Next Article in Journal
Improving Strategic Planning: The Crucial Role of Enhancing Relationships between Management Levels
Next Article in Special Issue
Managing Cultural Diversity and Conflict in Family Businesses: An Organizational Perspective
Previous Article in Journal
How Do the Labour Force Characteristics Encounter COVID-19 Economic Consequences—A Canadian Experience
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Disentangling Workplace Sexism in Age Generations: A Multi-Group Analysis on the Effects on Job Satisfaction and Task Performance

Section Psychology, Department of Educational Sciences, University of Catania, 95124 Catania, Italy
Adm. Sci. 2023, 13(9), 210; https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci13090210
Submission received: 11 August 2023 / Revised: 11 September 2023 / Accepted: 18 September 2023 / Published: 21 September 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Diversity, Equity & Inclusion and Its Perception in Organization)

Abstract

:
The gender gap is a current topical issue. Sexist assumptions that manifest as gender stereotypes are partially responsible for these inequalities. The ambivalent sexism theory argues that hostile sexism refers to explicitly antagonistic sexist attitudes, while benevolent sexism refers to apparently positive but implicitly malevolent attitudes. There has been evidence reported that benevolent sexism is detrimental to women’s personal and professional well-being, implies lower levels of career aspiration and impacts task performance. This study is aimed at examining the impact that the experience of benevolent and hostile sexism could have on performance and job satisfaction. A total of 402 female workers were enrolled. The results showed that an experience with benevolent sexism significantly decreased the positive relationship between work engagement, psychological capital and organisational support and outcomes. Conversely, hostile sexism only reduces job satisfaction in its interaction with work engagement and organisational support. Moreover, through a multi-group analysis, possible differences across age were examined in the theorised model. Here, the younger generation seems to be more affected and experience more benevolent sexism than the older generation, which is seen both in individual moderators and in their interactions with predictors. This study is helpful for a deeper comprehension of contemporary sexism, offering also suggestions for equality policies’ design.

1. Introduction

Measuring gender equality is a complex and difficult issue, reflecting its multidimensional nature. To assist in the analysis of gender equality in the various European states, the European Institute for Gender Equality developed a tool known as the Gender Equality Index. This tool measures progress in the current state of gender roles and focuses on specific areas in need of improvement and change, thus providing valuable suggestions and guidance for the policy design of more effective gender equality measures.
The Gender Equality Index tracks 32 different indicators under six different macro-dimensions: ‘Work’, ‘Money’, ‘Knowledge’, ‘Time’, ‘Power’ and ‘Health’. Looking at the results referring to the year 2022, gender inequality seems to persist despite the attempts and efforts of states and various institutions. Hence, gender equality still remains a goal to be pursued for most European countries. The overall score obtained by the European Union (EU), in fact, was 68.6 points out of 100. In addition, for the first time since this instrument was first measured and used, there was a decline in several areas due to the uneven impact of the COVID-19 pandemic. However, progress in some domains prevented an overall shift in gender equality. Trying to summarise the results from the Gender Equality Index, however, it seems that there is still much work left to be done to achieve full and satisfactory gender equality. According to this index, the most egalitarian countries are Sweden (83.9), Denmark (77.8) and the Netherlands (77.3), while the least egalitarian are Greece (53.4), Romania (53.7) and Hungary (54.2). Italy for example, the territory where this study took place, ranks 14th in the EU with a score of 65, just 3.6 points lower than the EU. Since 2010, Italy’s overall index has increased by 11.7 points, recording one of the largest long-term improvements among the Member States. A major step backwards, however, occurred in the work domain, where a not particularly high score (63.2 points) dropped further. As a result, Italy consistently ranks in last place among all the EU Member States in the work domain, with high levels of gender inequality, especially in the sub-domain of labour participation. Italy also seems to rank low with the gender employment gap (around 18%) and, again, with the percentage of employed women (51.1%). Reflecting on the current situation and discussing these inequalities and disparities does not seem utopian; on the contrary, the global pandemic seems to have exacerbated the difficulties already in place. Indeed, the COVID-19 pandemic was not only a health emergency but also contributed significantly to the deterioration of the economic crisis and the labour market in general. The impact on a global scale that people experienced is actually much broader and more complex than one might think. The economic and labour crisis caused by COVID-19 has significantly increased unemployment worldwide and worsened already precarious and difficult employment situations. This crisis also seems to have had a greater impact on certain groups of working men and women, thus increasing the inequalities. Among them, people in less protected and lower-paid jobs were those at greatest risk and who suffered the worst consequences, mainly including groups such as young and older workers, female workers and migrant workers. The pandemic crisis hit an economy and a society that already had low female employment rates, poor work–family reconciliation policies, very strict gender stereotypes and a very skewed, albeit very slowly evolving, division of labour in the family according to gender. The pandemic crisis has both accentuated these aspects and made visible the fragile balance on which they were held. In Italy, a country with a strong conservative tradition and rigid gender roles, the situation is certainly no different and opens up new and important reflections on inequalities and disparities, which deserve attention and in-depth study.
Although the social position of women in recent decades appears significantly improved in many areas including the workplace, it is still difficult to speak of true gender equality since gender gaps continue to exist. Indeed, in Europe and in Italy, the position of women compared to men has not changed in relation to employment rates, success and career choice/possibility or equal pay. An important indicator of the efforts towards achieving gender equality in the workplace was the presence of approximately equal numbers of women and men in leadership positions. However, the situation in organisational contexts is often not so equal. Gender differences actually persist and overwhelmingly influence workplace outcomes (Barreto et al. 2009).
The reasons underlying these differences are not women’s own attributes or attitudes; on the contrary, women are motivated and engaged in work activities in the same way and at the same level as men (Ellemers et al. 2004). A possible explanation can be found in gender discrimination. At the organisational level, sexist attitudes and beliefs are probably responsible for the approval of policies that hinder (rather than facilitate) the participation of women in the workforce (Benokraitis and Feagin 1986; Maier 1999). Several studies further prove that the endorsement of sexist beliefs (from male and female sources) is associated with the biased treatment of individual women, either in the personnel selection process, in the evaluation of their performance or in promotion decisions and opportunities (Bartol 1999; Ellemers et al. 2004; Heilman 2001). Despite these considerations, the literature reports that society currently tends to believe that gender discrimination is no longer a current problem (Swim et al. 1995; Tougas et al. 1995). Effectively, sexism still exists but has changed its nature by becoming more implicit, subtle and difficult to recognise (Zehnter et al. 2021; Morando et al. 2023). In spite of this, although expressed differently, sexist ideologies continue to be experienced and perceived by women, who are often affected and experience the strongest consequences.
The ambivalent sexism theory introduced by Glick and Fiske (1996, 1999) is an example of a contemporary form of sexism and argues that hostile sexism refers to explicitly antagonistic sexist attitudes, while benevolent sexism refers to apparently positive but implicitly malevolent attitudes (Acar and Sümer 2018). Furthermore, while hostile sexism is a more blatant form of sexism, benevolent sexism is a more subtle form of it. Both forms generally have concrete implications and significantly affect members of minority groups, although benevolent/subtle sexism has a differently stronger impact. Indeed, the literature on the topic suggests that more subtle forms of prejudgment influence individuals in the way they feel and act, even leading them to unconsciously behave in ways that help justify or perpetuate their own inferiority (Steele and Aronson 1995; Ellemers and Barreto 2008). Perceiving oneself as a victim of sexism has a strong and complex impact on an individual’s well-being and affects one’s existence in a holistic way. Perceiving oneself as a victim of discrimination implies devaluing the social group to which one belongs (a fundamental aspect of social identity) and represents a devaluation of a part of oneself, also communicating social exclusion to others (Branscombe et al. 1999; Schmitt et al. 2002). A woman who feels she is a target of gender discrimination, for example, not only has to face the negative outcome this entails but also has to realise that something fundamental about her (her gender) is devalued by others. Thus, while blatant sexism leads to attention and emotional and negative reactions to the source of the mistreatment, more subtle sexism leads to self-directed negative emotions, such as anxiety and self-directed anger (Barreto and Ellemers 2005). These emotions also influence attitudes and behaviour and, consistent with the concept of stereotype threat, cause the targets of discrimination to have poorer performance precisely because they believe themselves to be the targets of a negative stereotype, in a kind of self-fulfilling prophecy (Steele 1997).
Moreover, as benevolent sexism implicitly suggests women’s lack of competencewhile providing ambivalent messages and praise, it contributes to women doubting their ability to perform well and experiencing a decrease in self-esteem and self-confidence, as well as feeling anxious during tasks. This definitely impairs their performance (Dardenne et al. 2007). Different studies revealed the impact of benevolent sexism on women’s personal and professional well-being (Rubin et al. 2019; Dardenne et al. 2007; Moya et al. 2007), as better illustrated in the following Section 4.
Starting from these premises, the aim of the study is to examine the impact that the experience of benevolent and hostile sexism could have on performance and job satisfaction in a sample of female workers. To do so, common predictors in the literature that significantly predict the outcomes were selected, introducing experience with benevolent and hostile sexism as moderators and investigating their role in modifying the predictive power of the predictors. Specifically, work engagement and psychological capital were selected as personal variables and organisational support as a variable measuring the perception of the organisation. Work engagement, for example, was selected because it is one of the most important predictors with significant impacts on the level of job performance (Grobelna 2019) and for increasing job satisfaction (Zalewska 2020; Hakanen and Schaufeli 2012; Corbeanu and Iliescu 2023). Additionally, PsyCap was selected (in its individual components as well as a higher-order construct) due to it being commonly associated with job satisfaction (Görgens-Ekermans and Steyn 2016; Platania and Paolillo 2022). PsyCap has been found to predict task performance, whether it was self-rated, supervisor-rated or objectively measured (Avey et al. 2011). It has also been linked to contextual performance, viz., organisational citizenship behaviours and other forms of desirable performance criteria, including creative performance, problem-solving and innovation (Newman et al. 2014).
The literature maintains s that individuals’ POS helps boost their obligations toward an organisation in order to reciprocate favourably. There has been evidence reported that individuals’ POS enhances both in-role performance, such as goal attainment, and extra-role performance, such as helping and supportive behaviour toward coworkers (Eisenberger et al. 2001).
A meta-analysis conducted by Rhoades and Eisenberger (2002) revealed that favourable treatments, such as rewards from the organisation, beneficial working conditions and fairness, received by employees are directly linked to POS. Moreover, POS promotes auspicious outcomes such as high job satisfaction, lower turnover, enhanced dedication, positive emotions and better performance (Yu and Frenkel 2013; Koopmans et al. 2015).
Additionally, to provide a broader examination and understanding, the study aimed at examining, through a multi-group analysis, the possible difference across age in the theorised model.
Indeed, the literature provides us with some insight into how sexist attitudes might differ across the lifespan, but few studies have been carried out in relation to the possible different experiences with the two forms of sexism (hostile and benevolent). Surveys and polls seem to report that younger women who experience more sexism are more likely to experience negative effects on their health, well-being and work performance (Hammond et al. 2018; Buscemi et al. 2016).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedure

A total of 402 Italian female workers from Italian companies were enrolled for the study. The age of participants ranged between 18 and 64 (M = 35.31; S.D. = 4.91). In total, 47.4% of them work in a STEM workplace (science, technology, engineering and math) and 56.4% in a non-STEM workplace. Out of the 402 participants, 61.7% were employed in the private sector and 38.3% in the public sector. Of the companies involved, 27% belonged to large enterprises, 32% to medium-sized enterprises, 22% to small enterprises and 19% to micro enterprises. Furthermore, the majority of the sample reported having a full-time (78%) and open-ended (57%) contract. As for geographical distribution, the sample came from Northern Italy (19%), Central Italy (27%) and Southern Italy (54%).
The study was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was authorised by the Internal Ethics Committee of the Department of Education Sciences (Psychology Section) of the University of Catania (Ierb-Edunict-2020/2); the relevant research procedures followed all the guidelines of the AIP (Italian Psychology Association) and its Ethics Council.
Participants were recruited through convenience sampling, using an online survey. A survey link was posted on social media platforms, such as Facebook, Twitter and Instagram, and the survey link was sent to acquaintances via email and to the HR manager of some Italian companies in the period between October 2022 and May 2023. By clicking on the link, participants received an information sheet and an informed consent form, which, once accepted, led to the survey with instructions on how to complete it. Participation was voluntary.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Utrecht Work Engagement Scale

The Work engagement construct was measured using the 9-item Utrecht Work Engagement Scale. Schaufeli et al.’s (2003) scale, the Italian version of which is by Balducci et al. (2010), explores the experience of aspects of the construct through three item scales: Vigour (VI), Dedication (DE) and Absorption (AB). Item responses were given on a frequency scale, using a Likert scale ranging from 0 (never) to 6 (always).

2.2.2. Compound PsyCap Scale (CPC-12)

The Compound Psychological Capital Scale (CPC-12), conceptualised by Lorenz et al. (2016) and translated and validated in the Italian context by Platania and Paolillo (2022), was used to assess the psychological capital construct. The psychological capital construct is inspired by positive psychology and, more specifically, positive organisational behaviour. It is defined as a set of different positive individual resources that enable and influence the capital to achieve desired goals. The 12-item scale actually evaluates four different factors (optimism, resilience, hope and self-efficacy), grouped into one general factor known as psychological capital. Each item can be answered using a 7-point Likert agreement scale.

2.2.3. Perceived Organisational Support

Perceived organisational support was measured using the eight-item scale developed by Eisenberger et al. (1997), Italian adaptation by Battistelli and Mariani (2011). Employees were asked to rate the degree of perceived organisational support using a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree.

2.2.4. Experience with Ambivalent Sexism Inventory

For the measurement of experience with ambivalent sexism and hostile sexism, the Experience with Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (EASI) by Salomon et al. (2020) was chosen. Participants rated the frequency with which they encountered each of 28 hostile and benevolent sexism behaviours. All items were rated on 7-point scales from 1 = this has never happened to me to 7 = this has happened very frequently (more than twice per week). This inventory consists of four different factors: hostile sexism, heterosexual intimacy, benevolent sexism and heterosexual hostility. Specific to this study, items measuring hostile sexism (eight items) and benevolent sexism (eight items) were selected.

2.2.5. Generic Job Satisfaction Scale

For the measurement of the job satisfaction construct, the Generic Job Satisfaction Scale, by Macdonald and MacIntyre (1997), was selected. The choice of this scale depended on its advantages: it is brief, easily administered in workplaces and reliable for a wide variety of occupations. The scale consists of 10 items and has a 5-point Likert agreement scale as a response option.

2.2.6. Individual Work Performance Questionnaire (IWPQ)

The Individual Work Performance Questionnaire by Koopmans et al. (2015) was used to measure the concept of performance. Specifically for this study, only the items measuring task performance were selected. All participants responded to the five items using a 7-point Likert scale.
Task performance can be defined as “the competence with which individuals perform substantive or technical tasks that are fundamental to their work” (Campbell 1990). The behaviours used to describe performance often include quantity and quality of work, professional skills and knowledge (Campbell 1990; Rotundo and Sackett 2002).
On the task performance scale, work planning and organisation, result-oriented work, prioritisation and efficient work are evaluated.

2.3. Data Analysis

Data were analysed using SPSS 27.0, and AMOS 27.0. SPSS (version 27.0 for Windows; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) was used for the descriptive analysis and bivariate correlations of the variables in this study.
AMOS (version 27.0) and the structural equation Model (SEM) were used to test the fit of the measurement model, the interaction effects and the multigroup analysis. To evaluate the models’ goodness of fit, we used the comparative fit index (CFI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). χ2 values and ∆χ2 values were also used and were presented between the competing models, and the index ∆CFI was used with values not exceeding 0.01, indicating the equivalence of models in terms of fit (Meade et al. 2008).
The structural equation model (SEM) was also used to test moderation. The ‘interaction term’ method was used, in which a product term of the independent variable and the moderator was formed.
To evaluate invariance tests and the multigroup analysis, two group were extracted from the original sample: ‘younger workers’ (177, 44.03%) and ‘older workers’ (225, 55.97%). To perform this analysis and examine the differences, the sample was divided according to the commonly understood indications and subdivisions on generations; hence, it was considered the youngest workers to be those belonging to Generation Y (Millennials, born between 1983 and 1994) and Generation Z (born between 1995 and 2010), while the oldest were those belonging to the Boomers—those born between 1946 and 1964—and Generation X—those born between 1965 and 1982. Afterward, the maximum likelihood (ML) estimation method was used to adjust the model individually to each group, eliminating the items that did not contribute to the adjustment quality, and then the model estimation was tested across groups (Hayes and Scharkow 2013; Byrne 2010). The data analysis involved three different steps: testing measurement model invariance; testing structural model invariance; and testing structural-path-coefficient differences.
The invariance of the measurement model across the two groups was tested by comparing the unconstrained model (i.e., with all parameters free) to the model with measurement weights constrained (i.e., the measurement model per se). In the second step of this data analysis, the invariance of the structural model was tested across the two groups, considering the model with measurement weights constrained as being correct and comparing the model with the unconstrained structural coefficients to the constrained model (i.e., with structural weights constrained). Because of the results of the multigroup invariance, the differences in the structural path coefficients were investigated.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistic, Correlation and Reliability

The findings in Table 1 show the total mean scores for each variable of the sample. Furthermore, to estimate the internal consistency of the model, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was calculated for each of the variables examined. The construct validity of the model was examined in terms convergent validity and discriminant validity, which indicate the internal structure of the respective domains. Convergent validity was confirmed by the average variance extracted (AVE) and composite reliability (CR) values. The reliability analysis confirmed that the final model had a good level of internal consistency, as the Cronbach’s alpha value was above 0.8. The composite reliability values of the constructs ranged from 0.69 to 0.89, demonstrating convergent validity. Table 1 also reports the results of the intercorrelations; in our study, task performance had positive correlations with work engagement (r = 0.56), psychological capital (r = 0.39) and organisational support (r = 0.26). Similarly, job satisfaction was positively correlated with the same variables (WE (r = 0.38); PsyCap (r = 0.23); POS (r = 0.19)). Quite the reverse, the variables measuring experience with benevolent sexism and hostile sexism showed negative correlations with the other constructs, except for their intercorrelations (r = 0.66). This evidence was consistent with the current literature on the topic and the insights drawn for the elaboration of the following moderation and multi-group model.

3.2. Confirmatory Factor Analysis to Test the Model

In order to confirm the goodness of the model hypothesised and presented in Figure 1, a comparison of two different models was performed. A series of confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs), according to Harman’s one-factor test, were run in order to diagnose the extent to which the variance of the common method could be a problem. The CFAs were performed using robust maximum likelihood estimation to examine the structure of the constructs. Two different models were tested and compared.
The first model (1) included three second-order factors (work engagement, PsyCap, job satisfaction) and three first-order factors (experience with benevolent sexism, hostile sexism, task performance).
The results showed that the first model (1) provided a good fit to the data: χ2 [132] = 565.896, p < 0.001, χ2/df = 4.28, RMSEA = 0.06, CFI = 0.91, TLI = 0.90, and SRMR = 0.04.
In the second model (2), the CFA included the same factors, except that it considered all scales with a single-factor structure, in which all indicators were loaded on a single factor. The results of this model provided a worse fit to the data (χ2 [164] = 659.847, p < 0.001, χ2/df = 4.02, RMSEA = 0.12, CFI = 0.77, TLI = 0.69, and SRMR = 0.07). The differences were significant when comparing the chi-square values and degrees of freedom of both models (Δχ2 (32) = 93.951, p < 0.0). Model 1 showed the best fit to the data.

3.3. Main Effects and Moderating Effects

As suggested by Cohen et al. (2013), the moderation model was proposed and declined in three stages. In the first phase, the effect of the independent variables was tested, i.e., work engagement, psychological capital and organisational support on the dependent variables (job satisfaction and task performance). In the second phase, the effect of each of the moderator variables (experience with benevolent sexism and hostile sexism) on the dependent variables was tested. Finally, in the third stage, the interaction terms (each independent variable × each of the moderators) and their effect on the dependent variables were introduced. Before calculating the interaction terms, the predictor and moderator variables were centred to minimise multicollinearity between the interactions and their individual components (Aiken et al. 1991). To identify the form of moderation, when significant, the regression model was plotted at two values of the moderator variable, i.e., one standard deviation above the mean and one standard deviation below the mean. The results of the moderation analyses are presented in Table 2.
As displayed in Table 2, in Step 1, the perceived levels of organisational support, work engagement and psychological capital had a significant and positive impact on task performance and on job satisfaction. Conversely, in Step 2, experience with benevolent sexism and hostile sexism revealed a significant and negative impact on the same outcomes, task performance and job satisfaction, showing that it is harmful to women’s performance and wellbeing at work (Morando et al. 2023; Dardenne et al. 2007; Dumont et al. 2010; Jones et al. 2014; Vescio et al. 2005). Consistent with the literature, a different reaction and effect of women to hostile and benevolent sexism were found. Indeed, hostile sexism is reported to have a significantly less strong effect than the experience with benevolent sexism because it differently impacts women’s cognitions, emotions and behaviour. Whereas experiences with hostile sexism tend to arouse anger and arousal that fade away relatively quickly, interactions with benevolent sexism tend to arouse self-doubt, rumination and low-level arousal that simmers for a relatively long time. Hence, this has a different effect on both performance and overall satisfaction related to performance and the work environment (Pacilli et al. 2019; Barreto et al. 2010; Dardenne et al. 2007; Dumont et al. 2010).
Regarding the moderating effects of the relationship between the workplace predictors and the experience with benevolent sexism and hostile sexism, the results showed several significant and negative interaction effects on the perceived job outcomes. Indeed, experience with benevolent sexism significantly decreased the positive relationship between each of the predictors (WE, PsyCap and POS) and task performance and job satisfaction. This implies that direct experience with benevolent sexism has a harmful influence on both the individual contribution that each worker provides in his or her work and the perceived support and involvement from the company. Less significant results are found in the interaction of hostile sexism with workplace predictors. Actually, hostile sexism reduces job satisfaction in its interaction with work engagement and organisational support.

3.4. Multigroup Analysis: A Comparison of the Different Effects between Younger and Older Generation

In order to examine the possible existence of differences in the structural model for two different age groups (‘younger’ and ‘older’), we performed a multi-group analysis. To investigate these group differences, ascertainment is required that differences exist between these groups and that those differences derive from structural differences in the path coefficients between the groups. In particular, prior to assessing any evidence of structural pathway equality (structural invariance test), an important check needs to be carried out as to whether the measurement parameters operate in the same way for both groups (measurement invariance test) (Cheung and Rensvold 2002; De Pasquale et al. 2022; Platania et al. 2022a, 2022b; Morando and Platania 2022).
As the first step of this analysis, the invariance of the measurement model was assessed across the two groups, comparing the unconstrained model (i.e., with all parameters free) with the model with measurement weights constrained to be equal across the group (Tan and Pektas 2020).
The test reveals a good fit of the model for both observed groups (younger and older); however, a significantly worse adjustment is shown for the structural model with constrained coefficients compared to the model with all coefficients unconstrained [Δχ2 p < 0.001]. These findings, shown in Table 3, revealed that there is a moderating effect of the variable defining the groups.
Subsequently, as the results of the multi-group analysis provided evidence as a whole, in order to identify the substantial and significant differences in the structural relationships, we estimated the separate structural models for each group. The results, which showed the estimates for each group derived from the different structural models, are shown in Table 4. The results reported suggest that there are statistically significant differences between the two groups in the individual pathways. The employees of the younger generation seem to be affected and experience benevolent sexism more than the older generation, which is seen both in the individual moderators and in their interactions with the predictors. In contrast, in relation to hostile sexism, non-significant differences, or at least very similar effects, emerge between the groups. Furthermore, the older generation seems to report stronger effects in the paths concerning the direct relationship between workplace predictors (work engagement, PsyCap and organisational support) and task performance.

4. Discussion

The aim of the present research was to study the phenomenon of contemporary sexism and its impact on task performance and job satisfaction. In doing so, a sample of Italian female workers was enrolled, and the moderator role of the experience with benevolent and hostile sexism in modifying the predictive power of work engagement, psychological capital and organisational support on the outcomes was examined.
Expanding on the results obtained in the previous section, a finding certainly worth noting and discussing concerns the presence and perception of hostile sexism and benevolent sexism by the sample. The mean scores, related to the experience of the sample interviewed, reported a medium-low level and suggest a low presence or perception of sexism in the Italian workplace. This result may indeed be accurate, but consistently with the construct of social desirability, the limits inherent in the use of questionnaires and especially the not entirely implicit nature of the measurement scales used, it seems reasonable to doubt the limited presence of sexism in the workplace. Indeed, as pointed out in the introduction, Italy’s typical conservative and traditional approach, its cultural imprint and the cultural rigidity of the gender role suggest that the low scores on the perception of sexism are questionable. Moreover, current research seems to be progressively moving towards more implicit or, in any case, more subtle measurements of the phenomenon, which have revealed, even in Italy, the presence of sexism in the workplace, albeit in a ‘camouflaged’ manner (Zehnter et al. 2021; Morando et al. 2023). Thus, in line with the most recent studies, it can be argued that hostile and blatant sexism is still particularly common and present in the workplace, even for the sample of the present study, but in contemporary society, it is the benevolent and more subtle sexism that is more pervasive, which indeed has higher mean scores for the present sample. However, the results are equally interesting, as although the average scores are not very high, they reveal their influence and effect on the sample and the proposed outcomes.
The most interesting results, however, arise in the proposed moderation model. On the effects, completely consistent with the choice made and the literature on the topic, the predictors work engagement, psychological capital and organisational support turn out to be satisfactory and significant in predicting both outcomes, job satisfaction and task performance. In contrast, the two moderators, experiences with benevolent sexism and hostile sexism show a negative and therefore reducing effect on the same outcomes. Specifically, benevolent sexism seems to have a stronger impact than hostile sexism, confirming the change in contemporary sexism. This effect is more strongly supported by the interaction between each of the predictor variables and each of the moderators. Experiencing and perceiving benevolent sexism actually has negative and significant impacts, unlike hostile sexism. For this sample, experiencing and perceiving benevolent sexism means reducing the positive effects of commitment and involvement at work, the positive effect of the personal traits and transversal skills one holds, and the positive effect of the perceived support from the company towards both higher and better performance and higher job satisfaction (Platania et al. 2021; Caponnetto et al. 2022). Slightly different results were shown for the experience with hostile sexism. Participants who experience hostile sexism report a reduction in the positive effect only of the relationship between work engagement and job satisfaction and between organisational support and task performance and job satisfaction.
It is clear that hostile sexism is harmful to women through its direct discrimination. On the other hand, it is not so obvious how benevolent sexism is detrimental to women, since benevolent sexism is apparently positive. In line with the findings produced by this study, several studies argue that benevolent sexism is harmful to women’s personal well-being and job satisfaction (Dardenne et al. 2007; Moya et al. 2007) and that the experience of benevolent sexism often results in a lower level of career aspiration and generally lower self-esteem and self-efficacy (Moya et al. 2007; Yamamoto and Ohbuchi 2011; Jones et al. 2014). In an experimental study, Dardenne et al. (2007) proved how women’s exposure to benevolent sexism generates a devaluation of their capabilities, which disrupts their cognitive processes as intrusive thinking and compromises their performance. Hostile sexism also has discriminatory implications towards women’s capabilities, but its behavioural manifestations are usually accompanied by a clear hostility towards women. The women may therefore attribute these implications either to the personal factors of those who endorse sexism or to their own actual capabilities. In this case, they may not experience self-directed negative emotions but direct them towards those who use these sexist and discriminatory messages (Yamamoto and Ohbuchi 2011). Dumont et al. (2010) also examined the participants’ cognitive performance and autobiographical memory as a result of benevolent, hostile and neutral sexist attitudes. Their results revealed that the worst performance was recorded following benevolent sexist comments and attitudes compared to hostile or neutral sexist comments and attitudes. Recent neurological evidence further supports the idea that benevolent sexism hinders cognitive performance more, by examining changes in brain activity following benevolently sexist, hostilely sexist or neutral comments using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) techniques (Dardenne et al. 2013).
One of the aims of the present study was also to examine the possible generational differences that can be detected in the model previously discussed. The literature provides us with some indications on how sexist attitudes may differ over a lifespan, especially in relation to their endorsement. According to some studies, the incidence of sexism is generally stable, but significant differences may emerge over time and age. Women’s benevolent sexism and women’s hostile sexism are often relatively high in late adolescence, lower during middle adulthood and high again in older ages, following a kind of U-shaped trajectory (Hammond et al. 2018).
Conversely, few studies have been carried out on how benevolent and hostile sexist attitudes are perceived and experienced across age groups. For this reason, we divided our sample into two subgroups, younger and older, and investigated the differences in the previously identified moderation model. The results suggest that there are statistically significant differences between the two groups in the individual pathways. The younger generation seems to be affected and experience benevolent sexism more than those of the older generation, which is seen both in the individual moderators and in their interaction with the predictors. This would imply that the younger generation suffers more from the effects of benevolent sexism in reducing both task performance and job satisfaction. In contrast, in relation to hostile sexism, non-significant differences or at least very similar effects emerge between the groups. Furthermore, the older generation seems to report stronger effects in the pathways related to the direct relationship between workplace predictors and job performance. This evidence, also supported by indications that young women are more exposed and susceptible to sexism, suggests that younger generations are more capable of detecting and perceiving sexist attitudes and comments, but because of this, they are more likely to suffer their effects in terms of both personal and occupational well-being as well as work performance. The reasons why the younger generation has more experience of both forms of sexism may be various: many argue that younger women are often seen as an easier target and less likely to stand up for themselves. They are likely to be seen as patronisingly naïve and more easily the object of commentary due to the sexualisation by the mass media that has generated a distinctly distorted perception of women. Another motivation is also found in the greater capacity and actual awareness held by the younger generation compared to the older. It is possible, indeed, that older female workers are more influenced by the so-called construct of internalised misogyny. As Germer (2009) noted, “We’re like fish in the water of our culture, and when the water is polluted with racism, sexism, and ageism, we draw those prejudices inside” (pp. 203–204). Internalised misogyny exemplifies this point and demonstrates that sexism can assume the guise of a persistent internal criticism for women with the ability to change the way they view themselves and other women (Stevenson and Allen 2017). There is much scientific evidence that supports the relationship between exposure to sexism and internalised misogyny (Hammond et al. 2018; Szymanski et al. 2009). In the absence of proactive resistance, women may internalise benevolent sexist views (Hammond et al. 2018) and internalised misogyny, procuring depression, low levels of self-esteem (Piggot 2004), psychological distress and internalised objectification (Szymanski et al. 2009; Cherry and Wilcox 2021).

5. Limitations and Future Research

Despite the overall promising results, this research has some limitations. A first limitation is the use of self-report measures that could increase the probability of social desirability in the sample responses. In order to limit this, however, the common method bias was tested, and it was confirmed that it is not a problem for the data presented here (Podsakoff et al. 2003). A second limitation is the cross-sectional design, which clearly prevents us from clarifying whether the change in the perception of sexism really depends on the generations or on the individual maturation of the participant. Furthermore, as sexism is in general a rather sensitive topic, a further limitation is the possibility of reluctance among participants to admit to holding certain attitudes.
A further limitation concerns the sample. It is still too small and not entirely representative. The sample of the study is somewhat homogeneous and mainly composed of white, heterosexual, upper-middle class, educated and Christian-oriented women, which might have limited the variance of the scores. Furthermore, considering the Italian context specifically, the present study did not focus any attention on the possible effects and differences that other variables might have in the Italian work contexts (i.e., north/south of Italy as a geographical area; private/public; small and medium enterprises/large companies/public administration; academies/non-academies, etc.). Future research should recruit more heterogeneous samples to increase the study’s generalisability and test possible further interesting differences.
Finally, experiences with hostile and benevolent sexism are only one of many ways to conceptualise sexism as a whole. Therefore, the negative effects of sexism in general are not fully represented by the present study. Future studies could include new study variables and offer a more comprehensive overview.

6. Conclusions

In conclusion, this study aims at exploring sexism as a contemporary and ever-changing phenomenon, providing indications on how to design policies and practices and targets to be addressed. What emerges from the study is the prevalence of the more subtle and implicit aspects of sexism that affect women differently. This suggests that programmes need to be implemented to increase the awareness and knowledge of those who endorse sexist attitudes and comments, emphasising that even benevolent sexism represents a discriminatory and damaging form. Then, in relation to the differences that emerged by age group, the suggestion is to implement policies that directly involve women, providing them with the instruments, knowledge and skills to face sexism in full. This means that, depending on the age group, a different strategy needs to be thought out and implemented that can enhance different aspects to different degrees. Future research is, in any case, desirable, even introducing measurement instruments that are more implicit and less susceptible to social desirability.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol was authorised by the Internal Ethics Committee of the Department of Education Sciences (Psychology Section) of the University of Catania (Ierb-Edunict-2020/2); the relevant research procedures followed all the guidelines of the AIP (Italian Psychology Association) and its Ethics Council.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Acar, F. Pinar, and H. Canan Sümer. 2018. Another test of gender differences in assignments to precarious leadership positions: Examining the moderating role of ambivalent sexism. Applied Psychology 67: 498–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Aiken, Leona S., Stephen G. West, and Raymond R. Reno. 1991. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Newcastle upon Tyne: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  3. Avey, James B., Rebecca J. Reichard, Fred Luthans, and Ketan H. Mhatre. 2011. Meta-analysis of the impact of positive psychological capital on employee attitudes, behaviors, and performance. Human Resource Development Quarterly 22: 127–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Balducci, Cristian, Franco Fraccaroli, and Wilmar B. Schaufeli. 2010. Psychometric properties of the Italian version of the Utrecht Work Engagement Scale (UWES-9). European Journal of Psychological Assessment 26: 143–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Barreto, Manuela, and Naomi Ellemers. 2005. The burden of benevolent sexism: How it contributes to the maintenance of gender inequalities. European Journal of Social Psychology 35: 633–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Barreto, Manuela, Michelle K. Ryan, and Michael T. Schmitt. 2009. The Glass Ceiling in the 21st Century: Understanding Barriers to Gender Equality. Washington: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barreto, Manuela, Naomi Ellemers, Laura Piebinga, and Miguel Moya. 2010. How nice of us and how dumb of me: The effect of exposure to benevolent sexism on women’s task and relational self-descriptions. Sex Roles 62: 532–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bartol, Kathryn M. 1999. Gender influences on performance evaluations. In Handbook of Gender and Work. Edited by Gary. N. Powell. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc., pp. 165–78. [Google Scholar]
  9. Battistelli, Adalgisa, and Marco Giovanni Mariani. 2011. Supporto organizzativo: Validazione della versione italiana della survey of perceived organizational support (versione a 8 item). Giornale italiano di psicologia 38: 189–214. [Google Scholar]
  10. Benokraitis, Nijole Vaicaitis, and Joe R. Feagin. 1986. Modern Sexism: Blatant, Subtle, and Covert Discrimination. Hoboken: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
  11. Branscombe, Nyla R., Michael T. Schmitt, and Richard D. Harvey. 1999. Perceiving pervasive discrimination among African Americans: Implications for group identification and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77: 135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Buscemi, Andrea, Alessandro Rapisarda, Silvia Platania, Felice Maida, Demetrio Brancat1, Maria Cristina Petralia, Santo Di Nuovo, Giustiniani Salvatore, Lucia Caldarella, Maria Grazia Blandino, and et al. 2016. The woman in pregnancy: Body care by knowing of alternative medicine. Acta Medica Mediterranea 32: 953–58. [Google Scholar]
  13. Byrne, Barbara M. 2010. Structural Equation Modeling with AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. Multivariate Applications Series; New York: Taylor & Francis Group, vol. 396, p. 7384. [Google Scholar]
  14. Campbell, John P. 1990. Modeling the performance prediction problem in industrial and organizational psychology. In Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology. Edited by Marvine D. Dunnette and Leaettta M. Hough. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press, pp. 687–732. [Google Scholar]
  15. Caponnetto, Pasquale, Silvia Platania, Marilena Maglia, Martina Morando, Stefania Valeria Gruttadauria, Roberta Auditore, Caterina Ledda, Venerando Rapisarda, and Giuseppe Santisi. 2022. Health occupation and job satisfaction: The impact of psychological capital in the management of clinical psychological stressors of healthcare workers in the COVID-19 era. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19: 6134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Cherry, Marcus A., and Melanie M. Wilcox. 2021. Sexist microaggressions: Traumatic stressors mediated by self-compassion. The Counseling Psychologist 49: 106–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Cheung, Gordon W., and Roger B. Rensvold. 2002. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing meaurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling 9: 233–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Cohen, Jacob, Patricia Cohen, Stephen G. West, and Leona S. Aiken. 2013. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Abingdon: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  19. Corbeanu, Andreaa, and Dragos Iliescu. 2023. The link between work engagement and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Personnel Psychology 22: 111–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Dardenne, Benoit, Muriel Dumont, and Thierry Bollier. 2007. Insidious dangers of benevolent sexism: Consequences for women’s performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93: 764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Dardenne, Benoit, Muriel Dumont, Marie Sarlet, Christophe Phillips, Evelyne Balteau, Christian Degueldre, André Luxen, Eric Salmon, Pierre Maquet, and Fabienne Collette. 2013. Benevolent sexism alters executive brain responses. NeuroReport 24: 572–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. De Pasquale, Concetta, Martina Morando, Silvia Platania, Federica Sciacca, Zira Hichy, Santo Di Nuovo, and Maria Catena Quattropani. 2022. The Roles of Anxiety and Self-Esteem in the Risk of Eating Disorders and Compulsive Buying Behavior. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19: 16245. [Google Scholar]
  23. Dumont, Muriel, Marie Sarlet, and Benoit Dardenne. 2010. Be too kind to a woman, she’ll feel incompetent: Benevolent sexism shifts self-construal and autobiographical memories toward incompetence. Sex Roles 62: 545–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Eisenberger, Robert, Jim Cummings, Stephen Armeli, and Patrick Lynch. 1997. Perceived organizational support, discretionary treatment, and job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology 82: 812–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Eisenberger, Robert, Stephen Armeli, Barbara Rexwinkel, Patrick D. Lynch, and Linda Rhoades. 2001. Reciprocation of perceived organizational support. Journal of Applied Psychology 86: 42–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Ellemers, Naomi, and Manuela Barreto. 2008. Putting Your Own Down: How Members of Disadvantaged Groups Unwittingly Perpetuate or Exacerbate Their Disadvantage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  27. Ellemers, Naomi, Henriette Van den Heuvel, Dick De Gilder, Anne Maass, and Alessandra Bonvini. 2004. The underrepresentation of women in science: Differential commitment or the queen bee syndrome? British Journal of Social Psychology 43: 315–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Germer, Christopher. 2009. The Mindful Path to Self-Compassion: Freeing Yourself from Destructive Thoughts and Emotions. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  29. Glick, Peter, and Susan T. Fiske. 1996. The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70: 491–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Glick, Peter, and Susan T. Fiske. 1999. The ambivalence toward men inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent beliefs about men. Psychology of Women Quarterly 23: 519–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Görgens-Ekermans, Gina, and Ria Steyn. 2016. Optimism, self-efficacy and meaningfulness: A structural model of subjective well-being at work. Management Dynamics: Journal of the Southern African Institute for Management Scientists 25: 34–51. [Google Scholar]
  32. Grobelna, Aleksandra. 2019. Effects of individual and job characteristics on hotel contact employees’ work engagement and their performance outcomes: A case study from Poland. International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management 31: 349–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hakanen, Jari J., and Wilmar B. Schaufeli. 2012. Do burnout and work engagement predict depressive symptoms and life satisfaction? A three-wave seven-year prospective study. Journal of Affective Disorders 141: 415–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Hammond, Matthew D., Petar Milojev, Yanshu Huang, and Chris G. Sibley. 2018. Benevolent sexism and hostile sexism across the ages. Social Psychological and Personality Science 9: 863–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hayes, Andrew F., and Michael Scharkow. 2013. The relative trustworthiness of inferential tests of the indirect effect in statistical mediation analysis: Does method really matter? Psychological Science 24: 1918–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Heilman, Madeline E. 2001. Description and prescription: How gender stereotypes prevent women’s ascent up the organizational ladder. Journal of Social Issues 57: 657–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jones, Kristen, Kathy Stewart, Eden King, Whitney Botsford Morgan, Veronica Gilrane, and Kimberly Hylton. 2014. Negative consequence of benevolent sexism on efficacy and performance. Gender in Management: An International Journal 29: 171–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Koopmans, Linda, Claire M. Bernaards, Vincent H. Hildebrandt, Stef van Buuren, Allard J. van der Beek, and Henrica C. W. de Vet. 2015. Individual work performance questionnaire. Journal of Applied Measurement 14: 513. [Google Scholar]
  39. Lorenz, Timo, Clemens Beer, Jan Pütz, and Kathrin Heinitz. 2016. Measuring psychological capital: Construction and validation of the compound PsyCap scale (CPC-12). PLoS ONE 11: e0152892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Macdonald, Scott, and Peter MacIntyre. 1997. The generic job satisfaction scale: Scale development and its correlates. Employee Assistance Quarterly 13: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Maier, Mark. 1999. On the gendered substructure of organization: Dimensions and dilemmas of corporate masculinity. In Handbook of Gender and Work. Edited by Gary N. Powell. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, Inc., pp. 69–93. [Google Scholar]
  42. Meade, Adam W., Emily C. Johnson, and Phillip W. Braddy. 2008. Power and sensitivity of alternative fit indices in tests of measurement invariance. Journal of Applied Psychology 93: 568–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Morando, Martina, and Silvia Platania. 2022. Luxury Tourism Consumption in the Accommodation Sector: The Mediation Role of Destination Brand Love for Potential Tourists. Sustainability 14: 4007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Morando, Martina, Miriam Katharina Zehnter, and Silvia Platania. 2023. Reverse Sexism and Its Impact on Job Satisfaction and Career Planning: Italian Validation of the “Belief in Sexism Shift Scale”. Social Sciences 12: 357. [Google Scholar]
  45. Moya, Miguel, Peter Glick, Francisca Expósito, Soledad De Lemus, and Joshua Hart. 2007. It’s for your own good: Benevolent sexism and women’s reactions to protectively justified restrictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 33: 1421–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Newman, Alexander, Deniz Ucbasaran, Fei Zhu, and Giles Hirst. 2014. Psychological capital: A review and synthesis. Journal of Organizational Behavior 35: S120–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Pacilli, Maria Giuseppina, Federica Spaccatini, Ilaria Giovannelli, Delia Centrone, and Michele Roccato. 2019. System justification moderates the relation between hostile (but not benevolent) sexism in the workplace and state anxiety: An experimental study. The Journal of Social Psychology 159: 474–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Piggot, Marg. 2004. Double Jeopardy: Lesbians and the Legacy of Multiple Stigmatized Identities. Unpublished thesis, Psychology Strand at Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne, Australia. [Google Scholar]
  49. Platania, Silvia, and Anna Paolillo. 2022. Validation and measurement invariance of the Compound PsyCap Scale (CPC-12): A short universal measure of psychological capital. Anales de Psicología/Annals of Psychology 38: 63–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Platania, Silvia, Martina Morando, and Giuseppe Santisi. 2022a. Organisational Climate, Diversity Climate and Job Dissatisfaction: A Multi-Group Analysis of High and Low Cynicism. Sustainability 14: 4458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Platania, Silvia, Pasquale Caponnetto, Martina Morando, Marilena Maglia, Roberta Auditore, and Giuseppe Santisi. 2021. Cross-Cultural Adaptation, Psychometric Properties and Measurement Invariance of the Italian Version of the Job Satisfaction Scale. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 11: 1073–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Platania, Silvia, Stefania Valeria Gruttadauria, and Martina Morando. 2022b. Dispositional Resilience as Mediator in Psychological Stress on Healthcare Workers: A Multi-Group Analysis of Frontline and Non-Frontline Workers. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 12: 1285–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Podsakoff, Philip M., Scott B. MacKenzie, Jeong-Yeon Lee, and Nathan P. Podsakoff. 2003. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology 88: 879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Rhoades, Linda, and Robert Eisenberger. 2002. Perceived organizational support: A review of the literature. Journal of Applied Psychology 87: 698–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Rotundo, Maria, and Paul R. Sackett. 2002. The relative importance of task, citizenship, and counterproductive performance to global ratings of job performance: A policy-capturing approach. Journal of Applied Psychology 87: 66–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Rubin, Mark, Stefania Paolini, Emina Subašić, and Anna Giacomini. 2019. A confirmatory study of the relations between workplace sexism, sense of belonging, mental health, and job satisfaction among women in male-dominated industries. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 49: 267–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Salomon, Kristen, Jennifer K. Bosson, Mona El-Hout, Elizabeth Kiebel, Sophie L. Kuchynka, and Samantha L. Shepard. 2020. The experiences with ambivalent sexism inventory (EASI). Basic and Applied Social Psychology 42: 235–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Schaufeli, Wilmar B., Arnold B. Bakker, and Marisa Salanova. 2003. Utrecht work engagement scale-9. Educational and Psychological Measurement. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Schmitt, Michael T., Nyla R. Branscombe, Diane Kobrynowicz, and Susan Owen. 2002. Perceiving discrimination against one’s gender group has different implications for well-being in women and men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28: 197–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Steele, Claude M. 1997. A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. American Psychologist 52: 613–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Steele, Claude M., and Joshua Aronson. 1995. Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69: 797–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Stevenson, Olivia, and Ashley Batts Allen. 2017. Women’s empowerment: Finding strength in self-compassion. Women & Health 57: 295–310. [Google Scholar]
  63. Swim, Janet K., Kathryn J. Aikin, Wayne S. Hall, and Barbara A. Hunter. 1995. Sexism and racism: Old-fashioned and modern prejudices. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68: 199–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Szymanski, Dawn M., Arpana Gupta, Erika R. Carr, and Destin Stewart. 2009. Internalized misogyny as a moderator of the link between sexist events and women’s psychological distress. Sex Roles 61: 101–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Tan, Seref, and Sami Pektas. 2020. Examining the Invariance of a Measurement Model by Using the Covariance Structure Approach. International Journal of Contemporary Educational Research 7: 27–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Tougas, Francine, Rupert Brown, Ann M. Beaton, and Stéphane Joly. 1995. Neosexism: Plus ça change, plus c’est pareil. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21: 842–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Vescio, Theresa K., Sarah J. Gervais, Mark Snyder, and Ann Hoover. 2005. Power and the creation of patronizing environments: The stereotype-based behaviors of the powerful and their effects on female performance in masculine domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88: 658–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Yamamoto, Takehiro, and Ken-ichi Ohbuchi. 2011. Negative effects of the exposure to benevolent sexism on women’s task performance. Tohoku Psychologica Folia 70: 36–43. [Google Scholar]
  69. Yu, Chongxin, and Stephen J. Frenkel. 2013. Explaining task performance and creativity from perceived organizational support theory: Which mechanisms are more important? Journal of Organizational Behavior 34: 1165–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Zalewska, Anna M. 2020. Work-Family and Family-Work Conflicts and Health: The Protective Role of Work Engagement and Job-Related Subjective Well-Being. Medycyna Pracy, 33–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Zehnter, Miriam K., Francesca Manzi, Patrick E. Shrout, and Madeline E. Heilman. 2021. Belief in sexism shift: Defining a new form of contemporary sexism and introducing the belief in sexism shift scale (BSS scale). PLoS ONE 16: e0248374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Theoretical model.
Figure 1. Theoretical model.
Admsci 13 00210 g001
Table 1. Mean scores, reliability, composite reliability, average variance extracted and intercorrelations (N = 402).
Table 1. Mean scores, reliability, composite reliability, average variance extracted and intercorrelations (N = 402).
αCRAVE Mean (SD)123456
1Work Engagement (WE)0.910.890.754.5 (1.1)1
2Psychological Capital (PsyCap)0.900.840.814.5 (0.9)0.35 **1
3Organisational Support (POS)0.920.850.813.4 (0.6)0.44 **0.44 *1
4Experience with Benevolent Sexism0.890.870.913.1 (1.42)−0.19 **−0.15 **−0.14 **1
5Hostile Sexism (ASI)0.920.690.792.7 (1.10)−0.14 **−0.09 **−0.040.66 **1
6Task Performance0.870.760.814.3 (1.3)0.56 **0.39 **0.26 **−0.13 **−0.14 **1
7Job Satisfaction0.890.790.844.34 (1.7)0.38 **0.23 **0.19 **−0.12 **−0.11 **0.35 **
Note: ** correlations are significant at the p < 0.001 level; * correlations are significant at the p < 0.05 level.
Table 2. Results of moderation analysis.
Table 2. Results of moderation analysis.
Task PerformanceJob Satisfaction
Step 1ββ
Work Engagement0.31 *0.22 *
Psychological Capital0.27 *0.33 *
Organisational Support0.40 *0.34 *
Step 2
Experience with Benevolent Sexism−0.18 *−0.13 *
Hostile Sexism −0.10 *−0.09 *
Step 3
Work Engagement × Experience with Benevolent Sexism−0.11 *−0.08 *
Psychological Capital × Experience with Benevolent Sexism−0.11 *−0.12 *
Organisational Support × Experience with Benevolent Sexism−0.13 *−0.09 *
Work Engagement × Hostile Sexism n.s.−0.07 *
Psychological Capital × Hostile Sexism n.s.n.s.
Organisational Support × Hostile Sexism−0.08 *−0.11 *
Note * p < 0.01, n.s. = not significant.
Table 3. Multigroup analysis: test for measurement invariance across younger generation (N = 177) and older generation (N = 225).
Table 3. Multigroup analysis: test for measurement invariance across younger generation (N = 177) and older generation (N = 225).
Measurement ModelΧ2df∆χ²∆dfNFICFIRMSEA
Multigroup model for the total sample415.37192- 0.900.910.05
Unconstrained model593.84203178.47110.910.920.05
Measurement model868.12263274.28600.900.910.05
Structural model 1350.32285 *482.20 ***820.890.900.06
Note: *** p < 0.001, * p < 0.01, NFI = normed fit index; CFI = comparative fit index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation.
Table 4. Summary of path analysis between variables of our model among younger generation (N = 177) and older generation (N = 225).
Table 4. Summary of path analysis between variables of our model among younger generation (N = 177) and older generation (N = 225).
Paths of Interest Younger Generation (N = 177)Older Generation (N = 225)
B (SE)pB (SE)p
WETask Performance0.28<0.010.33<0.01
WEJob Satisfaction0.24<0.010.17<0.01
WE × EASTask Performance−0.17<0.05−0.09<0.05
WE × EASJob Satisfaction−0.12<0.01−0.04<0.01
WE × HSTask Performance−0.03n.s.−0.31n.s.
WE × HSJob Satisfaction−0.12n.s.−0.08<0.05
PsyCapTask Performance0.27<0.050.30<0.05
PsyCapJob Satisfaction0.23<0.010.26<0.01
PsyCap × EASTask Performance−0.12<0.05−0.11<0.05
PsyCap × EASJob Satisfaction−0.19<0.01−0.13<0.05
PsyCap × HSTask Performance−0.28n.s0.02n.s.
PsyCap × HSJob Satisfaction−0.03ns0.31n.s.
POSTask Performance0.38<0.010.40<0.01
POSJob Satisfaction0.36<0.010.32<0.01
POS × EASTask Performance−0.19<0.01−0.14<0.01
POS × EASJob Satisfaction−0.16<0.01−0.08<0.01
POS × HSTask Performance−0.09<0.01−0.08<0.01
POS × HSJob Satisfaction−0.11<0.01−0.10<0.01
EASTask Performance−0.19<0.01−0.14<0.01
EASJob Satisfaction−0.13<0.05−0.12<0.05
HSTask Performance−0.10<0.01−0.10<0.01
HSJob Satisfaction−0.09<0.01−0.09<0.01
Note: WE = work engagement; EAS = experience with ambivalent Sexism; HS = hostile sexism; PsyCap = psychological capital; POS = perceived organisational support; n.s. = not significant.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Morando, M. Disentangling Workplace Sexism in Age Generations: A Multi-Group Analysis on the Effects on Job Satisfaction and Task Performance. Adm. Sci. 2023, 13, 210. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci13090210

AMA Style

Morando M. Disentangling Workplace Sexism in Age Generations: A Multi-Group Analysis on the Effects on Job Satisfaction and Task Performance. Administrative Sciences. 2023; 13(9):210. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci13090210

Chicago/Turabian Style

Morando, Martina. 2023. "Disentangling Workplace Sexism in Age Generations: A Multi-Group Analysis on the Effects on Job Satisfaction and Task Performance" Administrative Sciences 13, no. 9: 210. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci13090210

APA Style

Morando, M. (2023). Disentangling Workplace Sexism in Age Generations: A Multi-Group Analysis on the Effects on Job Satisfaction and Task Performance. Administrative Sciences, 13(9), 210. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci13090210

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop