Do Human Assertions Really Adhere Strictly to Norms? The Effect of Threatening Content in Information on Personalized Norm Perception
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Norm of Assertion and Its Research Progress
1.2. Justified-Belief Norm Facing Shocks and Potential Hypotheses
1.3. The Potential Value and Significance of Testing the above Hypotheses
1.4. Research Program
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Participants, Materials, and Procedure
- General & Justify Belief—Jack is waiting for a flight to Russia at New York’s Kennedy International Airport. An elderly woman asks him if he can tell her which boarding gate the flight to France departs from. Jack remembers seeing a list of boarding gate information where the only gate listed for flights to France was Gate 24.
- Threatening & Justify Belief—Jack is waiting for a flight to Russia at New York’s Kennedy International Airport. An elderly woman asks him if she can board a flight to France through Gate 24. Jack recalls seeing a notice stating that Gate 24 has serious structural issues and could collapse, potentially harming those passing through.
- General & Belief—Jack is waiting for a flight to Russia at New York’s Kennedy International Airport. An elderly woman asks him if he can tell her which boarding gate the flight to France departs from. Jack couldn’t find any information regarding boarding gates, but he has a feeling that the flight to France leaves from Gate 24, so he believes it might be there.
- Threatening & Belief—Jack is waiting for a flight to Russia at New York’s Kennedy International Airport. An elderly woman asks him if she can board a flight to France through Gate 24. Jack didn’t see any notice, but suddenly has a premonition that there might be a risk of the building collapsing as the woman passes through Gate 24, so he believes she might be in danger.
- Assertion Question 1—Do you think Jack can say, “The flight to France is at Gate 24”? (Yes/No)
- Assertion Question 2—Do you think Jack should say, “The flight to France is at Gate 24”? (Yes/No)
- Truth Question—Is the boarding gate for the flight to France really at Gate 24? (Yes/No)
- Belief Question—Does Jack believe that the boarding gate for the flight to France is at Gate 24? (Yes/No)
- Justify Question—Does Jack’s belief that “the flight to France is at Gate 24” have reasonable evidence? (Yes/No)
- Assertion Question 1—Do you think Jack can say, “There’s danger at Gate 24”? (Yes/No)
- Assertion Question 2—Do you think Jack should say, “There’s danger at Gate 24”? (Yes/No)
- Truth Question—Is there really a possibility of danger at Gate 24? (Yes/No)
- Belief Question—Does Jack believe that the elderly woman might encounter danger passing through Gate 24? (Yes/No)
- Justify Question—Does Jack’s belief that “the elderly woman might encounter danger” have good evidence? (Yes/No)
2.2. Results
2.3. Discussion
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Participants, Materials, and Procedure
- General Condition & Belief—Mary stands at the entrance of an ophthalmology hospital. A young person asks her if this hospital performs the LINU surgery to improve eyesight. Mary responds, “This hospital doesn’t perform the LINU surgery”. It’s important to note that Mary isn’t certain if the hospital does perform this surgery. She heard one claim stating the hospital lacks doctors skilled in this technique, but later heard another claim that the hospital does possess this technology. Nevertheless, Mary firmly believes the hospital doesn’t possess the LINU technology.
- Threatening Condition & Belief—Mary stands at the entrance of an ophthalmology hospital. A young person asks her if the LINU technique for improving eyesight is safe. Mary responds, “The LINU technique isn’t safe”. It’s important to note that Mary isn’t sure about the safety of the LINU technique. She heard one claim stating the LINU technique isn’t safe and might lead to deteriorating eyesight after a dozen years. However, she later heard another claim stating LINU is safe. Overall, Mary firmly believes LINU is an unsafe technique.
- General Condition & No Belief (hesitation)—Mary stands at the entrance of an ophthalmology hospital. A young person asks her if this hospital performs the LINU surgery to improve eyesight. Mary responds, “This hospital doesn’t perform the LINU surgery”. It’s important to note that Mary isn’t certain if the hospital performs this surgery. She heard one claim stating the hospital lacks doctors skilled in this technique, but later heard another claim that the hospital does possess this technology. In conclusion, Mary feels uncertain about the hospital’s possession of the LINU technology, her belief vacillates.
- Threatening Condition & No Belief (hesitation)—Mary stands at the entrance of an ophthalmology hospital. A young person asks her if the LINU technique for improving eyesight is safe. Mary responds, “The LINU technique isn’t safe”. It’s important to note that Mary isn’t sure about the safety of the LINU technique. She heard one claim stating the LINU technique isn’t safe and might lead to deteriorating eyesight after a dozen years. However, she later heard another claim stating LINU is safe. In conclusion, Mary feels uncertain about the safety of the LINU technique, her belief vacillates.
- Assertion Question 1: Should Mary say “This hospital cannot perform the LINU surgery”? (Yes/No)
- Assertion Question 2: Is Mary permitted to say “This hospital cannot perform the LINU surgery”? (Yes/No)
- Truth Question: Can this hospital really not perform the LINU surgery? (Yes/Uncertain/No)
- Belief Question: Does Mary believe “This hospital cannot perform the LINU surgery”? (Yes/Uncertain/No)
- Blame Question: Do you think Mary’s behavior should be blamed? (Yes/No)
- Assertion Question 1: Should Mary say “LINU surgery is unsafe”? (Yes/No)
- Assertion Question 2: Is Mary permitted to say “LINU surgery is unsafe”? (Yes/No)
- Truth Question: Is LINU surgery really unsafe? (Yes/Uncertain/No)
- Belief Question: Does Mary believe “LINU surgery is unsafe”? (Yes/Uncertain/No)
- Blame Question: Do you think Mary’s behavior should be blamed? (Yes/No)
3.2. Results
3.3. Discussion
4. General Discussion
4.1. The Effect of Threatening Content on Individuals’ Perceptions of Assertions
4.2. The Effect of Threatening Content on “Justified-Belief Norm”
4.3. Potential Inspiration for Other Research Questions Arising
4.4. Research Gaps and Possible Research Directions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Green, M.S. Assertion; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2017; Volume 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marsili, N.; Green, M. Assertion: A (partly) social speech act. J. Pragmat. 2021, 181, 17–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boult, C. Epistemic normativity and the justification-excuse distinction. Synthese 2017, 194, 4065–4081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reynolds, S.L. Testimony, Knowledge, and Epistemic Goals. Philos. Stud. 2002, 110, 139–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turri, J. The test of truth: An experimental investigation of the norm of assertion. Cognition 2013, 129, 279–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Turri, J. Revisiting norms of assertion. Cognition 2018, 177, 8–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kneer, M. The norm of assertion: Empirical data. Cognition 2018, 177, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benton, M.A. Expert Opinion and Second-Hand Knowledge. Philos. Phenomenol. Res. 2016, 92, 492–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blaauw, M.J. Reinforcing the knowledge account of assertion. Analysis 2012, 72, 105–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brogaard, B. Intellectual Flourishing as the Fundamental Epistemic Norm. In Epistemic Norms; Littlejohn, C., Turri, J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2014; pp. 11–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schaffer, J. Knowledge in the Image of Assertion. Philos. Issues 2008, 18, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Turri, J. The Express Knowledge Account of Assertion. Australas. J. Philos. 2011, 89, 37–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williamson, T. Knowing and Asserting. Philos. Rev. 1996, 105, 489–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williamson, T. Knowledge and its Limits; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Weiner, M. Must We Know What We Say? Philos. Rev. 2005, 114, 227–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bach, K. Applying pragmatics to epistemology. Philos. Issues 2008, 18, 68–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bach, K.; Harnish, R. Linguistic Communication and Speech Acts; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Douven, I. Assertion, Knowledge, and Rational Credibility. Philos. Rev. 2006, 115, 449–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lackey, J. Norms of Assertion. Noûs 2007, 41, 594–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kneer, M. Norms of assertion in the United States, Germany, and Japan. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2021, 118, e2105365118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marsili, N.; Wiegmann, A. Should I say that? An experimental investigation of the norm of assertion. Cognition 2021, 212, 104657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jungherr, A.; Schroeder, R. Disinformation and the Structural Transformations of the Public Arena: Addressing the Actual Challenges to Democracy. Soc. Media Soc. 2021, 7, 2056305121988928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kappes, A.; Harvey, A.H.; Lohrenz, T.; Montague, P.R.; Sharot, T. Confirmation bias in the utilization of others’ opinion strength. Nat. Neurosci. 2020, 23, 130–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomaszewski, T.; Morales, A.; Lourentzou, I.; Caskey, R.; Liu, B.; Schwartz, A.; Chin, J. Identifying False Human Papillomavirus (HPV) Vaccine Information and Corresponding Risk Perceptions from Twitter: Advanced Predictive Models. J. Med. Internet Res. 2021, 23, e30451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bao, L.; Peng, C.; He, J.; Sun, C.; Feng, L.; Luo, Y. The Relationship between Fear Avoidance Belief and Threat Learning in Postoperative Patients After Lung Surgery: An Observational Study. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2023, 16, 3259–3267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhang, S.; Zheng, J.; Mo, L. The effect of the brightness metaphor on memory. Psychol. Res. Psychol. Forsch. 2022, 86, 1751–1762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Puttlitz, M.H.; Chivers, D.P.; Kiesecker, J.M.; Blaustein, A.R. Threat-sensitive predator avoidance by larval pacific treefrogs (Amphibia, Hylidae). Ethology 1999, 105, 449–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rieucau, G.; Boswell, K.M.; Robertis, A.D.; Macaulay, G.J.; Handegard, N.O. Experimental Evidence of Threat-Sensitive Collective Avoidance Responses in a Large Wild-Caught Herring School. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e86726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Betz-Richman, N. Lying, hedging, and the norms of assertion. Synthese 2022, 200, 176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ceccarini, F.; Capuozzo, P.; Colpizzi, I.; Caudek, C. Breaking (Fake) News: No Personal Relevance Effect on Misinformation Vulnerability. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fabian, M.; Pykett, J. Be Happy: Navigating Normative Issues in Behavioral and Well-Being Public Policy. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2022, 17, 169–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Enders, A.M.; Uscinski, J.E.; Klofstad, C.A.; Seelig, M.I.; Wuchty, S.; Murthi, M.N.; Premaratne, K.; Funchion, J.R. Do Conspiracy Beliefs Form a Belief System? Examining the Structure and Organization of Conspiracy Beliefs. J. Soc. Political Psychol. 2021, 9, 255–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, C.; Zhou, R.; Zhang, X. Positive Childhood Experiences and Depression Among College Students During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Moderated Mediation Model. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2023, 16, 4105–4115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zhang, S.; Diao, J.; Huang, J.; Liu, Y.; Mo, L. Do Human Assertions Really Adhere Strictly to Norms? The Effect of Threatening Content in Information on Personalized Norm Perception. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 625. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070625
Zhang S, Diao J, Huang J, Liu Y, Mo L. Do Human Assertions Really Adhere Strictly to Norms? The Effect of Threatening Content in Information on Personalized Norm Perception. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(7):625. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070625
Chicago/Turabian StyleZhang, Shijia, Jiangdong Diao, Jiahui Huang, Yanchi Liu, and Lei Mo. 2024. "Do Human Assertions Really Adhere Strictly to Norms? The Effect of Threatening Content in Information on Personalized Norm Perception" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 7: 625. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070625
APA StyleZhang, S., Diao, J., Huang, J., Liu, Y., & Mo, L. (2024). Do Human Assertions Really Adhere Strictly to Norms? The Effect of Threatening Content in Information on Personalized Norm Perception. Behavioral Sciences, 14(7), 625. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070625