Next Article in Journal
Effects of Grazing on the Behaviour, Oxidative and Immune Status, and Production of Organic Dairy Cows
Next Article in Special Issue
Could Greater Time Spent Displaying Waking Inactivity in the Home Environment Be a Marker for a Depression-Like State in the Domestic Dog?
Previous Article in Journal
Effects of Enrichment Type, Presentation and Social Status on Enrichment Use and Behaviour of Sows with Electronic Sow Feeding
Previous Article in Special Issue
Acquiring a Pet Dog: A Review of Factors Affecting the Decision-Making of Prospective Dog Owners
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Coping Styles in the Domestic Cat (Felis silvestris catus) and Implications for Cat Welfare

1
United States Department of Agriculture-Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service Center for Animal Welfare, 625 Harrison St, West Lafayette, IN 47907, USA
2
Department of Comparative Pathobiology, Purdue University, 625 Harrison St, West Lafayette, IN 47907, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2019, 9(6), 370; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9060370
Submission received: 16 May 2019 / Revised: 6 June 2019 / Accepted: 11 June 2019 / Published: 18 June 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Welfare of Cats and Dogs)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

It is important for cat caretakers to understand individual differences in response to stress. Identifying coping styles in cats may lead to improved health and welfare outcomes. In this study, we collected information from cat guardians pertaining to personality traits then singly-housed each cat for three days to mimic admittance to a veterinary hospital or shelter. Behavior was recorded hourly and response to approach of a familiar and unfamiliar person was assessed at the end of day 3. We found individual differences in the behavioral responses of cats to the acute stress of cage confinement. Additionally, guardian-rated personality traits agreed with the response of the cats when confined to a cage, suggesting that domestic cats have different coping styles. Identifying individual differences in response to stressful events or environments may provide caretakers with important information leading to improved welfare.

Abstract

Identifying coping styles in cats may lead to improved health and welfare. The aims of this study were to (1) identify individual differences in response to acute confinement, and (2) to assess the predictability of guardian-rated personality traits on behavior. Adult cats (n = 55) were singly housed in enriched cages and behavioral observations were recorded for three days. On day 3, familiar and unfamiliar person approach tests were conducted. Fecal glucocorticoid metabolites (FGM) were quantified from voided samples. A questionnaire assessing personality traits and sickness behaviors was completed by each guardian. Analysis identified two clusters—cats in Cluster 1 (n = 22) were described as shy, calm, mellow, and timid; cats in Cluster 2 (n = 33) were described as active, playful, curious, and easygoing. Multilevel mixed-effects GLM revealed significant differences between the clusters including food intake (C1 > C2, p < 0.0001), affiliative/maintenance behaviors (C2 > C1, p < 0.0001), vocalization (C2 > C1, p < 0.0001), hide (C1 > C2, p < 0.0001), perch (C2 > C1, p < 0.0001), and latency to approach a familiar (C1 > C2, p < 0.0001) and unfamiliar (C1 > C2, p = 0.013) person. No statistically significant differences in FGM concentrations were identified (cluster p = 0.28; day p = 0.16, interaction p = 0.26). Guardian-rated personality traits agreed with the response of the cats when confined to a cage, suggesting that domestic cats have different coping styles. Identifying individual differences in response to stressful events or environments may provide caretakers with important information leading to improved welfare.

1. Introduction

Cats face many challenges to their welfare when housed in cages in veterinary hospitals, shelters, or laboratories. These include physical components of the housing environment, such as temperature, noise, cage size, hiding and perching opportunities (or lack thereof) as well as the social environment, including the quality of the human–cat interactions, presence of unfamiliar cats, or other animals such as dogs [1,2,3,4]. Assessing individual differences in response to stressors during confinement may aid caretakers in making decisions that meet cats’ individual needs, thereby enhancing their welfare.
A stressor is a stimulus that disrupts homeostasis through activation of the hypothalamic stress response system that may be physical (e.g., heat/cold), psychological (e.g., perceived threat), or social (e.g., aggressive interactions) and categorized as either acute or chronic [5]. In order to reestablish homeostasis, the individual will elicit an adaptive response that includes both physiological and behavioral components. Exposure to unpredictable and uncontrollable environments that are intense, prolonged, or novel and exceed the individual’s ability to cope will result in negative outcomes [6,7,8]. These may include decreased food intake, inhibition of exploratory and social behaviors, learned helplessness, stereotypies, and aberrant immune responses [9,10,11]. For cats, such stressors include loud or unfamiliar noises, novel and unfamiliar places and objects, and the approach of strangers (humans, cats, or other animals) into their personal space. As in other species, breed and individual differences in temperament or stress susceptibility [12,13], as well as past experiences [14], also influence cats’ responses to the environment [15,16].
The terms personality, temperament, coping styles, and behavioral syndromes have been used interchangeably to mean behaviors that are relatively consistent across time and context (although see [17] for a criticism). Studies have identified several dimensions of felid personality [18]. A comparative study of 45 personality traits across species that included the domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus), Scottish wildcat (Felis silvestris grampia), clouded leopard (Neofelis nebulosa), snow leopard (Panthera uncia), and African lion (Panthera leo) identified three distinct factors from a principal components analysis for each species [19]. In that study, 20 caretakers rated 100 domestic cats’ in two shelters and the factors identified were as follows: the factor Dominance had the highest loadings on the traits aggressive to conspecifics, bullying, and dominant; the factor Impulsiveness had the highest loadings on the traits excitable, active, and playful; and Neuroticism had the highest loadings on the traits, anxious, insecure, tense, suspicious, and fearful of people [19]. Litchfield et al. [20] assessed 52 personality traits of 2802 cats rated by their guardians and identified five factors that represent traits related to Neuroticism, Dominance, Impulsiveness, Agreeableness, and Extroversion while two recent studies both identified six dimensions [21,22].
While discrepancy in the number of dimensions identified, naming conventions, and methodology across studies makes it difficult to utilize the potential benefits of understanding feline personality, including it as part of a welfare assessment could lead to improved care and health outcomes for cats. For example, a relationship between immune response and personality has been reported [23,24]. Cats who fall on the bold end of the shy–bold continuum in relation to conspecific social behavior have been reported to be at increased risk for contracting feline immunodeficiency virus (FIV) [25]. In this study, we use the term coping style as defined by Koolhaas et al. [26] as “a coherent set of behavioral and physiological stress responses which is consistent over time”. Two distinct coping styles have been identified in diverse species including fish (e.g., [27]), red jungle fowl (e.g., [28]), rodents (e.g., [29]), dogs (e.g., [30]), and non-human primates (e.g., [31]). The proactive coping style is an active response characterized by high sympathetic nervous system (SNS) activation and low hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis activation, while the behavioral response involves territorial control and aggression. The reactive coping style is a withdrawal response and is characterized by higher HPA-axis activation, higher parasympathetic reactivity and behaviorally by immobility and low levels of aggression [26,32].
Quantifying the intensity and duration of the activation of the HPA-axis is an important aspect of coping style studies. One non-invasive tool that can be utilized is measuring fecal glucocorticoid metabolites (FGM) which will be present in feces after a species-specific time delay roughly corresponding to gut passage time [33]. In cats, radiometabolism studies have shown that approximately 82% of glucocorticoid metabolites are found in feces with peak concentrations approximately 22 h after administration [34]. Therefore, FGM analysis may provide a useful, non-invasive way to monitor stress responses in cats.
Identifying coping styles in cats in response to challenging environments may lead to improvements in health and welfare when confined. Therefore, the aims of this study were to (1) identify individual differences in response to acute cage confinement, and (2) to assess the predictability of those responses based on guardian-rated personality traits in the home environment. We hypothesized that cats that have a reactive coping style would have decreased food intake, eliminations, and vocalizations, spend more time hiding and have higher FGM concentrations than cats that have a proactive coping style, and that these responses would be associated with guardian-rated personality traits.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects

Adult cats were recruited from faculty, staff and students of Purdue University’s College of Veterinary Medicine. Fifty-five neutered, non-pedigreed cats (n = 28 female, n = 27 male) from 36 households completed the study (mean age 5.2 years, range 1–12.8 years). All cats were healthy and current on vaccinates for viral rhinotracheitis, calicivirus, panleukopenia, and rabies at the time of participation. Participants were admitted to the study between 16:00 and 19:00 h on day 0 and placed in an individual stainless-steel cage then released back to their guardians between 16:00 and 19:00 h on day 3. Guardians provided informed consent when their cat(s) were volunteered for the study. The Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, Institutional Review Board, and the Clinical Trials Office in the College of Veterinary Medicine at Purdue University approved all experimental procedures (No. 1402001023).

2.2. Housing Environment

Cats were housed in the Purdue University vivarium in individual stainless-steel cages measuring 73 × 73 × 73 cm (Figure 1). The front half of the cage floor was covered by a mat (Sporttime yoga mat, School Specialty, Appleton, WI, USA). Each cage contained a cardboard box (ShelterDen by C Specialties Inc., Indianapolis, IN, USA) that had a hiding area (23 × 23 × 23 cm) with two access openings, placed in the rear corner of the cage. A shelf for perching (53 × 30 cm) was provided on the outside wall of the cage 27 cm from the top and bottom of the cage, flush with the back wall of the cage. Bedding (72 × 53 cm towel folded into quarters) was provided in the hide box and on the perch. A plastic litter pan (24 × 25 × 10 cm) was placed in the front half of the cage, filled with clumping litter (Tidy Cat, Nestle Purina Petcare Company, Wilkes Barre, PA, USA) at a depth of 3 cm. Guardians were asked to supply their cats’ regular diets and cats were fed per the instructions provided. Food and water were provided in separate 0.6 L stainless steel bowls.
The housing room had dimensions of 57 m × 84 m and held 20 cages along two walls facing each other. A 13:11 (07:00: 20:00 h) light:dark schedule was maintained to approximate length of day in Indiana, USA, at that time of year (September/October). Room temperature (mean ± SD) of 22 ± 1.6 °C (72 ± 4°F) was maintained throughout the vivarium. Disturbances from people, barking dogs, or other unpredictable noises and events were avoided to the extent possible. Cages were spot cleaned by a single researcher to minimize disruption to the cat and ensure consistent handling. The study was conducted in three replicates with no more than 20 cats randomized to each replicate.

2.3. Data Collection

Prior to the cats’ arrival at the veterinary school, guardians completed a questionnaire on sickness behavior, personality traits (adapted from [19]), and management practices (reported in [35]) (Appendix A).
Prior to routine husbandry each day, one researcher stood in front of each cage for 30–60 s to record the previous night’s food intake, urination, defecation, and other sickness behaviors (e.g., vomit, diarrhea, or eliminations outside of the litter pan) for each cat (Appendix B). During husbandry, any evidence of the cat’s behavior or state that could not be seen from outside the cage was recorded (e.g., cached food, vomit, or eliminations outside of the litter pan). All husbandry procedures occurred between 08:00 and 10:00 each day.
Behavioral observations were collected between 08:00 and 16:00 h on day 1 and 2 and 08:00 and 14:00 on day 3 using two sampling techniques. A scan sample was collected every 2 h that included the cat’s position in the cage, the type of behavior(s) they were exhibiting, and vocalizations based on an ethogram for cats in cages (adapted from [2]) (Appendix C). The observer stood quietly in the middle of the housing room and recorded these parameters. Duration of observation time for each scan sample was approximately 3 min. Because observer effects on the behavior of the cats could not be ruled out during scan sampling, on the alternate hours, a five-minute, continuous focal sample using the same ethogram for cats in cages was video recorded for later coding. Two cats were recorded simultaneously for 5 min with a total of five replicates per observation hour so that each cat was recorded once per recording hour. Video cameras (Canon 53× HD Vixio HF R40, Canon USA, Inc., Melville, NY, USA) were placed on tripods, one recording a cat housed in an upper cage and one recording a cat housed in a lower cage, while the researcher left the housing room to reduce observer effects on the cats’ behavior. All videos were coded by one researcher.
After the last scan sample observation on day 3, all cage doors were covered, and a three-step unfamiliar person approach test followed by a familiar person approach test (adapted from [36]) was conducted starting approximately 30 min after cage doors were covered. Cats were tested in a randomized order for both tests. Each cat cage was uncovered for the test immediately prior to commencement of step 1. One woman, unfamiliar to the cats, served as the stranger and the caretaker served as the familiar person. Data were recorded live as well as video recorded for further analysis. During step 1, the person stood quietly 1 m from the cage for 30 s. In step 2, the person placed her hand on the cage door and stood quietly for 30 s. In the last step (step 3), the person opened the cage door and stood quietly with her hand extended toward the cat for 30 s. The cage door was re-covered immediately after completion of step 3. Latency to interact, duration of interaction, and the cat’s response to approach were recorded at each step. Response to approach was scored as follows: 1 = actively avoidant, aggressive, or displaying other signs of distress; 2 = avoidant without showing aggression or signs of distress; 3 = remaining in the same position in the cage; with or without purring, rubbing, kneading paws; 4 = responding positively to the person, approaching observer; 5 = actively seeking interaction with observer; rubbing cage door, rolling, purring, meowing, soliciting play, etc.

2.4. Fecal Glucocorticoid Metabolite (FGM) Analysis

Fecal samples were collected daily from the litter pan of each cat that defecated, individually placed into a sealed plastic bag, labeled and frozen at −20 °C until analysis. Samples were shipped overnight on dry ice to the Chicago Zoological Society Endocrinology Service Lab for analysis. Fecal glucocorticoid metabolites (FGM) were extracted using 80% ethanol in dH2O. First, 0.5 g (±0.05 g) of each fecal sample was weighed out (Mettler balance, model #AB104-5) into 16 × 125 mm polypropylene tubes. Next, 5 mL of 80% ethanol solution was added to each extraction tube. Each tube was vortexed and placed on a rotator (Labline Maxi Rotator, model #4631/Fisher) overnight (14–18 h). Each tube was then centrifuged for 15 min at 1500 rpm (Marathon 3000R centrifuge, model #120). For each sample, 1 mL of supernatant and 1 mL of assay buffer (0.1 M phosphate buffered saline containing 1% BSA, pH 7.0) was pipetted into 12 × 75 mm polypropylene tubes to produce a 1:10 dilution and frozen at −20 °C until assay analyses.
The samples were assayed using a commercially available corticosterone EIA kit (Enzo Life Sciences, Ann Arbor, MI, USA, catalog # 901-097), to determine FGM concentrations. The manufacturer supplied all needed instructions and components. Plates were read on a photospectrometer plate reader (Dynex MRX Revelation) at 405 nm. Biochemical validation of the assays consisted of the following: (1) parallelism with the standard curve, and (2) recovery to determine the percentage of exogenous hormone measured. For the recovery test, one sample was spiked with the highest five standards. The cross-reactivity of the Enzo Life Sciences corticosterone anti-body is as follows: 100% corticosterone, 28.6% desoxycorticosterone, 1.7% progesterone, 0.28% tetrahydrocorticosterone, 0.18% aldosterone, 0.13% testosterone and any other steroids were <0.05%. Assay sensitivity was 26.99 pg/mL and the intra-assay coefficient of variation was 6.63% at 44.79% binding. Inter-assay variation was 4.72% at 60.49% binding. The recovery of exogenous corticosterone (250–4000 pg/mL) was 143.58% in domestic cats. All hormone data are expressed in ng/g wet feces.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

2.5.1. Cluster Analysis

Two cluster analyses were performed on information provided by the guardians prior to the study. The aim of this analysis was to determine if the reported sickness behavior and personality traits in the home correlated with those exhibited when the cat was confined, indicating a consistent coping style. Cluster analysis one (CA1) (kmeans, Gower measure for mixed data) grouped cats by the guardian-reported information that consisted of 33 variables related to personality traits (describes your cat, 1 = yes or 0 = no) and frequency of sickness behaviors (0 = I have never seen it, 1 = I have seen it at least once, 2 = I see it at least once per year, 3 = I see it at least once per month, 4 = I see it at least once per week, 5 = I see it daily) in the home environment. Fifty-one of the 55 cats in the study fit into one of the two clusters; Cluster 1 contained 62.8% of the cats (n = 32) and Cluster 2 contained 37.2% (n = 19) of the cats. Cluster analysis two (CA2) (kmeans, matched measure for binary data) grouped cats by the guardian-reported cat personality traits that consisted of 16 dichotomous variables. All 55 cats fit into one of the clusters; Cluster 1 contained 40% of cats (n = 22) and Cluster 2 contained 60% of the cats (n = 33).

2.5.2. FGM

A 2-way ANOVA (cluster, day and their interaction) was performed on FGM concentrations.

2.5.3. Multilevel Mixed-Effects Generalized Linear Models

Multilevel mixed-effects generalized linear models (Gaussian distribution, identity link function) were used to examine the association between cluster and the data collected in the vivarium environment. Four models were constructed, one each for sickness behavior, scan sampled behaviors, focal sampled behaviors, and approach test due to variation in the number of observation points for each type of data. Each model was run twice, once with CA1 as the outcome variable and once with CA2 as the outcome variable.

2.5.4. Sickness Behavior (SB)

Food intake, urination, and defecation were the independent variables, cluster was the dependent variable and day (n = 3) was the random effect. Cage use and other SB were removed from analysis due to lack of variability and low frequency, respectively.

2.5.5. Scan Sampling

Behavior (affiliative, aggressive, agonistic), position in cage (front half, rear half, in litter pan, in hide box, perching) and vocalizations (none, meow, hiss/growl) collected by scan sampling were the independent variables, cluster was the dependent variable and observation (14 per cat) was the random effect.

2.5.6. Focal Sampling

Duration of observation time spent occupied in 12 behaviors were included in the model; position in cage (front half, rear half, hide box, perch), alert relaxed, rest, groom, alert tense, freeze, eat, attempt to hide, turn away. The other variables on the ethogram were removed due to infrequency of observation (<10%). Cluster was the dependent variable and observation (12 per cat) was the random effect.

2.5.7. Approach Test

Latency to approach a familiar and an unfamiliar person and response to approach of a familiar and an unfamiliar person were included as independent variables, cluster was the dependent variable and step of the test (n = 3) was the random effect. Duration of interaction with the familiar and unfamiliar person were removed from the analysis, as they were the inverse of latency to approach for the majority of cats.
All analyses were performed using STATA 15 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX, USA) and graphed in GraphPad Prism 7 (GraphPad Software Inc., La Jolla, CA, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Cluster Analysis

CA1: Grouping cats using guardian-reported sickness behavior and personality traits failed to predict response to the confinement experience. The only statistically significant result was food intake (coefficient −0.0969, 95% CI −0.1846, −0.00092, p = 0.003). The remainder of the paper will discuss the results of the models using clusters from CA2.
CA2: Table 1 presents the percentage of guardians reporting each personality trait in their cat. Cats in Cluster 1 were more likely to be described as shy, calm, mellow, and timid with strangers, while cats in Cluster 2 were more likely to be described as active, playful with people, social with strangers, curious, easygoing, needy, and social with familiar people.
The percentage of cats reported to exhibit sickness behaviors at least once per month is presented in Table 2. Cats in Cluster 2 were more likely to vomit, defecate outside the litter pan, groom excessively, and exhibit fearful, nervous, and aggressive behaviors than cats in Cluster 1.

3.2. Fecal Glucocorticoid Metabolite

No statistically significant differences were identified (cluster p = 0.28; day p = 0.16, interaction p = 0.26) by 2-way ANOVA although a trend was found of increased FGM concentrations for cats in Cluster 1 on days 2 and 3 compared to cats in Cluster 2 (Table 3, Figure 2).

3.3. Sickness Behavior (SB)

The percentages of cats eating greater than half of the offered food (p < 0.0001) and urinating in the litter pan at least once per day (p < 0.0001) were statistically significant. Cats in Cluster 1 were more likely to eat greater than half the offered food on days 2 and 3 and more likely to urinate in the litter pan on day 1 than cats in Cluster 2 (Table 4).

3.4. Scan-Sampled Behavior

Cats in Cluster 2 were more likely to exhibit affiliative or maintenance behaviors (p < 0.0001) and meow (p < 0.0001) during the study and did so earlier than cats in Cluster 1 (Table 4). Cats in Cluster 1 spent more time in the hide box while cats in Cluster 2 were more likely to be observed perching throughout the study (Table 4, Figure 3).

3.5. Focal Sampling Behavior

Cats in Cluster 1 spent more of the observation time in the front half of the cage and in the hide box than cats in Cluster 2 while cats in Cluster 2 spent the majority of their time perching (Table 4). Cats in Cluster 2 spent more of the observation time alert relaxed or resting while cats in Cluster 1 spent more time alert tense or freezing (Table 4).

3.6. Approach Test

Cats in Cluster 2 had a shorter latency to approach a familiar person (p < 0.0001), an unfamiliar person (p = 0.013) and showed more affiliative responses to approach by an unfamiliar person (p < 0.0001) than cats in Cluster 1 (Table 4, Figure 4).

4. Discussion

The guardian rating, behavioral coding, and physiologic stress response (FGM) data collected in this study suggest that individual differences in response to acute confinement in a cage exist and that guardian-reported cat personality traits were consistent with their observed behaviors when confined. Behavioral and physiologic response patterns suggest that cats in Cluster 1 had a reactive coping style while cats in Cluster 2 had a proactive coping style. Koolhaas and van Reenan [37] have proposed a 3-dimensional model of response patterns to stressors with coping style, emotionality, and sociality as independent dimensions. Coping style is a behavioral pattern that reflects the type of response an individual makes (qualitative) while emotionality is the intensity of the response reflected in the duration of the behavior and physiologic activation (quantitative). The third dimension, sociality, has been proposed as a separate trait that appears to be largely uncorrelated with other behavioral and physiological measures. Individuals with a proactive coping style tend to be less behaviorally flexible relying on previous experience to influence their responses while individuals with a reactive coping style are more flexible and cue-dependent, continually gathering information from the environment and then reacting. Therefore, individuals with a proactive coping style are more successful under stable conditions while those with a reactive coping style are more successful in variable conditions [26,32].
Two methods are commonly used when assessing animal personality or coping style: coding and rating. The rating method involves an observer making judgments about an individual animal’s behavioral traits based on experience with that individual and is increasingly used in large-scale internet-based studies of companion animals (e.g., [38]). The coding method involves researchers observing an animal’s behaviors and describing it in terms of personality traits (e.g., [30]). In this study, both methods were employed as it has been suggested that a combined approach may provide more information, increasing the validity and reliability of the measures [39,40,41].
The guardian-rated personality traits identified two groups—cats in Cluster 1 were rated as shy, calm, mellow, quiet, and timid with unfamiliar people whereas cats in Cluster 2 were rated as active, curious, independent, and social with both familiar and unfamiliar people. Behavioral coding during the confinement stressor largely agreed with the guardian-ratings. When challenged with confinement stress, the cats in Cluster 1 exhibited freezing behavior, were alert and tense, and spent much of the time in their hiding box. In contrast, the cats in Cluster 2 were resting or alert and relaxed and spent much of the time perching. Gourkow et al. [42] reported similar behavioral differences in response to confinement of 34 cats admitted to a shelter. Three dimensions of behavior were identified: 1 included behaviors such as hiding, freeze, flat, startle, retreat from humans; 2 included meow, scan, escape bouts, pacing, redirected aggression, and 3 included lie on side, sleep, friendly to humans, walk, eat, groom, and rub. The behaviors included in Dimension 1 appear to be similar to those of our Cluster 1 while Dimension 3 resembles our Cluster 2. We could not identify a group corresponding to Dimension 2.
In this study, we provided a housing environment that was quiet, predictable, and enriched, to minimize stress and promote acclimation [2,43]. Cats had the opportunity to hide and perch and the two clusters utilized the resources differently. While hiding behavior decreased and perching behavior increased across days in both groups, there was still a defined preference for hiding or perching based on cluster with cats in Cluster 1 preferring to hide and cats in Cluster 2 to perch. This suggests the importance of supporting individual animals’ needs by providing both hiding and perching opportunities to confined cats, as has been previously reported [2,43,44,45,46].
No statistically significant difference in FGM concentrations was identified. This is likely due to the infrequency of defecation; only eight cats voided on each day of the study. However, cats in Cluster 1 showed a trend toward increased FGM concentration on days 2 and 3 compared with day 1, consistent with a reactive coping style (Figure 2). Cats in Cluster 2 had a relatively constant FGM concentration across days that was lower than the cats in Cluster 1, consistent with a proactive coping style. FGM concentrations are considered a reliable metric of HPA-axis activation in cats, with the major route of glucocorticoid metabolite excretion being via feces [33]. Ellis et al. [47] found FGM concentrations to be elevated during week 1 gradually decreasing by week 5 in singly-caged cats studied over a 30-day period. The mean FGM was lower in week 1 than was observed in our current study, possibly due to differences in reporting; Ellis et al. [47] reported a weekly average whereas we assessed concentration daily. The current study may have captured the peak response, suggesting that all cats experienced stress during confinement and may not have fully acclimated by the end of the study. Affiliative and maintenance behavior, food intake and urination in the litter pan increased in all cats from day 1 to day 3 following the acclimation pattern previously reported [2,43]. It is possible that FGM was beginning to decrease as well and that a longer study duration might have indicated this given that Ellis et al. [47] reported a significant decrease in stress behavior from week 1 to week 2 while FGM concentrations took longer to return to baseline. Future research is needed to assess cats’ FGM responses during confinement and to determine if the trends identified here are consistent in a larger study population.
We also identified differences in cat sociability and found them to be consistent between the guardian-reported ratings and the coded behavioral observations. Guardians described cats in Cluster 1 as timid with strangers but friendly with familiar people. During confinement, they were slower to exhibit affiliative behavior, spent much of the time in their hide box, and in the approach test had a longer latency to approach the unfamiliar person compared to cats in Cluster 2. In contrast, cats in Cluster 2 were reported to be social with both familiar and unfamiliar people. When confined, they exhibited affiliative behavior early and throughout the study and meowed more often. Cat vocalizations are more common in the presence of a human and meowing is an intra-specific attention-seeking behavior [48,49] supporting the increased sociality of this cluster. Interestingly, during the approach test, the average latency to approach the familiar person was similar in both clusters (Figure 4a). However, cats in Cluster 1 had a longer latency and exhibited fewer affiliative behaviors in response to approach of an unfamiliar person (Figure 4b,d). There was high variability between individuals in both clusters, possibly supporting Koolhaas and van Reenan’s [37] third dimension of sociality. Regardless, this finding demonstrates the importance of consistent caretakers and suggests that development of a human–cat relationship may be formed in as little as 3 days, even for less sociable cats.
Differences in owner-reported sickness behavior between clusters were identified. Cats in Cluster 2 were more likely to vomit, defecate outside the litter pan, groom excessively, and exhibit fearful, nervous, and aggressive behaviors than cats in Cluster 1. Sickness behaviors have previously been reported to increase in response to unusual external events [10,11] in a laboratory environment. Increased aggression and defecating out of the litter box may be indicative of territorial behaviors consistent with a proactive coping style, as has been reported in rodents [32]. It is less clear why vomiting, excessive grooming, and fearful and nervous behaviors would be increased, but one explanation is that these cats may be less behaviorally flexible to changes in their environment, and therefore may experience more stress, which manifests in these behaviors. Future research should aim to look for an association between the predictability of the home environment and sickness behavior.
This study had several limitations. First, the tool used by guardians to rate personality traits was not validated and data were collected as a dichotomous variable, whereas a 5-point Likert scale of each trait is more often utilized. Even with this limitation, though, individual differences were identified. Second, we coded behavior only in one context—cage confinement. Ideally, many contexts across time should be assessed to ascertain consistent individual responses. Finally, the sample size was small and since all cats were volunteered, volunteer bias may have been introduced.
Recognizing how individuals respond to stressful events or environments may help caretakers make informed, welfare-friendly decisions pertaining to cat housing, handling, enrichment, and resource allocation. This has the potential to improve cat welfare, facilitating better physical health, shorter times to adoption, and improved human–cat interactions. Future studies should aim to develop a validated personality assessment for cats and expand this investigation with a larger sample size in multiple contexts.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.S. and C.C.; Methodology, J.S. and C.C.; Software, J.S.; Validation, J.S. and C.C.; Formal Analysis, J.S.; Investigation, J.S. and C.C.; Resources, C.C.; Data Curation, J.S.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, J.S.; Writing—Review and Editing, J.S. and C.C.; Visualization, J.S. and C.C.; Supervision, C.C.; Project Administration, C.C.; Funding Acquisition, J.S. and C.C.

Funding

This research was funded by Morris Animal Foundation, grant number NCE-1–14077043.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the invaluable contributions of Jocelyn Bryant of the Chicago Zoological Society Endocrinology Lab for FGM analysis, Megan LaFollette for assistance with behavior coding of videos, and the Purdue University College of Veterinary Medicine faculty, staff, students and their cats who volunteered to participate.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare there are no conflicts. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Cat History Form
Owner name____________________________ Cat’s name______________ Date_____________
Phone#__________________________ E-mail __________________________
Breed________________ Date of Birth __________________Weight _____________________l b/kg
Gender: (circle one) female male spay/neutered Color:_________________
Declawed? No___ Yes___ If yes, Front___ All___
How long have you owned your cat? ___________years ____________months
Total Cats_______Total Dogs________Other Pets______________Other people_______________
Housing: Apartment: ☐studio, ☐1–2 bedrooms, ☐3 or more bedrooms, ☐attached house/twin duplex, ☐attached house, 3 or more units, ☐single house, ☐other_________________________________________
Previous Illnesses or Surgeries
Please check the box that best applies to your cat
Diet—wet food
(brand name__________________________)
☐None☐25%☐50%☐75%☐100%
Diet—dry food
(brand name__________________________)
☐None☐25%☐50%☐75%☐100%
Litter type: (clumping, clay, recycled paper, etc.)_________________________________________________
How many hours each day, on average, does your cat spend indoors? (check one)☐0–6  ☐6–12  ☐12–18  ☐18–24  ☐ Indoor Only
If you have more than one cat, what is their relationship?
☐Not Related    ☐Littermate    ☐Parent-Offspring    ☐Single Cat Household    ☐Other
Where did you obtain your cat (source)?
☐Shelter   ☐Offspring from a pet I already own(ed)   ☐Purchased from a friend
☐Purchased from a breeder   ☐Purchased from a pet shop   ☐Gift ☐Stray adult   ☐ stray/orphan kitten   ☐Other______________
Frequency of Sickness Behavior
For items below, please use the following choices to describe how many times you have seen your pet experience the symptom, adding comments/explanation—as appropriate Score =
0 = I have NEVER seen it         3 = I see it at least ONCE per MONTH
1 = I have seen it at least ONCE      4 = I see it at least ONCE per WEEK
2 = I see it at least ONCE per YEAR     5 = I see it DAILY
ScoreHow Often Does Your Cat:Comments/Explanation
1 Have excessive appetite
2 Have little appetite
3 Vomit (food, hair, bile, other)
4 Have diarrhea
5 Have constipation
6 Defecate outside the litter box
7 Strain or have frequent attempts to urinate
8 Urinate outside the litter box
9 Have blood in the urine
10 Spray urine
11 Grooms excessively
12 Have excessive hair loss
13 Scratch themselves excessively
14 Have discharge from eyes
15 Seem nervous (anxious)
16 Seem fearful
17 Seem aggressive
18 Seem “needy” of contact or attention
Personality Traits
How would you best describe your cat’s personality in your home? Circle all that apply
  • Active
  • Shy
  • Calm
  • Mellow
  • Playful with people
  • Vocal
  • Likes attention but is not demanding
  • Social with strangers
  • Curious
  • Independent
  • Quiet
  • Easy going
  • Playful with toys
  • Needy, forces attention
  • Social with familiar people
  • Timid with strangers
Client Resource Checklist
The following questions we ask about your cat’s resources because we want to learn more about your cat’s environment. Please ✓ DK if you don’t know, NA if a question does not apply to your home, or Yes or No after each question.
SpaceDKNAYesNoComments
1Does each cat have its own resting area in a convenient location that provides some privacy?
2Are resting areas located such that another animal cannot sneak up on the cat while it rests?
3Are resting areas located away from appliances or air ducts (machinery) that could come on unexpectedly while the cat rests?
4Does each cat have the opportunity to move to a warmer or cooler area if it chooses to?
Food and Water
5Does each cat have its own food bowl?
6Does each cat have its own water bowl?
7Are the bowls located in a convenient location that provides some privacy while it eats or drinks?
8Are bowls located such that another animal cannot sneak upon this cat while it eats or drinks?
9Are bowls located away from machinery that could come on unexpectedly?
Litter boxes
10Does each cat have its own box in a convenient, well-ventilated location that still gives the cat some privacy while using it (1 litter box per cat + 1)?
11Are boxes located in more than one area of the house?
12Are boxes located so another animal cannot sneak up on the cat during use?
13Are boxes located away from machinery that could come on unexpectedly during use?
14Are boxes washed regularly (at least monthly) with a mild detergent (like dishwashing liquid), rather than strongly scented cleaners?
Social Contact
15Does each cat have the opportunity to engage in play with other animals or the owner if it chooses to on a daily basis?
16Does each cat have the option to disengage from other animals or people in the household at all times?
17How many hours a day are you in sight of your cat? ______(h/day)
18How many minutes a do you spend petting your cat? ____(min/day)
19How many minutes a do you spend playing with your cat? ____(min/day)
Body Care and Activity
20Are horizontal scratching posts provided?
21Are vertical scratching posts provided?
22Are chew items (e.g., cat-safe grasses) provided?
23Does each cat like to play with toys?
24Does each cat have toys to chase that mimic quickly moving prey?
25Does each cat have toys that can be picked up, carried, and tossed in the air?
26Are toys rotated on a regular basis (at least weekly) to provide novelty?

Appendix B

Cage Factors recorded daily.
BehaviorOutcomesDescription
Food IntakeAte more than 50% of the offered food
Ate less than 50% of the offered food
UrinationIn litter panUrine is deposited in the litter pan
Out of litter panUrine is deposited out of the litter pan
No urineNo urine is observed in the cage
DefecationIn litter panFeces are deposited in the litter pan
Out of litter panFeces are deposited out of the litter pan
No BMNo feces are observed in the cage
Other Sickness BehaviorsLower GI symptomsFeces are soft or diarrhea
Upper GI symptomsVomiting of hair, food, bile

Appendix C

Ethogram used for the observation of cats in cages.
Behavior Indicative of Positive Affect
Alert-relaxed: Cat may be calmly displaying maintenance or self-care behaviors (e.g., grooming, eating, scratching, etc.), lying ventrally or laterally belly may be partially exposed, sitting, or standing, tail is up, loosely downward, or loosely wrapped, the head may or may not be resting on a surface, the pupils are not dilated.
Rest: The cat is in a lateral or curled position with the eyes closed and head hanging below the line of the body, resting on the paws or surface. Respiration rate is decreased, the tail is loosely wrapped or extended. The eyes are closed or half closed and possibly blinking slowly. The belly is often exposed.
Eat: The cat consumes food. Drink: The cat laps water or other liquid.
Groom: The cat licks its body or its paw and pass the paw over its head.
Approach: Cat comes toward the observer.
Rub: Cat rubs its body, head, or tail along the ground or object.
Tail up: Cat raises its tail to a vertical position.
Other Behaviors:
  • Exploratory behavior: The cat may be sniffing, rearing, playing with objects.
    • Stretch: The cat extends itself or its limbs to full length.
    • Yawn: To open the mouth wide with a deep inhalation, usually involuntarily from drowsiness, fatigue, or boredom.
    • Play: Cat manipulates an object with its paws in an apparently playful manner. The cat may pat, throw, pounce or wrestle with the object.
      • Self—Cat plays with its own body, usually the tail.
      • Social—Cat directs play at another cat or the observer.
    • Climb: Cat ascends an object.
    • Knead: Cat presses and stretches its paws on a surface, alternating feet.
    • Scratch: Object—Cat repeatedly scrapes its extended claws against a rough surface.
      • Self—Cat scratches itself as if it has an itch.
Behavior indicative of negative affect
Alert/tense Cat remains generally inactive with eyes fully open, muscles are tensed ready to react. Cat may be in a crouched or flattened body posture, displaying quick, frequent eye, ear and/or head movement scanning the environment. Cats frequently display fully dilated pupils, erect ears, and have an increased respiratory rate. Tail may be twitching, held low, or wrapped tight around the body. The belly is not exposed.
Freeze: Cat doesn’t move from its position. May be feigning sleep. Respiratory rate is increased, muscles are tensed ready to react. The cat is lying ventrally or crouching without moving from the position. The tail is tightly wrapped around the body. The legs are held tight and the belly is not visible. The head has little or no movement and is held in the same plane of the body. If eyes are open, the pupils are fully dilated. If the eyes are closed they may be slits or occasionally quickly flick open. The ears may be erect and twitch occasionally.
Crouch: The cat is positioned with all four feet in contact with the ground with its legs bent. The tail may be flicking or wrapped tightly around the body.
Lip lick: Cat licks its lips briefly. This is a non-appetitive behavior.
Attempt to hide: The cat tries to conceal itself in a location other than the hide box. This may be: in the litter box, crouched behind the litter box, behind a disrupted towel, or behind the hide box.
Turned away: The cat has its entire body or head and neck positioned away from the observer, camera, or front of the cage.
Other Behaviors:
  • Agonistic: Cat reacts to another cat in a nearby cage by hissing, growling, withdrawing to rear of cage, or displaying other aggressive behaviors.
  • Startle: Cat starts or jumps involuntarily, as by surprise or alarm.
  • Attempt to escape (<5%): Cat tries to get out of the cage; may scratch at the door, wall, and floor, with or without vocalizing. May be rearing
  • Lunge (<5%): Cat makes a sudden movement forward.
  • Flattened body (<5%): This is an extreme version of crouch. Cat often is behind something, has flattened ears, and is hiding its head and/or averting its gaze.
  • Avert gaze (<5%): The cat stops looking in the direction it was and quickly glances away from the observer, cat, room, etc.
Vocalizations
  • None: Cat makes no sound.
  • Meow: Cat makes a distinct sound, usually when it is trying to obtain something from another cat, but it is often directed toward human caretakers.
  • Growl: Cat makes a low-pitched rumbling noise.
  • Hiss: Cat makes a drawn-out SSSS sound, which is unvoiced.
Position in cage
  • Front: Cat is in the front half of the cage.
  • Rear: Cat is in the rear half of the cage.
  • In litter pan: Cat is positioned in the litter pan but is not urinating or defecating.
  • Hide: Cat is positioned in a hiding box.
  • Perch: Cat is positioned on an elevated structure.

References

  1. Amat, M.; Camps, T.; Manteca, X. Stress in owned cats: Behavioural changes and welfare implications. J. Fel. Med. Surg. 2016, 18, 577–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Stella, J.L.; Croney, C.C.; Buffington, T.B. Environmental Factors that Affect the Behavior and Welfare of Domestic Cats (Felis silvestris catus) Housed in Cages. J. App. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 160, 94–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Stella, J.L.; Croney, C.C. Environmental aspects of domestic cat care and management: Implications for cat welfare. Sci. World J. 2016, 2016, 6296315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Vinke, C.M.; Godijn, L.M.; van der Leij, W.J.R. Will a hiding box provide stress reduction for shelter cats? App. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 160, 86–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. McEwen, B.S. Central effects of stress hormones in health and disease: Understanding the protective and damaging effects of stress and stress mediators. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2008, 583, 174–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Broom, D.M.; Johnson, K. Stress and Animal Welfare, 2nd ed.; Kluwer Academic Publishers: Boston, MA, USA, 2000; ISBN 0 412 39580 0. [Google Scholar]
  7. Weiss, J.M. Effects of coping behavior in different warning signal conditions on stress pathology in rats. J. Comp. Physiol. Pychol. 1971, 77, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Weiss, J.M. Psychological factors in stress and disease. Sci. Am. 1972, 226, 104–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Bassett, L.; Buchanan-Smith, H.M. Effects of predictability on the welfare of captive animals. App. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 223–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Stella, J.L.; Lord, L.K.; Buffington, C.A.T. Sickness behaviors in response to unusual external events in healthy cats and cats with feline interstitial cystitis. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 2011, 238, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Stella, J.L.; Croney, C.C.; Buffington, C.A.T. Effects of Stressors on the Physiology and Behavior of Domestic Cats. J. App. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 143, 157–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Adamec, R.E. Anxious personality in the cat: Its ontogeny and physiology. In Psychopathology and the Brain; Carroll, B.J., Barrett, J.E., Eds.; Raven Press: New York, NY, USA, 1991; pp. 153–168. ISBN 9780199737055. [Google Scholar]
  13. Feaver, J.; Mendl, M.; Bateson, P. A method for rating the individual distinctiveness of domestic cats. Anim. Behav. 1986, 34, 1016–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Gottlieb, G.; Halpern, C.T. A relational view of causality in normal and abnormal development. Dev. Psychopathol. 2002, 14, 421–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Adamec, R.; Kent, P.; Anisman, H.; Shallow, T.; Merali, Z. Neural plasticity, neuropeptides and anxiety in animals-Implications for understaning and treating affective disorder following traumatic stress in humans. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 1998, 23, 301–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Boissy, A. Fear and Fearfulness in Animals. Q. Rev. Biol. 1995, 70, 165–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. MacKay, J.R.D.; Haskkell, M.J. Consistent Individual Behavioral Variation: The Difference between Temperament, Personality and Behivoral Syndromes. Animals 2015, 5, 455–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Gartner, M.C.; Weiss, A. Personality in felids: A review. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 144, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gartner, M.C.; Powell, D.M.; Weiss, A. Personality Structure in the Domestic Cat (Felis sivlestris catus), Scottish Wildcat (Felis silvestris grampia), Clouded Leopard (Neofelis nebulosa), Snow Leopard (Panthera uncia), and African Lion (Panthera leo): A Comparative Study. J. Comp. Psychol. 2014, 128, 414–426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Litchfield, C.A.; Quinton, G.; Tindle, H.; Chiera, B.; Kikillus, K.H.; Roetman, P. The ‘Feline Five’: An exploration of personality in pet cats (Felis catus). PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0183455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Bennett, P.C.; Rutter, N.J.; Woodhead, J.K.; Howell, T.J. Assessment of domestic cat personality, as perceived by 416 owners, suggests six dimensions. Behav. Proc. 2017, 141, 273–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ha, D.; Ha, J. A subjective domestic cat (Felis silvestris catus) temperament assessment results in six independent dimensions. Behav. Proc. 2017, 141, 351–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Capitanio, J.P. Nonhuman primate personality and immunity: Mechanisms of health and disease. In Personality and Temperament in Nonhuman Primates; Weiss, A., King, J.E., Murray, L., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 233–255. ISBN 9781461429715. [Google Scholar]
  24. Ironson, G.H.; O’Cleirigh, C.; Weiss, A.; Schneiderman, N.; Costa, P.T. Personality and HIV disease progression: Role of NEO-PI-R openness, extraversion, and profiles of engagement. Psychosom. Med. 2008, 70, 245–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Natoli, E.; Ludovic, S.; Cafazzo, S.; Bonanni, R.; Schmid, M.; Pontier, D. Bold attitude makes male urban feral domestic cats more vulnerable to Feline Immunodeficiency Virus. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005, 29, 151–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Koolhaas, J.M.; Korte, S.M.; De Boer, S.F.; Van Der Vegt, B.J.; Van Reenen, C.G.; Hopster, H.; De Jong, I.C.; Ruis, M.A.W.; Blokhuis, H.J. Coping styles in animals: Current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neuro. Biobehav. Rev. 1999, 23, 925–935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Castanheira, M.F.; Conceicao, L.E.C.; Millot, S.; Rey, S.; Begout, M.L.; Damsgard, B.; Kristiansen, T.; Hoglund, E.; Overli, O.; Martins, C.I.M. Coping styles in farmed fish: Consequences for aquaculture. R. Aquaculture 2018, 9, 23–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Zidar, J.; Balogh, A.; Favati, A.; Jensen, P.; Leimar, O.; Lovlie, H. A comparison of animal personality and coping styles in the red junglefowl. Anim. Behav. 2017, 130, 209–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. De Boer, S.F.; Buwalda, B.; Koolhaas, J.M. Untangling the neurobiology of coping styles in rodents: Towards neural mechanisms underlying individual differences in disease susceptability. Neuro. Biobehav. Rev. 2016, 74, 401–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Horvath, Z.; Igyarto, B.Z.; Magyar, A.; Miklosi, A. Three different coping styles in police dogs exposed to a short-term challenge. Horm. Behav. 2007, 52, 621–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Ferreira, R.; Mendl, M.; Wagner, P.G.C.; Araujo, T.; Nunes, D.; Mafra, A.L. Coping strategies in captive capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 176, 120–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Koolhaas, J.M.; de Boer, S.F.; Coppens, C.M.; Bewalda, B. Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: Towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2010, 31, 307–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Palme, R. Non-invasive measurment of glucocorticoids: Advances and problems. Physiol. Behav. 2018, 199, 229–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Schatz, S.; Palme, R. Measurement of Faecal Cortisol Metabolites in Cats and Dogs: A Non-invasive Method for Evaluating Adrenocortical Function. Vet. Res. Commun. 2001, 25, 271–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Stella, J.L.; Croney, C.C. Management practices of cats owned by faculty, staff, and students at two Midwest veterinary schools. Sci. World J. 2016, 2016, 7108374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Marston, L.C.; Bennett, P.C. Admissions of Cats to Animal Welfare Shelters in Melbourne, Australia. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2009, 12, 189–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Koolhaas, J.M.; van Reenan, C.G. Animal behavior and Well-being Symposium: Interaction between coping style/personality, stress, and welfare: Relevance for domestic farm animals. J. Anim. Sci. 2016, 94, 2284–2296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Finka, L.R.; Ward, J.; Farnsworth, M.J.; Mills, D.S. Owner personality and the wellbeing of their cats share parallels with the parent-child relationship. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0211862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Hill, H.M.; Yeater, D.; Lenhart, E.; Highfill, L. A Comparative Perspective on Personality. In Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences; Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T.K., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; pp. 1–12. ISBN 978 3 319 24610 9. [Google Scholar]
  40. Mather, J. Individual differences. In Comparative Psychology: A Handbook; Greenberg, G., Haraway, M.M., Eds.; Garland Publishing: New York, NY, USA, 1998; pp. 134–137. ISBN 9781138971332. [Google Scholar]
  41. Vazire, S.; Gosling, S.D. Personality and temperament: A comparative perspective. In Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior; Bekoff, M., Ed.; Greenwood Publishing Group: Westport, CT, USA, 2004; pp. 818–822. ISBN 9780313327469. [Google Scholar]
  42. Gourkow, N.; LaVoy, A.; Dean, G.A.; Phillips, C.J.C. Associations of behavior with secretory immunoglobulin A and cortisol in domestic cats during their first week in an animal shelter. App. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 150, 55–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Stella, J.L.; Croney, C.C.; Buffington, T.B. Behavior and Welfare of Domestic Cats Housed in Cages Larger than U.S. Norm. J. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2017, 3, 296–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Gourkow, N.; Fraser, D. The effect of housing and handling practices on the welfare, behaviour and selection of domestic cats (Felis sylvestris catus) by adopters in an animal shelter. Anim. Welf. 2006, 15, 371–377. [Google Scholar]
  45. Kry, K.; Casey, R. The effect of hiding enrichment on stress levels and behaviour of domestic cats (Felis sylvestris catus) in a shelter setting and the implications for adoption potential. Anim. Welf. 2007, 16, 375–383. [Google Scholar]
  46. Roclitz, I. Feline welfare issues. In The Domestic Cat: The Biology of Its Behavior, 2nd ed.; Turner, D.C., Bateson, P., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2000; pp. 207–226. ISBN 0 521 63648 5. [Google Scholar]
  47. Ellis, J.J.; Protopapadaki, V.; Stryhn, H.; Spears, J.; Cockram, M.S. Behavioural and faecal lucocorticoid metabolite responses of single caging in six cats over 30 days. Vet. Rec. Open 2014, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Turner, D. A review of over three decades of research on cat-human and human-cat relationships. Behav. Proc. 2017, 141, 297–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Yeon, S.C.; Kim, Y.K.; Park Se, J.; Lee, S.S.; Suh, E.H.; Houpt, K.A.; Lee, H.C.; Yang, B.G.; Lee, H.J. Differences between vocalization evoked by social stimuli in feral cats and house cats. Behav. Proc. 2011, 87, 183–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Study cat during confinement showing cage set-up.
Figure 1. Study cat during confinement showing cage set-up.
Animals 09 00370 g001
Figure 2. Fecal glucocorticoid metabolite (FGM) concentrations (ng/g) by cluster and day (mean and standard deviation).
Figure 2. Fecal glucocorticoid metabolite (FGM) concentrations (ng/g) by cluster and day (mean and standard deviation).
Animals 09 00370 g002
Figure 3. Percentage of cats in the front half, hide box, or perch by cluster during scan sampling.
Figure 3. Percentage of cats in the front half, hide box, or perch by cluster during scan sampling.
Animals 09 00370 g003
Figure 4. Latency to approach a familiar person (a) or unfamiliar person (b) and response to approach of a familiar (c) or unfamiliar person (d).
Figure 4. Latency to approach a familiar person (a) or unfamiliar person (b) and response to approach of a familiar (c) or unfamiliar person (d).
Animals 09 00370 g004
Table 1. Percentage of cats rated by their guardians with each personality trait.
Table 1. Percentage of cats rated by their guardians with each personality trait.
Personality TraitCluster 1 (n = 22)Cluster 2 (n = 33)
Active989
Shy556
Calm4127
Mellow4127
Playful with people576
Vocal3648
Likes attention but is not demanding3648
Social with strangers2367
Curious591
Independent5573
Quiet4115
Easygoing2770
Playful with toys5076
Needy, forces attention1842
Social with familiar people4179
Timid with strangers5921
Table 2. Guardian-reported frequency of sickness behaviors by cluster (% reporting at least 1/month).
Table 2. Guardian-reported frequency of sickness behaviors by cluster (% reporting at least 1/month).
How Often Does Your Cat:Cluster 1 (n = 22)Cluster 2 (n = 33)
Have excessive appetite4136
Have little appetite50
Vomit (food, hair, bile, other)1439
Have diarrhea03
Have constipation01
Defecate outside the litter pan012
Strain or have frequent attempts to urinate00
Urinate outside the litter box99
Have blood in the urine00
Spray urine56
Grooms excessively09
Have excessive hair loss53
Scratch themselves excessively53
Have discharge from eyes93
Seem nervous (anxious) 527
Seem fearful1830
Seem aggressive915
Seem “needy” of contact or attention6867
Table 3. Fecal glucocorticoid metabolite (FGM) concentration (ng/g) by cluster and day.
Table 3. Fecal glucocorticoid metabolite (FGM) concentration (ng/g) by cluster and day.
DayClusterN (%)MeanSDMinMax
117 (32)3753251591054
218 (55)79293493451
2111 (50)118511951233757
215 (45)5313411481300
3111 (50)16792923931061
219 (58)9226981742130
Table 4. Results of a mixed effects generalized linear model (n = 55). FP familiar person; UFP unfamiliar person.
Table 4. Results of a mixed effects generalized linear model (n = 55). FP familiar person; UFP unfamiliar person.
PredictorCoefficient95% CIp-Value
Sickness behaviors
Food Intake0.10390.07, 0.14<0.0001
Urination−0.1798−0.22, −0.14<0.0001
Defecation−0.0735−0.1799, 0.03290.18
Scan sampled behaviors
Behavior−0.0882−0.1261, −0.0503<0.0001
Position0.05330.0357, 0.0709<0.0001
Vocalizations0.16890.1192, 0.2187<0.0001
Focal sampled behaviors
Front half−0.0022−0.0026, −0.0018<0.0001
Rear half−0.0017−0.0022, −0.0013<0.0001
Hide −0.0014−0.0018, −0.0009<0.0001
Perch −0.0008−0.0011, −0.0004<0.0001
Alert relaxed−0.0007−0.0015, 0.000020.06
Rest −0.0011−0.0019, −0.00030.009
Groom −0.00002−0.0008, 0.000770.9
Eat 0.0020.0001, 0.00230.03
Alert tense−0.0011−0.0019, 0.00020.02
Freeze −0.0021−0.0028, −0.0014<0.0001
Attempt to hide−0.002−0.0026, −0.0015<0.0001
Turned away−0.0002−0.0007, 0.00040.5
Approach tests
Latency to approach FP0.00370.0035, 0.0039<0.0001
Latency to approach UFP0.00460.0009, 0.00820.013
Response to approach FP−0.0297−0.0853, 0.02580.3
Response to approach UFP0.15290.0858, 0.2199<0.0001

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Stella, J.; Croney, C. Coping Styles in the Domestic Cat (Felis silvestris catus) and Implications for Cat Welfare. Animals 2019, 9, 370. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9060370

AMA Style

Stella J, Croney C. Coping Styles in the Domestic Cat (Felis silvestris catus) and Implications for Cat Welfare. Animals. 2019; 9(6):370. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9060370

Chicago/Turabian Style

Stella, Judith, and Candace Croney. 2019. "Coping Styles in the Domestic Cat (Felis silvestris catus) and Implications for Cat Welfare" Animals 9, no. 6: 370. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9060370

APA Style

Stella, J., & Croney, C. (2019). Coping Styles in the Domestic Cat (Felis silvestris catus) and Implications for Cat Welfare. Animals, 9(6), 370. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9060370

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop