; Flegr, J. Women’s preference for dominant male odour: Effects of menstrual cycle and relationship status. Biol. Lett. 2005, 1, 256–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Groot, J.H.B.; Smeets, M.A.M.; Kaldewaij, A.; Duijndam, M.J.A.; Semin, G.R. Chemosignals communicate human emotions. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 1417–1424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Groot, J.H.B.; Smeets, M.A.M.; Rowson, M.J.; Bulsing, P.J.; Blonk, C.G.; Wilkinson, J.E.; Semin, G.R. A sniff of happiness. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 26, 684–700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Groot, J.H.B.; Smeets, M.A.M.; Semin, G.R. Rapid stress system drives chemical transfer of fear from sender to receiver. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0118211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gasper, K.; Clore, G.L. Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 13, 34–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Susskind, J.M.; Lee, D.H.; Cusi, A.; Feiman, R.; Grabski, W.; Anderson, A.K. Expressing fear enhances sensory acquisition. Nat. Neurosci. 2008, 11, 843–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kamiloglu, R.; Smeets, M.; Groot, J.; Semin, G. Fear Odor Facilitates the Detection of Fear Expressions Over Other Negative Expressions. Chem. Senses 2018, 43, 419–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McClintock, M.K. Human Pheromones: Primers, Releasers, Signalers or Modulators? In Reproduction in Context; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 355–420. [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson, R.J. An initial evaluation of the functions of human olfaction. Chem. Senses 2010, 35, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wyatt, T.D. The search for human pheromones: The lost decades and the necessity of returning to first principles. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2015, 282, 20142994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pause, B.M. Processing of Body Odor Signals by the Human Brain. Chemosens. Percept. 2012, 5, 55–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pettersson, H.; Kaminski, J.; Herrmann, E.; Tomasello, M. Understanding of human communicative motives in domestic dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 133, 235–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Payne, E.; DeAraugo, J.; Bennett, P.; McGreevy, P. Exploring the existence and potential underpinnings of dog-human and horse-human attachment bonds. Behav. Process. 2016, 125, 114–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thalmann, O.; Shapiro, B.; Cui, P.; Schuenemann, V.J.; Sawyer, S.K.; Greenfield, D.L.; Germonpré, M.B.; Sablin, M.V.; López-Giráldez, F.; Domingo-Roura, X.; et al. Complete Mitochondrial Genomes of Ancient Canids Suggest a European Origin of Domestic Dogs. Science 2013, 342, 871–874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Outram, A.K.; Stear, N.A.; Bendrey, R.; Olsen, S.; Kasparov, A.; Zaibert, V.; Thorpe, N.; Evershed, R.P. The Earliest Horse Harnessing and Milking. Science 2009, 323, 1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Aniello, B.; Polese, G.; Luongo, L.; Scandurra, A.; Magliozzi, L.; Aria, M.; Pinelli, C. Neuroanatomical relationships between FMRFamide-immunoreactive components of the nervus terminalis and the topology of olfactory bulbs in teleost fish. Cell Tissue Res. 2016, 364, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pinelli, C.; Rastogi, R.K.; Scandurra, A.; Jadhao, A.G.; Aria, M.; D’Aniello, B. A comparative cluster analysis of nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide phosphate (NADPH)-diaphorase histochemistry in the brains of amphibians. J. Comp. Neurol. 2014, 522, 2980–3003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kretchmer, K.R.; Fox, M.W. Effects of domestication on animal behaviour. Vet. Rec. 1975, 96, 102–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Prato-Previde, E.; Custance, D.M.; Spiezio, C.; Sabatini, F. Is the dog-human relationship an attachment bond? An observational study using Ainsworth’s strange situation. Behaviour 2003, 140, 225–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valsecchi, P.; Previde, E.P.; Accorsi, P.A.; Fallani, G. Development of the attachment bond in guide dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 123, 43–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mariti, C.; Ricci, E.; Carlone, B.; Moore, J.L.; Sighieri, C.; Gazzano, A. Dog attachment to man: A comparison between pet and working dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2013, 8, 135–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scandurra, A.; Alterisio, A.; D’Aniello, B. Behavioural effects of training on water rescue dogs in the Strange Situation Test. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 174, 121–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Aniello, B.; Scandurra, A.; Alterisio, A.; Valsecchi, P.; Prato-Previde, E. The importance of gestural communication: A study of human-dog communication using incongruent information. Anim. Cogn. 2016, 19, 1231–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scandurra, A.; Alterisio, A.; Marinelli, L.; Mongillo, P.; Semin, G.R.; D’Aniello, B. Effectiveness of verbal and gestural signals and familiarity with signal-senders on the performance of working dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 191, 78–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scandurra, A.; Alterisio, A.; Aria, M.; Vernese, R.; D’Aniello, B. Should I fetch one or the other? A study on dogs on the object choice in the bimodal contrasting paradigm. Anim. Cogn. 2018, 21, 119–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaminski, J.; Call, J.; Fischer, J. Word Learning in a Domestic Dog: Evidence for “Fast Mapping”. Science 2004, 304, 1682–1683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pilley, J.W.; Reid, A.K. Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents. Behav. Process. 2011, 86, 184–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grassmann, S.; Kaminski, J.; Tomasello, M. How two word-trained dogs integrate pointing and naming. Anim. Cogn. 2012, 15, 657–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Nagasawa, M.; Murai, K.; Mogi, K.; Kikusui, T. Dogs can discriminate human smiling faces from blank expressions. Anim. Cogn. 2011, 14, 525–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morisaki, A.; Takaoka, A.; Fujita, K. Are dogs sensitive to the emotional state of humans? J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2009, 4, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deputte, B.; Doll, A. Do dogs understand human facial expressions? J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2011, 6, 78–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albuquerque, N.; Guo, K.; Wilkinson, A.; Savalli, C.; Otta, E.; Mills, D. Dogs recognize dog and human emotions. Biol. Lett. 2016, 12, 20150883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Proops, L.; McComb, K.; Reby, D. Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 947–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proops, L.; McComb, K. Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus) extends to familiar humans. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2012, 279, 3131–3138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sankey, C.; Henry, S.; Andre, N.; Richard-Yris, M.-A.; Hausberger, M. Do horses have a concept of person? PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e18331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wathan, J.; McComb, K. The eyes and ears are visual indicators of attention in domestic horses. Curr. Biol. CB 2014, 24, 677–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Briefer, E.F.; Maigrot, A.-L.; Mandel, R.; Freymond, S.B.; Bachmann, I.; Hillmann, E. Segregation of information about emotional arousal and valence in horse whinnies. Sci. Rep. 2015, 4, 9989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wathan, J.; Burrows, A.M.; Waller, B.M.; McComb, K. EquiFACS: The Equine Facial Action Coding System. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0131738. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, A.V.; Proops, L.; Grounds, K.; Wathan, J.; McComb, K. Functionally relevant responses to human facial expressions of emotion in the domestic horse (Equus caballus). Biol. Lett. 2016, 12, 20150907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lanata, A.; Guidi, A.; Valenza, G.; Baragli, P.; Scilingo, E.P. Quantitative heartbeat coupling measures in human-horse interaction. In Proceedings of the 38th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Orlando, FL, USA, 16–20 August 2016; pp. 2696–2699. [Google Scholar]
- Siniscalchi, M.; d’Ingeo, S.; Quaranta, A. Orienting asymmetries and physiological reactivity in dogs’ response to human emotional faces. Learn. Behav. 2018, 46, 574–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siniscalchi, M.; d’Ingeo, S.; Quaranta, A. The dog nose “KNOWS” fear: Asymmetric nostril use during sniffing at canine and human emotional stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 2016, 304, 34–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Aniello, B.; Semin, G.R.; Alterisio, A.; Aria, M.; Scandurra, A. Interspecies transmission of emotional information via chemosignals: From humans to dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Anim. Cogn. 2018, 21, 67–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lanata, A.; Nardelli, M.; Valenza, G.; Baragli, P.; DrAniello, B.; Alterisio, A.; Scandurra, A.; Semin, G.R.; Scilingo, E.P. A Case for the Interspecies Transfer of Emotions: A Preliminary Investigation on How Humans Odors Modify Reactions of the Autonomic Nervous System in Horses. In Proceedings of the 40th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Honolulu, HI, USA, 18–21 July 2018; pp. 522–525. [Google Scholar]
- Lanata, A.; Guidi, A.; Valenza, G.; Baragli, P.; Scilingo, E.P. The role of nonlinear coupling in Human-Horse Interaction: A preliminary study. In Proceedings of the 39th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), Seogwipo, Korea, 11–15 July 2017; pp. 1320–1323. [Google Scholar]
- Guidi, A.; lanatà, A.; Baragli, P.; Valenza, G.; Scilingo, E. A Wearable System for the Evaluation of the Human-Horse Interaction: A Preliminary Study. Electronics 2016, 5, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Groot, J.H.B.; van Houtum, L.A.E.M.; Gortemaker, I.; Ye, Y.; Chen, W.; Zhou, W.; Smeets, M.A.M. Beyond the west: Chemosignaling of emotions transcends ethno-cultural boundaries. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2018, 98, 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Semin, G.R.; Scandurra, A.; Baragli, P.; Lanatà, A.; D’Aniello, B. Inter- and Intra-Species Communication of Emotion: Chemosignals as the Neglected Medium. Animals 2019, 9, 887. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9110887
Semin GR, Scandurra A, Baragli P, Lanatà A, D’Aniello B. Inter- and Intra-Species Communication of Emotion: Chemosignals as the Neglected Medium. Animals. 2019; 9(11):887. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9110887
Chicago/Turabian StyleSemin, Gün R., Anna Scandurra, Paolo Baragli, Antonio Lanatà, and Biagio D’Aniello. 2019. "Inter- and Intra-Species Communication of Emotion: Chemosignals as the Neglected Medium" Animals 9, no. 11: 887. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9110887
APA StyleSemin, G. R., Scandurra, A., Baragli, P., Lanatà, A., & D’Aniello, B. (2019). Inter- and Intra-Species Communication of Emotion: Chemosignals as the Neglected Medium. Animals, 9(11), 887. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9110887