Next Article in Journal
Erratum: Pagonabarraga, J.; et al. A Spanish Consensus on the Use of Safinamide for Parkinson’s Disease in Clinical Practice. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 176
Next Article in Special Issue
Promoting the Social Inclusion of Children with ASD: A Family-Centred Intervention
Previous Article in Journal
Glutamatergic Fate of Neural Progenitor Cells of Rats with Inherited Audiogenic Epilepsy
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Parental Education and Youth Inhibitory Control in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study: Blacks’ Diminished Returns

Department of Family Medicine, Charles R Drew University of Medicine and Science, Los Angeles, CA 90059, USA
Brain Sci. 2020, 10(5), 312; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050312
Submission received: 18 April 2020 / Revised: 12 May 2020 / Accepted: 19 May 2020 / Published: 21 May 2020

Abstract

:
Background: Non-Hispanic Black (NHB) youth are at a higher risk of high-risk behaviors compared to non-Hispanic White (NHW) youth. Some of this racial gap is shown to be due to weaker effects of parental educational attainment on reducing the prevalence of behavioral risk factors such as impulsivity, substance use, aggression, obesity, and poor school performance for NHBs, a pattern called Minorities’ Diminished Returns. These diminishing returns may be due to lower than expected effects of parental education on inhibitory control. Aim: We compared NHW and NHB youth for the effect of parental educational attainment on youth inhibitory control, a psychological and cognitive construct that closely predicts high-risk behaviors such as the use of drugs, alcohol, and tobacco. Methods: This was a cross-sectional analysis that included 4188 youth from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study. The independent variable was parental educational attainment. The main outcome was youth inhibitory control measured by the stop-signal task (SST), which was validated by parent reports on the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL). Results: In race/ethnicity-stratified models, high parental educational attainment was associated with a higher level of inhibitory control for NHB than NHW youth. In the pooled sample, race/ethnicity showed a statistically significant interaction with parental educational attainment on youth inhibitory control suggesting that high parental educational attainment has a smaller boosting effect on inhibitory control for NHB than NHW youth. Conclusion: Parental educational attainment boosts inhibitory control for NHW but not NHB youth. To minimize the racial gap in youth brain development, we need to address societal barriers that diminish the returns of family economic and human resources, particularly parental educational attainment, for racial and ethnic minority youth. Social and public policies should address structural and societal barriers such as social stratification, segregation, racism, and discrimination that hinder NHB parents’ abilities to effectively mobilize their human resources and secure tangible outcomes for their developing youth.

1. Introduction

Inhibitory control (IC), also known as response inhibition, is a cognitive process/executive function—that can be defined as an ability to inhibit impulses in order to increase the chance of appropriate behaviors that are consistent with completing long-term goals [1,2]. An extreme example of a disorder with impaired IC is attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [3]. Low IC is associated with a wide range of outcomes such as obesity, food choices, eating disorders, school performance, peer preferences, externalizing behaviors, aggression, prosocial behaviors, sexual debut, and use of drugs, alcohol, and tobacco [4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12]. Low IC may be one of the reasons why youth from low socioeconomic status (SES) and racial and ethnic minority groups engage in more risky behaviors, compared to high SES and majority youth [1,13,14,15,16]. Thus, studies are needed that investigate the additive effects of race/ethnicity and SES on youth IC as a mechanism of disparities and inequalities in high-risk behaviors [1,10,17,18,19].
Family SES and parental behaviors are shown to be predictors of IC in youth [20,21,22,23,24]. In a study on 147 7–10-year-old children, Cabello et al. investigated the relationship between parental educational attainment and youth IC as well as aggressive behavior. Teachers rated the aggressive behavior of the children using the Teacher Rating Scale (TRS) of the Behavior Assessment System for Children 2 (BASC-2). The youth themselves completed a go/no-go task that assessed their IC. Parents reported their educational attainment. Analysis of the data showed that both lower parental educational attainment and lower IC were predictive of higher aggressive behavior in the youth. Interestingly, authors found evidence suggesting that IC partially mediates the effects of parental educational attainment on aggressive behavior. However, sex moderated this mediation, as IC explained this effect for boys, not girls. Their study suggests the parental educational attainment impacts IC and IC may be why parental educational attainment is associated with youth aggressive behavior, particularly in boys [6].
Compared to non-Hispanic White (NHW) youth, non-Hispanic Black (NHB) youth are at an increased risk of high-risk behaviors that are largely associated with poor IC. For example, NHB youth are more likely than NHW youth to be at risk of engaging in aggressive behavior [25], early sexual debut [26], and becoming school dropouts [27]. As many of these early undesired outcomes are gateways to future economic and health problems later in life [28,29,30,31], it is important to study why and how IC is different in NHB and NHW youth. Such knowledge may be helpful in closing, or at least reducing, the existing racial and ethnic economic and health inequalities experienced later in life [28,29,30,31].
Given the close overlap between race/ethnicity and SES in the US, there has been an interest in trying to decompose the effects of race/ethnicity and SES on health and behavioral inequalities [32,33,34]. In theory, both low SES and racial/ethnic minority status generate marginalization and increase exposure to food and housing insecurity, economic adversities, stress, and financial difficulties [35,36,37,38]. Thus, one remaining question is whether race/ethnicity and SES have separate, additive, or multiplicative effects on health inequalities [39,40,41,42]. While SES provides access to buffers and protections, the SES effects on health and behaviors and their precursors [39,40,41,42] may depend on race/ethnicity. In addition, SES may carry the effect of race/ethnicity on health and behavioral outcomes [43].
There are, however, two complementary hypotheses that explain how race/ethnicity and family SES may jointly impact youth health and behavioral outcomes. The first hypothesis, a more traditional one, attributes the racial and ethnic gap in youth outcomes to racial and ethnic differences in family SES (e.g., lower SES in NHBs than NHWs) [43,44,45,46]. In this view, family SES is believed to mediate the effects of racial and ethnic minority status on youth outcomes [47,48,49]. As such, the belief is that enhancing racial and ethnic minorities’ family SES and closing the racial and ethnic differences in SES would be the primary strategy for closing racial and ethnic youth inequalities [50,51]. Some policy solutions based on this view is income redistribution, income tax credit, and empowering racial and ethnic minorities to attain and accumulate education, income, and wealth.
The alternative explanation is Minorities’ Diminished Returns (MDRs) [52,53], which explains that some of the racial and ethnic inequalities are due to the weaker effects of family SES indicators for NHBs than NHWs. This view is supported by extensive empirical evidence showing that parental education [54], family income [55,56], and marital status [57] generate more outcomes for NHW than NHB youth. However, this literature is mainly focused on self-reported outcomes [54,55,56,58,59]. For example, high family SES showed smaller preventive effects on impulsivity [55], ADHD [60], depression [58], anxiety [61], aggression [54], poor GPA [54,62,63], and substance use [54] for NHB than NHW youth. That means, while high SES NHW youth show the lowest levels of risk across domains, high SES NHB youth remain at high levels of risk across multiple domains and outcomes.
As shown by the MDRs literature, parental educational attainment [64,65,66] generates unequal outcomes for diverse racial and ethnic groups. This might be because society may differently allow NHB and NHW parents to mobilize their education and secure high paying jobs, particularly at high levels of education [53,55,61,65,67,68]. As a result of diminishing returns of parental education, compared to their non-HW counterparts, NHB youth with highly educated parents may show worse than expected outcomes that are disproportionate to their family SES [52,53,55,56,59].

2. Aims

To extend the existing knowledge on the role of IC as a mechanism for explaining MDRs for other behavioral outcomes, we studied the interactive and combined effects of race/ethnicity and parental educational attainment on youth outcomes. This study compared NHB and NHW youth for the effects of parental educational attainment, as one of the strongest family SES indicators, on youth IC. We expected weaker effects of parental educational attainment on youth IC for NHB than NHW youth.

3. Methods

3.1. Design and Settings

We performed a secondary analysis of data from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study [69,70,71,72,73], a landmark youth brain development study in the United States. Detailed information on the details of the ABCD study is available elsewhere [69,74].

3.2. Participants and Sampling

Participants of the ABCD study were youth age 9–10 years. Youth in the ABCD study were recruited from multiple cities across states. Overall, there were 21 sites that recruited youth to the ABCD study. The recruitment of the ABCD sample was mainly done through school systems. A detailed description of the ABCD sampling is available here [75]. Four thousand one hundred eighty-eight participants entered our analysis. Eligibility for our analysis had valid data on race/ethnicity, CBCL, task-based IC, parental education, marital status, and being NHB or NHW.

3.3. Study Variables

The study variables included race/ethnicity, demographic factors, parental educational attainment, parental marital status, and task-based IC (validated by the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL).

3.3.1. Outcome

The study also used the stop-signal task (SST) to measure IC. The SST applied two runs of 180 trials showing images of a black arrow pointing either right or left are displayed on the screen participants’ view while in the scanner. They were instructed to click the appropriate button corresponding to the arrow direction as quickly as they can after seeing the image using their dominant hand. Thirty of the 180 trials display neither option, signaling the participant to inhibit answering with either option being randomly dispersed throughout the trials. Successful inhibition of motor response represents a successful trial, while impulsively answering with either wrong answer is considered unsuccessful inhibition. IC was measured as the rate of correct "Stop" trials in the run. This variable was treated as a continuous measure with a higher score indicating a higher level of IC [76,77,78,79].

3.3.2. Validation of the IC Using CBCL Domains

We used the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) to validate lab(task) based measures of IC. The CBCL, also known as the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment, the study had the following eight outcomes: (1) Anxious and depressed mood, (2) withdrawn and depressed affect, (3) social and interpersonal problems, (4) somatic complaints, (5) thought problems, (6) attention problems, (7) violent and aggressive behaviors, and (8) rule-breaking behaviors [80]. These CBCL sub-scores closely correlate with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) based diagnoses [81]. The CBCL instrument uses parental reports to screen for social, behavioral, and emotional problems. The CBCL is commonly used across settings including but not limited to schools, medical settings, mental health facilities, child and family services, Health Management Organizations, and public health agencies [82]. CBCL has been used by thousands of published scholarly articles [82]. As expected, in our study, IC was correlated with behavioral rather than emotional domains of the CBCL, which was an indicator of its validity.

3.3.3. Moderator

Race/ethnicity. Race/ethnicity was self-identified. Race/ethnicity was a categorical variable and coded 1 for NHBs and 0 for NHWs (reference category).

3.3.4. Independent Variable

Parental Educational Attainment. Participants were asked, “What is the highest grade or level of school you have completed or the highest degree you have received?” Responses were 0 = Never attended/Kindergarten only; 1 = 1st grade; 2 = 2nd grade; 3 = 3rd grade; 4 = 4th grade 4; 5 = 5th grade; 6 = 6th grade 6; 7 = 7th grade 7; 8 = 8th grade; 9 = 9th grade; 10 = 10th grade 10; 11 = 11th grade; 12 = 12th grade; 13 = High school graduate; 14 = GED or equivalent Diploma; 15 = Some college; 16 = Associate degree: Occupational; 17 = Associate degree: Academic Program; 18 = Bachelor’s degree (ex. BA; 19 = Master’s degree (ex. MA; 20 = Professional School degree (ex. MD; 21 = Doctoral degree. This variable was an interval measure with a range between 1 and 21, with a higher score indicating higher educational attainment.

3.3.5. Confounders

Age, sex, and parental marital status were the confounders. Parents reported the age of their youth. Age (years) was calculated as the difference between the date of birth to the date of enrollment to the study. Sex was a dichotomous variable: males = 1, females = 0. Parental marital status was a dichotomous variable. This variable was self-reported by the parent who was interviewed. This variable was coded as married = 1 vs. other = 0.

3.4. Data Analysis

We used the statistical package SPSS to perform our data analysis. Mean (standard deviation (SD)) and frequency (%) were described depending on the variable type. We also performed a Pearson bivariate test to test bivariate associations between study variables. For our multivariable modeling, we performed four linear regression models. Our first two models were performed specifically to race/ethnicity (race-stratified models). Model 1 was tested in NHW, and Model 2 was tested in NHB youth. The next two models were performed in the overall sample. Model 3 was performed without the interaction terms. Model 4 added an interaction term between race/ethnicity and parental education attainment. Our models used age, sex, and marital status were the covariates. Unstandardized regression coefficient (b), standardized regression coefficient, SE, 95% CI, and p-value were reported for each model. p values equal to or less than 0.05 were statistically significant.

3.5. Ethical Aspect

The ABCD study received an Institutional Review Board (IRB) approval from the University of California, San Diego (UCSD). Each youth participant provided an assent. Each parent signed an informed consent [74]. As this analysis was performed on fully de-identified data, the study was found to be non-human subject research. Thus, our analysis was exempt from a full IRB review.

4. Results

4.1. Descriptives

As shown in Table 1, 4188, 8–11 years old youth entered to this analysis. From this number, most were NHWs (n = 2985; 71.3%) and the rest were NHBs (n = 1203; 28.7%). Table 1 presents a summary of the descriptive statistics for the pooled sample.
Table 2 shows a correlation matrix of all study variables, including the CBCL reports in the pooled sample and by race/ethnicity. In the pooled sample, NHB status was closely associated with lower parental education and marital status but was not associated with IC. IC was associated with behavioral rather than emotional domains of CBCL measure, which indicated the validity of our IC measure. In the pooled sample, parental educational attainment was not correlated with IC. In NHWs (r = 0.06, p < 0.05), but not in NHBs (r = −0.04, p > 0.05), parental educational attainment was correlated with IC.

4.2. Multivariate Analysis (Race-Stratified models)

Table 3 shows the results of two linear regression models in racial/ethnic groups. Model 3 (NHW) showed protective effects of high parental educational attainment on IC of NHW youth. Model 4 (NHB) did not have a protective effect of high parental educational attainment on IC for NHB youth.

4.3. Multivariate Analysis (Pooled Sample)

Table 4 shows the results of two linear regression models in the overall (pooled) sample. Model 1 (Main Effect Model) showed an only marginally significant effect of high parental educational attainment on IC. Model 2 (Interaction Model) showed a statistically significant interaction term between race/ethnicity and parental educational attainment on IC, suggesting that the boosting effect of high parental educational attainment on IC is significantly weaker for NHB youth relative to NHW youth.

5. Discussion

In our race-specific models, high parental educational attainment was associated with higher IC for NHW but not NHB youth. This finding was also supported by a statistical interaction between race/ethnicity and parental educational attainment on youth IC in the pooled sample. Thus, high parental educational attainment has smaller boosting effects on IC for NHB than NHW youth.
Here first, we discuss the observed MDRs. That is why NHB and NHW youth show a different gain in terms of increased IC due to an increase in parental educational attainment. In this section, we report similar literature and also some potential societal mechanisms that may help us understand these MDRs. Then we discuss the results specific to the IC. That is, SES (parental education) boosts IC and, at the same time, race (NHB) may reduce IC. However, the association between race and IC could only be observed for parental reports rather than the lab-based measure of IC. In the end, we list the limitations and strengths of our study. Among the strengths, we discuss why there were different methods of measuring IC (based on the IC neurocognitive task and parent’s behavioral ratings).
The observed diminished returns of parental education on IC is in line with the MDRs reported on impulsivity [55], ADHD [60], depression [58], anxiety [61], aggression [54], GPA [54,62,63], and substance use [54]. A large body empirical evidence supports diminishing returns of parental and own educational attainment for NHBs than NHWs [67,83,84,85]. MDRs are already documented both within individuals and families, and hold across SES resources, age groups, outcomes, and marginalizing identities [52,53]. MDRs are shown for income [55], education [84], employment [86], and marital status [61]. Parental educational attainment results in more gain for NHW than NHB youth [55,56,59], adults [67], and older adults [87]. Also, MDRs not only apply to NHBs [56], or HWs [84,88,89,90] but also for Asian Americans [91], Native Americans [92], and LGBTQ persons [83].
Various mechanisms may be involved in explaining the MDRs of parental educational attainment or NHB families. NHB families face disproportionately higher levels of financial, environmental, and race-related stress in their daily lives, at all SES levels. According to the social reproduction theory, intergenerational educational outcomes may vary across groups [93]. Chetty showed that the intersection of race/ethnicity and sex alter the likelihood of upward social mobility in the US [94]. Increased exposure to stress is believed to reduce youths’ ability to gain from their available SES resources such as education. It is shown that for NHB families, an increase in SES means an increase in experience [95,96,97,98,99] and vulnerability [100] to discrimination. This might be because high SES NHB families are more likely to be surrounded by NHW families, which means a higher level of exposure to discrimination [95,96]. Needless to say, a high level of discrimination means undesired outcomes and reduced gains of SES [98,100,101].
Residential segregation results in differences in NHB and NHW environmental and contextual exposures. Due to segregation, school options are different for high SES NHB and HW families. As a result, children of high SES NHB families attend highly segregated schools with low resources [62,63,102]. That means there are differential effects of SES on education and schooling of NHW and NHB. While high SES NHW youth attend schools in suburban areas with more funding and higher-quality teachers, NHBs should go to schools that are of lower quality [103].
While lower SES of NHBs is one type of disadvantage, MDRs reflect another class of disadvantage [52,53]. While the first one is about a lack of access to SES resources, MDRs are reflective of unequal outcomes despite similar access to SES. Thus, researchers and policymakers should not only address inequality in SES, but they should also address inequality in the returns of SES. Unfortunately, NHB families are at a double disadvantage because they are affected by low SES and low return of the existing SES resources [52,104].
Multilevel economic, psychological, and societal mechanisms may be involved in explaining racial and ethnic gaps in the returns of parental education [52,104]. MDRs may be due to racism across multiple societal institutions and social structures [52,104]. Racial/ethnic prejudice interferes with the processes that are needed to gain benefits from available SES resources [105,106,107]. MDRs of educational attainment may be in part due to a history of childhood poverty [108]. The current study, however, did not explore societal and contextual processes that could explain such MDRs.
NHB families are more likely to stay in poor neighborhoods despite high SES. Highly educated NHBs are more likely to stay poor than NHWs [66,109]. Similarly, NHB families from high SES backgrounds may remain at risk of environmental exposures than NHWs with similar SES [95,96,98,110,111,112,113]. Similarly, high SES NHB youth spend time with peers with higher risk and behavioral problems than NHWs with the same SES [54,91].
The implications of these MDRs are that it is not merely the access to SES, but also the degree to which SES can result in outcomes that should be addressed. Interventions should target the societal, social, environmental, and structure processes that generate MDRs. We argue that the solutions to MDRs-related disparities are different from those due to low SES. While the solution to disparities due to the gap in SES is to increase NHBs’ access to SES resources, the remedy to MDRs-related inequalities to empower NHBs so they can more efficiently translate their SES to outcomes. The latter solution requires policies and programs that go beyond access and address structural and environmental factors. For the latter, there is a need to equalize the life conditions of NHBs and NHWs.

6. Implications

The results may still be preliminary and need further evaluation. If these results are replicated by other datasets and samples, then there is a need for an intervention. It can be argued that NHB parents may require some additional help to mobilize their educational resources. NHB parents may benefit from programs that help with the employability of highly educated NHB parents. Schools in urban areas may benefit from additional resources and an enhanced quality of teachers. After school programs are some example solutions that may compensate reduced IC of NHB youth across SES levels. Interventions may also specifically target the developmental needs of NHB youth across all SES levels. Clinicians should be aware that some of the needs of NHB youth remain high across all SES levels. So, clinicians should be informed that some of the benefits and advantages that NHW youth receive from high SES may not be relevant in NHB families. Low attention may also be diagnosed and treated in NHB families across SES levels. Policymakers may also go beyond family SES and address structural factors that hinder NHB families from mobilizing their SES resources, particularly educational attainment, for securing outcomes.

7. Limitations and Strengths

Each study comes with specific types of methodological limitations. As our data were cross-sectional, we cannot draw any causal inferences between race, parental educational attainment, and youth IC. Similarly, this study only tested the MDRs of parental educational attainment. Other research may test if MDRs also hold for other SES indicators such as parental occupational prestige, income, wealth, and neighborhood SES. These investigations are important because associations between various SES indicators depend on race/ethnicity. Finally, this study only described the MDRs and did not explore contextual factors that could potentially explain the observed MDRs.
To list the study strengths, we can refer to the national scope, multi-center nature, large sample size, large sample of NHBs, and focusing on multiplicative rather than additive effects of SES and race. We can also refer to the application of multiple IC measures (the stop-signal task and CBCL) as another strength of the study. It is important to validate the youth IC task performance with the CBCL parent ratings for several reasons. First, in a clinical setting, most clinicians can easily utilize data of CBCL and measure IC using parents’ ratings. However, in many clinical and school settings, collecting data on IC using time-consuming tasks may be difficult. This is also applicable to scaling up the study results. While public health programs and interventions may apply self-reported measures of attention as a screening tool, it is unlikely that large programs can access laboratory measures of attention. So, our multiple measures of IC make our results applicable to a large-scale intervention. Finally, any study of racial differences should always cross-validate their measure. We found the IC similarly correlate with clinical presentation (parental report) of IC, so our IC construct was reflecting the same concept and is similarly applicable to NHW and NHB youth. In other terms, our lab-based measure of IC refers to the very same understanding of parents about their youth behavioral and attention problems. So, the results are similarly relevant to NHB and NHW families. This approach helps us rule out that the observed racial differences are simply an artifact and due to racial variation in measurement properties.

8. Conclusions

When compared to NHWs, NHB youth show lower parental educational attainment and parent reported IC. This adversity is compound by another more profound and systemic adversity, which reflects a weaker effect of parental educational attainment and youth IC. As a result of the latter disadvantage, NHB youth show low IC even when they have highly educated parents. That means some of the inequalities in IC remains across all SES levels. In other terms, racial and ethnic inequalities in IC show a spill-over effect in middle-class people. As a result of high IC, NHB youth with highly educated parents may remain at risk of social, emotional, and behavioral problems.

Funding

Shervin Assari is supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) grants D084526-03, CA201415 02, DA035811-05, U54MD008149, U54MD007598, and U54CA229974.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Deater-Deckard, K.; Li, M.; Lee, J.; King-Casas, B.; Kim-Spoon, J. Poverty and Puberty: A Neurocognitive Study of Inhibitory Control in the Transition to Adolescence. Psychol. Sci. 2019, 30, 1573–1583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Chikara, R.K.; Lo, W.C.; Ko, L.W. Exploration of Brain Connectivity during Human Inhibitory Control Using Inter-Trial Coherence. Sensors 2020, 20, 1722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Neely, K.A.; Wang, P.; Chennavasin, A.P.; Samimy, S.; Tucker, J.; Merida, A.; Perez-Edgar, K.; Huang-Pollock, C. Deficits in inhibitory force control in young adults with ADHD. Neuropsychologia 2017, 99, 172–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Bartholdy, S.; O’Daly, O.G.; Campbell, I.C.; Banaschewski, T.; Barker, G.; Bokde, A.L.W.; Bromberg, U.; Buchel, C.; Quinlan, E.B.; Desrivieres, S.; et al. Neural Correlates of Failed Inhibitory Control as an Early Marker of Disordered Eating in Adolescents. Biol. Psychiatry 2019, 85, 956–965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Bessette, K.L.; Karstens, A.J.; Crane, N.A.; Peters, A.T.; Stange, J.P.; Elverman, K.H.; Morimoto, S.S.; Weisenbach, S.L.; Langenecker, S.A. A Lifespan Model of Interference Resolution and Inhibitory Control: Risk for Depression and Changes with Illness Progression. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Cabello, R.; Gutierrez-Cobo, M.J.; Fernandez-Berrocal, P. Parental Education and Aggressive Behavior in Children: A Moderated-Mediation Model for Inhibitory Control and Gender. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Dieter, J.; Hoffmann, S.; Mier, D.; Reinhard, I.; Beutel, M.; Vollstadt-Klein, S.; Kiefer, F.; Mann, K.; Lemenager, T. The role of emotional inhibitory control in specific internet addiction—An fMRI study. Behav. Brain Res. 2017, 324, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Ely, A.V.; Jagannathan, K.; Hager, N.; Ketcherside, A.; Franklin, T.R.; Wetherill, R.R. Double jeopardy: Comorbid obesity and cigarette smoking are linked to neurobiological alterations in inhibitory control during smoking cue exposure. Addict. Biol. 2020, 25, e12750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Humphrey, G.; Dumontheil, I. Development of Risk-Taking, Perspective-Taking, and Inhibitory Control During Adolescence. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2016, 41, 59–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Porter, L.; Bailey-Jones, C.; Priudokaite, G.; Allen, S.; Wood, K.; Stiles, K.; Parvin, O.; Javaid, M.; Verbruggen, F.; Lawrence, N.S. From cookies to carrots; the effect of inhibitory control training on children’s snack selections. Appetite 2018, 124, 111–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Troller-Renfree, S.V.; Buzzell, G.A.; Bowers, M.E.; Salo, V.C.; Forman-Alberti, A.; Smith, E.; Papp, L.J.; McDermott, J.M.; Pine, D.S.; Henderson, H.A.; et al. Development of inhibitory control during childhood and its relations to early temperament and later social anxiety: Unique insights provided by latent growth modeling and signal detection theory. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2019, 60, 622–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Huijbregts, S.C.; Warren, A.J.; de Sonneville, L.M.; Swaab-Barneveld, H. Hot and cool forms of inhibitory control and externalizing behavior in children of mothers who smoked during pregnancy: An exploratory study. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2008, 36, 323–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Hao, J. Do Children with Better Inhibitory Control Donate More? Differentiating between Early and Middle Childhood and Cool and Hot Inhibitory Control. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 2182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Hsieh, I.J.; Chen, Y.Y. Determinants of aggressive behavior: Interactive effects of emotional regulation and inhibitory control. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0175651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Nakamichi, K. Differences in Young Children’s Peer Preference by Inhibitory Control and Emotion Regulation. Psychol. Rep. 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Froeliger, B.; McConnell, P.A.; Bell, S.; Sweitzer, M.; Kozink, R.V.; Eichberg, C.; Hallyburton, M.; Kaiser, N.; Gray, K.M.; McClernon, F.J. Association Between Baseline Corticothalamic-Mediated Inhibitory Control and Smoking Relapse Vulnerability. JAMA Psychiatry 2017, 74, 379–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Mora-Gonzalez, J.; Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Solis-Urra, P.; Migueles, J.H.; Cadenas-Sanchez, C.; Molina-Garcia, P.; Rodriguez-Ayllon, M.; Hillman, C.H.; Catena, A.; Pontifex, M.B.; et al. Fitness, physical activity, sedentary time, inhibitory control, and neuroelectric activity in children with overweight or obesity: The ActiveBrains project. Psychophysiology 2020, e13579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Cueli, M.; Areces, D.; Garcia, T.; Alves, R.A.; Gonzalez-Castro, P. Attention, inhibitory control and early mathematical skills in preschool students. Psicothema 2020, 32, 237–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Zhang, Z.; Wang, Q.; Liu, X.; Song, P.; Yang, B. Differences in Inhibitory Control between Impulsive and Premeditated Aggression in Juvenile Inmates. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Skowron, E.A.; Cipriano-Essel, E.; Gatzke-Kopp, L.M.; Teti, D.M.; Ammerman, R.T. Early adversity, RSA, and inhibitory control: Evidence of children’s neurobiological sensitivity to social context. Dev. Psychobiol. 2014, 56, 964–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Swingler, M.M.; Isbell, E.; Zeytinoglu, S.; Calkins, S.D.; Leerkes, E.M. Maternal behavior predicts neural underpinnings of inhibitory control in preschoolers. Dev. Psychobiol. 2018, 60, 692–706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Holochwost, S.J.; Volpe, V.V.; Gueron-Sela, N.; Propper, C.B.; Mills-Koonce, W.R. Sociodemographic risk, parenting, and inhibitory control in early childhood: The role of respiratory sinus arrhythmia. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2018, 59, 973–981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bruce, J.; Fisher, P.A.; Graham, A.M.; Moore, W.E.; Peake, S.J.; Mannering, A.M. Patterns of brain activation in foster children and nonmaltreated children during an inhibitory control task. Dev. Psychopathol. 2013, 25, 931–941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Zaidman-Zait, A.; Shilo, I. Parental ADHD Symptoms and Inhibitory Control in Relation to Parenting Among Mothers of Children with and Without ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Cotten, N.U.; Resnick, J.; Browne, D.C.; Martin, S.L.; McCarraher, D.R.; Woods, J. Aggression and fighting behavior among African-American adolescents: Individual and family factors. Am. J. Public Health 1994, 84, 618–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Cavazos-Rehg, P.A.; Krauss, M.J.; Spitznagel, E.L.; Schootman, M.; Bucholz, K.K.; Peipert, J.F.; Sanders-Thompson, V.; Cottler, L.B.; Bierut, L.J. Age of sexual debut among US adolescents. Contraception 2009, 80, 158–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Bumpus, J.P.; Umeh, Z.; Harris, A.L. Social Class and Educational Attainment: Do Blacks Benefit Less from Increases in Parents’ Social Class Status? Sociol. Race Ethn. 2020, 6, 233–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Cohen, G.L.; Sherman, D.K. Stereotype threat and the social and scientific contexts of the race achievement gap. Am. Psychol. 2005, 60, 270–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Burchinal, M.; McCartney, K.; Steinberg, L.; Crosnoe, R.; Friedman, S.L.; McLoyd, V.; Pianta, R.; Network, N.E.C.C.R. Examining the Black-White achievement gap among low-income children using the NICHD study of early child care and youth development. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 1404–1420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gorey, K.M. Comprehensive School Reform: Meta-Analytic Evidence of Black-White Achievement Gap Narrowing. Educ. Policy Anal. Arch. 2009, 17, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Hair, N.L.; Hanson, J.L.; Wolfe, B.L.; Pollak, S.D. Association of Child Poverty, Brain Development, and Academic Achievement. JAMA Pediatr. 2015, 169, 822–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Valencia, M.L.C.; Tran, B.T.; Lim, M.K.; Choi, K.S.; Oh, J.K. Association Between Socioeconomic Status and Early Initiation of Smoking, Alcohol Drinking, and Sexual Behavior Among Korean Adolescents. Asia Pac. J. Public Health 2019, 31, 443–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Ahmad, A.; Zulaily, N.; Shahril, M.R.; Syed Abdullah, E.F.H.; Ahmed, A. Association between socioeconomic status and obesity among 12-year-old Malaysian adolescents. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0200577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Merz, E.C.; Tottenham, N.; Noble, K.G. Socioeconomic Status, Amygdala Volume, and Internalizing Symptoms in Children and Adolescents. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2018, 47, 312–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Dismukes, A.; Shirtcliff, E.; Jones, C.W.; Zeanah, C.; Theall, K.; Drury, S. The development of the cortisol response to dyadic stressors in Black and White infants. Dev. Psychopathol. 2018, 30, 1995–2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hanson, J.L.; Nacewicz, B.M.; Sutterer, M.J.; Cayo, A.A.; Schaefer, S.M.; Rudolph, K.D.; Shirtcliff, E.A.; Pollak, S.D.; Davidson, R.J. Behavioral problems after early life stress: Contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Biol. Psychiatry 2015, 77, 314–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Miller, B.; Taylor, J. Racial and socioeconomic status differences in depressive symptoms among black and white youth: An examination of the mediating effects of family structure, stress and support. J. Youth Adolesc. 2012, 41, 426–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. DeSantis, A.S.; Adam, E.K.; Doane, L.D.; Mineka, S.; Zinbarg, R.E.; Craske, M.G. Racial/ethnic differences in cortisol diurnal rhythms in a community sample of adolescents. J. Adolesc. Health 2007, 41, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Alvarado, S.E. The impact of childhood neighborhood disadvantage on adult joblessness and income. Soc. Sci. Res. 2018, 70, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Barreto, S.M.; de Figueiredo, R.C.; Giatti, L. Socioeconomic inequalities in youth smoking in Brazil. BMJ Open 2013, 3, e003538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Schreier, H.M.; Chen, E. Socioeconomic status and the health of youth: A multilevel, multidomain approach to conceptualizing pathways. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 606–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Hemovich, V.; Lac, A.; Crano, W.D. Understanding early-onset drug and alcohol outcomes among youth: The role of family structure, social factors, and interpersonal perceptions of use. Psychol. Health Med. 2011, 16, 249–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Kaufman, J.S.; Cooper, R.S.; McGee, D.L. Socioeconomic status and health in blacks and whites: The problem of residual confounding and the resiliency of race. Epidemiology 1997, 621–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Bell, C.N.; Sacks, T.K.; Thomas Tobin, C.S.; Thorpe Jr, R.J. Racial Non-equivalence of Socioeconomic Status and Self-rated Health among African Americans and Whites. SSM Popul. Health 2020, 10, 100561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Samuel, L.J.; Roth, D.L.; Schwartz, B.S.; Thorpe, R.J.; Glass, T.A. Socioeconomic Status, Race/Ethnicity, and Diurnal Cortisol Trajectories in Middle-Aged and Older Adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2018, 73, 468–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Fuentes, M.; Hart-Johnson, T.; Green, C.R. The association among neighborhood socioeconomic status, race and chronic pain in black and white older adults. J. Natl. Med. Assoc. 2007, 99, 1160–1169. [Google Scholar]
  47. Assari, S.; Khoshpouri, P.; Chalian, H. Combined Effects of Race and Socioeconomic Status on Cancer Beliefs, Cognitions, and Emotions. Healthcare 2019, 7, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Assari, S. Number of Chronic Medical Conditions Fully Mediates the Effects of Race on Mortality; 25-Year Follow-Up of a Nationally Representative Sample of Americans. J. Racial. Ethn. Health Disparities 2017, 4, 623–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Assari, S. Distal, intermediate, and proximal mediators of racial disparities in renal disease mortality in the United States. J. Nephropathol. 2016, 5, 51–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Williams, D.R.; Costa, M.V.; Odunlami, A.O.; Mohammed, S.A. Moving upstream: How interventions that address the social determinants of health can improve health and reduce disparities. J. Public Health Manag. Pract. 2008, 14, 8–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Williams, D.R. Race, socioeconomic status, and health the added effects of racism and discrimination. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999, 896, 173–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Assari, S. Health Disparities due to Diminished Return among Black Americans: Public Policy Solutions. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2018, 12, 112–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Assari, S. Unequal Gain of Equal Resources across Racial Groups. Int. J. Health Policy Manag. 2017, 7, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H.; Bazargan, M. Association Between Parental Educational Attainment and Youth Outcomes and Role of Race/Ethnicity. JAMA Netw. Open 2019, 2, e1916018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H.; Mincy, R. Family Socioeconomic Status at Birth and Youth Impulsivity at Age 15; Blacks’ Diminished Return. Children 2018, 5, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Assari, S.; Thomas, A.; Caldwell, C.H.; Mincy, R.B. Blacks’ Diminished Health Return of Family Structure and Socioeconomic Status; 15 Years of Follow-up of a National Urban Sample of Youth. J. Urban Health 2018, 95, 21–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Assari, S.; Bazargan, M. Being Married Increases Life Expectancy of White but Not Black Americans. J. Fam. Reprod. Health 2019, 13, 132–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H. High Risk of Depression in High-Income African American Boys. J. Racial Ethn. Health Disparities 2018, 5, 808–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H.; Mincy, R.B. Maternal Educational Attainment at Birth Promotes Future Self-Rated Health of White but Not Black Youth: A 15-Year Cohort of a National Sample. J. Clin. Med. 2018, 7, 93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H. Family Income at Birth and Risk of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder at Age 15: Racial Differences. Children 2019, 6, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H.; Zimmerman, M.A. Family Structure and Subsequent Anxiety Symptoms; Minorities’ Diminished Return. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Assari, S. Parental Educational Attainment and Academic Performance of American College Students; Blacks’ Diminished Returns. J. Health Econ. Dev. 2019, 1, 21–31. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  63. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H. Parental Educational Attainment Differentially Boosts School Performance of American Adolescents: Minorities’ Diminished Returns. J. Family Reprod. Health 2019, 13, 7–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Assari, S. Parental Education Attainment and Educational Upward Mobility; Role of Race and Gender. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Assari, S. Parental Educational Attainment and Mental Well-Being of College Students; Diminished Returns of Blacks. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Assari, S. Parental Education Better Helps White than Black Families Escape Poverty: National Survey of Children’s Health. Economies 2018, 6, 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Assari, S. Blacks’ Diminished Return of Education Attainment on Subjective Health; Mediating Effect of Income. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Assari, S.; Hani, N. Household Income and Children’s Unmet Dental Care Need; Blacks’ Diminished Return. Dent. J. 2018, 6, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews Editorial Stuff. NIH’s Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. Alcohol. Res. 2018, 39, 97. [Google Scholar]
  70. Casey, B.J.; Cannonier, T.; Conley, M.I.; Cohen, A.O.; Barch, D.M.; Heitzeg, M.M.; Soules, M.E.; Teslovich, T.; Dellarco, D.V.; Garavan, H.; et al. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 43–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Karcher, N.R.; O’Brien, K.J.; Kandala, S.; Barch, D.M. Resting-State Functional Connectivity and Psychotic-like Experiences in Childhood: Results From the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. Biol. Psychiatry 2019, 86, 7–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Lisdahl, K.M.; Sher, K.J.; Conway, K.P.; Gonzalez, R.; Feldstein Ewing, S.W.; Nixon, S.J.; Tapert, S.; Bartsch, H.; Goldstein, R.Z.; Heitzeg, M. Adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Overview of substance use assessment methods. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 80–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Luciana, M.; Bjork, J.M.; Nagel, B.J.; Barch, D.M.; Gonzalez, R.; Nixon, S.J.; Banich, M.T. Adolescent neurocognitive development and impacts of substance use: Overview of the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) baseline neurocognition battery. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 67–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Auchter, A.M.; Hernandez Mejia, M.; Heyser, C.J.; Shilling, P.D.; Jernigan, T.L.; Brown, S.A.; Tapert, S.F.; Dowling, G.J. A description of the ABCD organizational structure and communication framework. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Garavan, H.; Bartsch, H.; Conway, K.; Decastro, A.; Goldstein, R.Z.; Heeringa, S.; Jernigan, T.; Potter, A.; Thompson, W.; Zahs, D. Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 32, 16–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Clark, S.V.; King, T.Z.; Turner, J.A. Cerebellar Contributions to Proactive and Reactive Control in the Stop Signal Task: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Dupuis, A.; Indralingam, M.; Chevrier, A.; Crosbie, J.; Arnold, P.; Burton, C.L.; Schachar, R. Response Time Adjustment in the Stop Signal Task: Development in Children and Adolescents. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 263–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Hiraoka, K.; Kinoshita, A.; Kunimura, H.; Matsuoka, M. Effect of variability of sequence length of go trials preceding a stop trial on ability of response inhibition in stop-signal task. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2018, 35, 95–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Carver, A.C.; Livesey, D.J.; Charles, M. Age related changes in inhibitory control as measured by stop signal task performance. Int. J. Neurosci. 2001, 107, 43–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. American Psychiatry Association (A.P.A.). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®); American Psychiatric Pubishing: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  81. Achenbach, T.M.; Rescorla, L. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles: An Integrated System of Multi-Informant Assessment; Aseba: Burlington, VT, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  82. ASEBA® Web-Link ASEBA Overview. 2020. Available online: https://aseba.org/aseba-overview/ (accessed on 5 May 2020).
  83. Assari, S. Education Attainment and Obesity Differential Returns Based on Sexual Orientation. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Assari, S.; Farokhnia, M.; Mistry, R. Education Attainment and Alcohol Binge Drinking: Diminished Returns of Hispanics in Los Angeles. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  85. Assari, S. Family Income Reduces Risk of Obesity for White but Not Black Children. Children 2018, 5, 73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Assari, S. Life Expectancy Gain Due to Employment Status Depends on Race, Gender, Education, and Their Intersections. J. Racial Ethn. Health Disparities 2018, 5, 375–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Assari, S.; Lankarani, M.M. Education and Alcohol Consumption among Older Americans; Black-White Differences. Front. Public Health 2016, 4, 67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Shervin, A.; Ritesh, M. Diminished Return of Employment on Ever Smoking Among Hispanic Whites in Los Angeles. Health Equity 2019, 3, 138–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Assari, S. Socioeconomic Determinants of Systolic Blood Pressure; Minorities’ Diminished Returns. J. Health Econ. Dev. 2019, 1, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  90. Assari, S. Socioeconomic Status and Self-Rated Oral Health; Diminished Return among Hispanic Whites. Dent. J. 2018, 6, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Assari, S.; Boyce, S.; Bazargan, M.; Caldwell, C.H. Mathematical Performance of American Youth: Diminished Returns of Educational Attainment of Asian-American Parents. Educ. Sci. 2020, 10, 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Assari, S.; Bazargan, M. Protective Effects of Educational Attainment Against Cigarette Smoking; Diminished Returns of American Indians and Alaska Natives in the National Health Interview Survey. Int. J. Travel Med. Glob. Health 2019, 7, 105–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Bowden, M.; Bartkowski, J.; Xu, X.; Lewis, R., Jr. Parental occupation and the gender math gap: Examining the social reproduction of academic advantage among elementary and middle school students. Soc. Sci. 2017, 7, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Chetty, R.; Hendren, N.; Kline, P.; Saez, E. Where is the land of opportunity? The geography of intergenerational mobility in the United States. Q. J. Econ. 2014, 129, 1553–1623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Assari, S.; Gibbons, F.X.; Simons, R. Depression among Black Youth; Interaction of Class and Place. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  96. Assari, S.; Gibbons, F.X.; Simons, R.L. Perceived Discrimination among Black Youth: An 18-Year Longitudinal Study. Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  97. Assari, S. Does School Racial Composition Explain Why High Income Black Youth Perceive More Discrimination? A Gender Analysis. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  98. Assari, S.; Lankarani, M.M.; Caldwell, C.H. Does Discrimination Explain High Risk of Depression among High-Income African American Men? Behav. Sci. 2018, 8, 40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  99. Assari, S.; Moghani Lankarani, M. Workplace Racial Composition Explains High Perceived Discrimination of High Socioeconomic Status African American Men. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. Assari, S.; Preiser, B.; Lankarani, M.M.; Caldwell, C.H. Subjective Socioeconomic Status Moderates the Association between Discrimination and Depression in African American Youth. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Assari, S.; Caldwell, C.H. Social Determinants of Perceived Discrimination among Black Youth: Intersection of Ethnicity and Gender. Children 2018, 5, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Boyce, S.; Bazargan, M.; Caldwell, C.H.; Zimmerman, M.A.; Assari, S. Parental Educational Attainment and Social Environment of Urban Public Schools in the U.S.: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Children 2020, 7, 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Jefferson, A.L.; Gibbons, L.E.; Rentz, D.M.; Carvalho, J.O.; Manly, J.; Bennett, D.A.; Jones, R.N. A life course model of cognitive activities, socioeconomic status, education, reading ability, and cognition. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2011, 59, 1403–1411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Assari, S.; Cobb, S.; Saqib, M.; Bazargan, M. Diminished Returns of Educational Attainment on Heart Disease among Black Americans. Open Cardiovasc. Med. J. 2020, 14, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Hudson, D.L.; Bullard, K.M.; Neighbors, H.W.; Geronimus, A.T.; Yang, J.; Jackson, J.S. Are benefits conferred with greater socioeconomic position undermined by racial discrimination among African American men? J. Mens. Health 2012, 9, 127–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  106. Hudson, D.L.; Neighbors, H.W.; Geronimus, A.T.; Jackson, J.S. The relationship between socioeconomic position and depression among a US nationally representative sample of African Americans. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2012, 47, 373–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Hudson, D.; Sacks, T.; Irani, K.; Asher, A. The Price of the Ticket: Health Costs of Upward Mobility among African Americans. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 1179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  108. Bartik, T.J.; Hershbein, B. Degrees of poverty: The relationship between family income background and the returns to education. SSRN Electron. J. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Assari, S.; Preiser, B.; Kelly, M. Education and Income Predict Future Emotional Well-Being of Whites but Not Blacks: A Ten-Year Cohort. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. Assari, S.; Bazargan, M. Second-hand exposure home Second-Hand Smoke Exposure at Home in the United States; Minorities’ Diminished Returns. Int. J. Travel Med. Glob. Health 2019, 7, 135–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Assari S, B.M. Unequal Effects of Educational Attainment on Workplace Exposure to Second-Hand Smoke by Race and Ethnicity; Minorities’ Diminished Returns in the National Health Interview Survey (NHIS). J. Med. Res. Innov. 2019, 3, e000179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Assari, S. Race, Intergenerational Social Mobility and Stressful Life Events. Behav Sci. 2018, 8, 86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  113. Assari, S.; Bazargan, M. Unequal Associations between Educational Attainment and Occupational Stress across Racial and Ethnic Groups. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 3539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Table 1. Data overall and by race/ethnicity (n = 4188).
Table 1. Data overall and by race/ethnicity (n = 4188).
All NHWs NHBs
n%n%n%
Ethnicity
 NHWs298571.32985100.0--
 NHBs120328.7--1203100.0
Sex
 Male202648.4142647.860049.9
 Female216251.6155952.260350.1
Marital Status *
 Other110326.539913.470460.0
 Married305473.5258486.647040.0
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Age (Year)9.450.509.450.509.460.51
Parental Educational Attainment *16.902.4217.581.9915.202.55
CBCL Anxiety Depressed *2.623.152.743.192.313.05
CBCL Withdraw Depressed *1.001.710.931.631.161.89
CBCL Somatic Complaints1.481.911.511.891.421.98
CBCL Social Problems *1.582.291.462.221.882.44
CBCL Thought Problems *1.662.251.712.221.542.32
CBCL Rule Breaking *1.251.941.071.691.702.40
CBCL Attention Problems *5.395.405.245.245.755.77
CBCL Aggressive Behaviors *3.444.583.274.323.855.15
14 SST-based IC0.540.110.540.100.540.12
Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL); Inhibitory Control (IC); Non-Hispanic Black (NHB); non-Hispanic White (NHW); Stop-Signal Task (SST); Standard Deviation (SD); * p < 0.05.
Table 2. Correlations between study variables (n = 4188).
Table 2. Correlations between study variables (n = 4188).
1234567891011121314
All
1 Race (NHBs)1−0.020.01−0.48 **−0.45 **−0.06 **0.06 **−0.020.08 **−0.03 *0.15 **0.04 **0.06 **0.02
2 Sex (male) 10.030.01−0.000.010.05 **−0.04 *0.05 **0.09 **0.11 **0.15 **0.10 **−0.11 **
3 Age (Years) 1−0.01−0.020.01.042 **0.020.000.010.020.000.00−0.03
4 Family Structure (married) 10.37 **−0.02−0.11 **−0.05 **−0.11 **−0.03 *−0.17 **−0.10 **−0.11 **−0.02
5 Parental Educational Attainment (Years) 10.00−0.11 **−0.04 *−0.13 **−0.07 **−0.18 **−0.10 **−0.11 **0.01
6 Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) Anxiety Depressed 10.57 **0.48 **0.61 **0.60 **0.40 **0.56 **0.56 **−0.02
7 CBCL Withdraw Depressed 10.40 **0.55 **0.49 **0.38 **0.47 **0.49 **−0.01
8 CBCL Somatic Complaints 10.43 **0.45 **0.29 **0.44 **0.39 **−0.03 *
9 CBCL Social Problems 10.61 **0.56 **0.70 **0.68 **−0.02
10 CBCL Thought Problems 10.52 **0.73 **0.64 **−0.06 **
11 CBCL Rule Breaking 10.67 **0.75 **−0.04 **
12 CBCL Attention Problems 10.76 **−0.05 **
13 CBCL Aggressive Behaviors 1−0.04 *
14 IC 1
NHWs
2 Sex (male) 10.03−0.02−0.030.010.06 **−0.05 **0.04 *0.08 **0.10 **0.15 **0.10 **−0.13 **
3 Age (Years) 10.01−0.030.030.05 **0.03−0.000.020.000.000.01−0.06 **
4 Family Structure (married) 10.17 **−0.07 **−0.12 **−0.09 **−0.11 **−0.07 **−0.13 **−0.11 **−0.10 **0.04 *
5 Parental Educational Attainment (Years) 1−0.08 **−0.12 **−0.12 **−0.16 **−0.13 **−0.18 **−0.15 **−0.14 **0.06 **
6 CBCL Anxiety Depressed 10.57 **0.47 **0.59 **0.57 **0.39 **0.53 **0.55 **−0.00
7 CBCL Withdraw Depressed 10.38 **0.53 **0.48 **0.40 **0.45 **0.48 **−0.03
8 CBCL Somatic Complaints 10.42 **0.43 **0.30 **0.44 **0.38 **−0.03
9 CBCL Social Problems 10.60 **0.56 **0.68 **0.67 **−0.03
10 CBCL Thought Problems 10.51 **0.72 **0.61 **−0.05 **
11 CBCL Rule Breaking 10.64 **0.74 **−0.04 *
12 CBCL Attention Problems 10.73 **−0.04 *
13 CBCL Aggressive Behaviors 1−0.03
14 IC 1
NHBs
2 Sex (male) 10.030.030.020.030.02−0.010.09 **0.12 **0.14 **0.17 **0.12 **−0.06 *
3 Age (Years) 1−0.010.02−0.010.030.010.010.010.04−0.00−0.000.04
4 Family Structure (married) 10.26 **−0.01−0.06 *−0.02−0.05−0.03−0.09 **−0.05−0.08 **−0.08 **
5 Parental Educational Attainment (Years) 10.07 *−0.050.07 *−0.01−0.01−0.07 *0.01−0.03−0.04
6 CBCL Anxiety Depressed 10.59 **0.51 **0.70 **0.67 **0.48 **0.64 **0.62 **−0.04
7 CBCL Withdraw Depressed 10.44 **0.58 **0.52 **0.34 **0.51 **0.50 **0.01
8 CBCL Somatic Complaints 10.46 **0.49 **0.31 **0.45 **0.42 **−0.04
9 CBCL Social Problems 10.65 **0.58 **0.72 **0.70 **−0.01
10 CBCL Thought Problems 10.57 **0.75 **0.69 **−0.07 *
11 CBCL Rule Breaking 10.72 **0.79 **−0.05
12 CBCL Attention Problems 10.80 **−0.07 *
13 CBCL Aggressive Behaviors 1−0.04
14 IC 1
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 3. Summary of linear regressions by race/ethnicity (n = 4183).
Table 3. Summary of linear regressions by race/ethnicity (n = 4183).
Model 1
NHWs
Model 2
NHBs
BetaSEpBetaSEp
Sex (Male)−0.13−0.03<0.001−0.06−0.010.047
Age−0.06−0.010.0020.040.010.165
Married household0.030.010.139−0.08−0.020.012
Parental Educational Attainment0.050.000.003−0.020.000.503
Constant 0.60<0.001 0.48<0.001
Unstandardized Regression Coefficient (B); Confidence Interval (CI); Standard Error (SE); Non-Hispanic Black (NHB); Outcome: Stop-Signal Task (SST)-based Inhibitory Control (IC).
Table 4. Summary of linear regressions overall (n = 4183).
Table 4. Summary of linear regressions overall (n = 4183).
Model 1
Main Effects
Model 2
Interaction Effects
BetaSEpBetaSEp
Race/Ethnicity (NHBs)0.020.000.3150.350.080.001
Sex (Male)−0.11−0.02<0.001−0.10−0.02<0.001
Age−0.02−0.010.118−0.020.000.131
Married household−0.020.000.300−0.020.000.386
Parental Educational Attainment0.030.000.0970.070.000.001
Parental Educational Attainment × NHBs −0.310.000.002
Constant 0.58<0.001 0.54<0.001
Unstandardized Regression Coefficient (B); Confidence Interval (CI); Standard Error (SE); Non-Hispanic Black (NHB); Outcome: Stop-Signal Task (SST)-based Inhibitory Control (IC).

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Assari, S. Parental Education and Youth Inhibitory Control in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 312. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050312

AMA Style

Assari S. Parental Education and Youth Inhibitory Control in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(5):312. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050312

Chicago/Turabian Style

Assari, Shervin. 2020. "Parental Education and Youth Inhibitory Control in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study: Blacks’ Diminished Returns" Brain Sciences 10, no. 5: 312. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050312

APA Style

Assari, S. (2020). Parental Education and Youth Inhibitory Control in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study: Blacks’ Diminished Returns. Brain Sciences, 10(5), 312. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050312

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop