Next Article in Journal
A Moderate Duration of Stress Promotes Behavioral Adaptation and Spatial Memory in Young C57BL/6J Mice
Next Article in Special Issue
Short-Term Benefits of Continuous Positive Airway Pressure Treatment on Cognition in the Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome: A Retrospective Study
Previous Article in Journal
A Review of Hand Function Rehabilitation Systems Based on Hand Motion Recognition Devices and Artificial Intelligence
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Functional Relationship between Inhibitory Control, Cognitive Flexibility, Psychomotor Speed and Obesity

1
Department of Experimental Medicine, University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”, 80138 Naples, Italy
2
Department of Psychology, University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”, 81100 Caserta, Italy
3
Neurological Unit, CTO Hospital, AORN “Ospedali dei Colli”, 80131 Naples, Italy
4
Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Foggia, 71122 Foggia, Italy
5
Department of Education Sciences, Psychology, and Communication, University of Bari, 70121 Bari, Italy
6
Department of Movement Sciences and Wellbeing, University of Naples “Parthenope”, 80138 Naples, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Brain Sci. 2022, 12(8), 1080; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12081080
Submission received: 13 July 2022 / Revised: 3 August 2022 / Accepted: 13 August 2022 / Published: 15 August 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Cognitive Functioning in Obesity: New Evidence from Neuropsychology)

Abstract

:
In the last decades, it has been proposed that executive functions may be particularly vulnerable to weight-related issues. However, evidence on the matter is mixed, especially when the effects of sociodemographic variables are weighted. Thus, the current study aimed at further examining the relationship between executive functions and obesity. To this aim, we compared treatment-seeking overweight, obese, and morbidly obese patients with normal-weight control participants. We examined general executive functioning (Frontal Assessment Battery–15) and different executive subdomains (e.g., inhibitory control, verbal fluency, and psychomotor speed) in a clinical sample including 208 outpatients with different degrees of BMI (52 overweight, BMI 25–30, M age = 34.38; 76 obese, BMI 30–40, M age = 38.00; 80 morbidly obese, BMI > 40, M age = 36.20). Ninety-six normal-weight subjects served as controls. No difference on executive scores was detected when obese patients were compared with over- or normal-weight subjects. Morbidly obese patients reported lower performance on executive scores than obese, overweight, and normal-weight subjects. Between-group difference emerged also when relevant covariates were taken into account. Our results support the view that morbid obesity is associated with lower executive performance, also considering the critical role exerted by sociodemographic (i.e., sex, age, and education) variables. Our results support the view that executive functioning should be accounted into the management of the obese patient because of non-negligible clinical relevance in diagnostic, therapeutic, and prognostic terms.

1. Introduction

Obesity represents a strong concern for public health [1] since it is linked to decreased quality of life and worse prognosis for concomitant chronic diseases [2]. Moreover, obesity is a major risk factor for several morbidities [3,4,5,6]. Recently, scientific research has looked beyond the physical consequences of obesity and sought to explore the association between obesity and cognitive processes [7]. Indeed, it appears that obesity predicts adverse neurocognitive outcomes [8]. More specifically, obesity would increase the risk of cognitive defects, besides being a potential predictor of cognitive decline and mild/major neurocognitive disorders [9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19].
The first generation of studies investigating the relationship between obesity and cognition found that obese patients performed worse on tasks assessing global cognitive functioning, with particular reference to attention and memory [20,21], showing also higher longitudinal decline than their leaner peers [22,23,24]. Subsequently, the same research line has moved the attention towards executive function (EFs). Strictly related to neural activity within the prefrontal cortex (PFC), with particular reference to dorsolateral and orbitofrontal cortices, EFs represent higher cognitive processes supporting cognitive control [25,26] and goal-directed behaviours [27]. EFs embrace several domains (e.g., attention, processing speed, set-shifting, inhibitory control, working memory, concept formation, and problem solving) [28,29,30,31,32] involved in applying adaptive behaviors to efficiently face environmental demands, especially in conflicting contexts [28,30,33]. Particularly, EFs act to inhibit dominant responses or interfering stimuli, and to resist temptations [27].
The prefrontal cortex, in concert with mesolimbic structures, is engaged in the food self-regulation process related to reward mechanisms and in processing the strong hedonic component of food choice [25,34]. Interestingly, inefficiency of frontal/executive processes was found to be associated with weak food inhibition [35,36,37], increased fatty foods intake [38], poor appetite regulation [25], insufficient physical activity [39], higher emotional eating [40], delay in weight loss [35], and lower adherence to, and unsatisfactory outcomes following, dietary interventions [41,42]. These findings are supported by neuroimaging studies showing hypoperfusion within frontal territories and the adjacent portions of temporal and parietal cortices in patients with a higher body mass index (BMI) [43,44,45]. However, some investigations have reported conflicting evidence suggesting that obese subjects would get, in line with the “obesity paradox” [46,47], equal or better performance than normal-weight subjects on executive tasks [24,48,49,50].
Previous research on obesity has provided mixed evidence, especially over the inhibitory control, verbal fluency, and psychomotor speed domains [7,25,51,52,53,54]. Inhibitory control allows the voluntary suppression of prepotent/habitual responses in line with task demands. Although some studies have shown that obese subjects demonstrated lower inhibitory control than normal-weight subjects [24,35,54], other studies have found no relationship between obesity and inhibition processes [7,55,56,57]. Verbal fluency enables spontaneous retrieval of specific information within phonemic or semantic constraints [58,59]. Although some studies have found that obese subjects performed worse than non-obese ones [24,60], other investigations have shown no between-group differences [61] or even better performance in obese participants [7,48,54]. Finally, psychomotor speed underlies the ability to detect and respond to rapid changes in the environment, such as the presence of a stimulus [59]. Also in the latter case, some studies have reported lower performance in obese subjects compared with controls [62], while other evidence suggests a lack of significant between-group differences [63] or better performance in obese patients [64].
Experimental studies involving subjects with Class III obesity (formally known as “morbid obesity”) are limited [34,65,66]. In this population, a number of comorbidities may independently affect cognitive performance. Indeed, obesity increases, per se, the risk of cardio-cerebrovascular diseases, diabetes, and metabolic syndrome; this could sully the relationship between obesity and cognition [67,68]. However, it has been documented that cognitive inefficiency can also occur independently of medical comorbidities [48]. In addition, one should also note that many studies have failed to mention relevant data characterizing the study sample (e.g., sex, age, education, and general clinical status). Therefore, it is unclear whether obesity predisposes to cognitive failures, especially considering the possible confounding/mediating effects of covariates.
To disentangle the link binding obesity to EFs, the current study aimed at verifying whether differences exist on general executive functioning and selective executive subdomains (e.g., inhibitory control, verbal fluency, and psychomotor speed) between treatment-seeking overweight, obese, and morbidly obese patients, using normal-weight subjects as control participants. Particularly, we checked the modulating effects of sociodemographic characteristics (i.e., sex, age, and years of formal education) and morbidity indexes. Since obesity is a clinical condition that is often accompanied by a number of chronic diseases likely inducing neurofunctional changes, we expect to find a relationship between obesity and executive performance. In particular, we hypothesize that BMI-ranked groups, i.e., from normal-weight to morbid obesity, show different performances in the executive tasks employed.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Three hundred and four subjects agreed to participate in this study. Eighty were morbidly obese patients (sixty females, M age = 36.20, SD = 13.85; M education = 9.65, SD = 3.67; M BMI = 46.18, SD = 5.11), seventy-six were obese patients (thirty-six females, M age = 38.00, SD = 11.82; M education = 10.58, SD = 2.98; M BMI = 34.17, SD = 2.20), fifty-two were overweight patients (twenty females, M age = 34.38, SD = 10.57; M education = 13.00, SD = 0.86; M BMI = 27.25, SD = 1.10), and ninety-six were normal-weight subjects that served as controls (fifty-two females, M age = 30.00, SD = 6.76; M education = 12.58, SD = 1.39; M BMI = 23.74, SD = 1.55). Patients were recruited at the Department of Experimental Medicine (outpatient clinic of dietetics, sports medicine, and psychophysical health) of the University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli” (Italy). Inclusion criteria were age ≥18 years and formal education ≥5 years. Furthermore, all patients (from overweight to morbidly obese individuals) underwent at least the first outpatient examination, and they had been prescribed a low-calorie diet aimed at weight loss. Exclusion criteria were: BMI < 18.5 (i.e., underweight), previous or current history of intellectual and/or linguistic deficits, previous or current history of psychopathological, psychiatric or neurocognitive disorders (including non-progressive or metabolic dementia), history of alcohol or substance abuse/addiction, or ongoing pharmacological treatment with drugs interfering with cognitive abilities.

2.2. Materials and Procedure

For each participant, we collected demographic information, i.e., sex, age and education (years of education were recorded in accordance with the Italian schooling system: 5 years = primary school; 8 years = secondary school; 13 years = high school; 16 years = bachelor’s degree; 18 years = master’s degree; 20 years = graduate school, PhD), and anthropometric measures (i.e., BMI). Therefore, participants were administered a neuropsychological battery including a screening test of general executive functioning (i.e., the Frontal Assessment Battery–15), and three executive tasks assessing impulsivity/inhibitory control (i.e., Stroop Color-Word Test), cognitive flexibility (i.e., FAS verbal fluency test), and psychomotor speed (i.e., Digit Symbol Substitution Test), respectively. Finally, the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale was used for computing the number of concurrent diseases and related severity.
Frontal Assessment Battery–15 (FAB15) [69]. The FAB15 is a short cognitive screening battery exploring general executive functioning (abstraction, generativity, cognitive flexibility, planning, sensitivity to interference, and inhibitory control). The scoring range is 0–15 and a higher score reflects better performance.
Stroop Color-Word Test (SCWT) [70]. This task assesses the ability to inhibit interference from a dominant response tendency. Moreover, it appears to tap other cognitive domains such as attention, processing speed, cognitive flexibility, and working memory [71,72]. The interference effect on both execution time (Stroop-T) and errors (Stroop-E) served as dependent variables.
FAS Verbal Fluency Test (FAS) [59,73,74]. It is a test assessing phonemic verbal fluency, which requires participants to produce as many words as possible that begin with letters “F”, “A” and “S” (omitting personal proper nouns, surnames, and place names). The total number of produced words represents the dependent variable. The FAS fluency is considered a valid measure of selective attention, set-shifting, generativity, and self-monitoring abilities.
Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) [75,76]. The DSST is a pencil-and-paper test for assessing psychomotor speed; however, it is widely used to explore alternative domains such as processing speed, set-shifting, working memory, and associative and implicit learning. More specifically, participants are presented with a grid of numbers and matching symbols under which there is a test section with numbers and empty boxes. The task consists of filling as many empty boxes as possible with the appropriate symbol. The number of correct number-symbol matches completed in 90 sec is scored and entered as dependent variable.
Cumulative Illness Rating Scale (CIRS) [77]. The CIRS consists of fourteen categories related to different body systems and scores the severity of each condition on a five-point (0–4) Likert scale. Comorbidity and severity indexes entered as dependent variables.

2.3. Statistical Analyses

The assumptions of the Generalized Linear Model (GLM) were verified by checking univariate and multivariate normality. For descriptive purposes, we ran parametric (univariate Analysis of Variance, ANOVA) and non-parametric analyses (two-way chi-squared test, χ2), when needed. In addition, correlation analysis (Spearman’s correlation, ρs) was used to quantify the relationships between the variables under examination. The effects of sex, age, education, and morbidity (e.g., number of illnesses and the respective severity) were taken into account. Indeed, multivariate Analysis of Covariance (MANCOVA) was performed, with the five executive scores entering the model as dependent variables, the BMI subgroups (normal-weight, overweight, obese, and morbidly obese) as fixed factors, and sex, age, education, CIRS-morbidity, and CIRS-severity scores as covariates. Any post-hoc analysis was performed according to Bonferroni’s correction for multiple comparisons (pbonf). Eta squared (η2) or partial eta squared ( η p 2 ) were used to quantify the effect sizes. Statistical analyses were conducted by using IBM SPSS v. 26 and JASP packages.

3. Results

To assess univariate normality, skewness and kurtosis indexes were assessed. Values ranging from −2 and +2 suggest the absence of appreciable deviations from normality. Square root transformation (√Xi) was applied to normalize variables in line with skewness index (i.e., |1|< γ <|2|). Univariate outliers, i.e., z-scores higher than 3 in absolute terms, were removed. For multivariate diagnostics of outliers, Mahalanobis’ distance ( D i 2 ) was calculated. Accordingly, no multivariate outliers were detected (mean D i 2 = 9.92, df = 10, ps > 0.001). Multivariate normality was assumed by Mardia’s coefficient ( i = 1 N ( D i 2 ) 2   N ) = 0.33 < 120. Analysis of missing data revealed random missingness (MCAR) that was handled via the recommended multiple imputation method [78,79].

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

Sample characteristics are summarized in Table 1. A difference was found in the frequency of gender levels between the four BMI subgroups (χ2(3) = 20.50, p < 0.001, φ = 0.26). Particularly, according to the analysis of adjusted-standardized residuals (zr) [80], the number of females was significantly lower than expected (zr = −2.7) in the overweight subgroup; conversely, the number of females was significantly higher than expected (zr = 4.1) within the morbidly obese patients. Results of univariate ANOVA showed that female participants had a higher BMI than males [F(1, 302) = 11.90, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.06; female, M BMI = 34.51, SD = 10.22 vs. male, M BMI = 30.77, SD = 7.79]. No gender difference was found on the FAB15 score [F(1, 302) = 0.14, p = 0.20], DSST score [F(1, 302) = 3.21, p = 0.07], and Stroop-E [F(1, 302) = 0.41, p = 0.52], while differences emerged on the FAS score [F(1, 302) = 5.48, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.03] and Stroop-T [F(1, 302) = 7.66, p = 0.06, η2 = 0.02]. As for the FAS score, females outperformed males, whereas the opposite pattern was observed for the Stroop-T score.
A significant age effect was found among BMI subgroups [F(3, 300) = 8.70, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.08]. Post-hoc analysis revealed that obese (mean diff. = 8.00, SE = 1.67, pbonf < 0.001) and morbidly obese patients (mean diff. = 6.20, SE = 1.65, pbonf = 0.01) were older than normal-weight controls. As for the education variable, a between-group difference emerged [F(3, 300) = 29.41, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.23]; more specifically, normal-weight (vs. obese, mean diff. = 2.00, SE = 0.39, pbonf < 0.001; vs. morbidly obese, mean diff. = 2.93, SE = 0.38, pbonf = < 0.001) and overweight (vs. obese, mean diff. = 2.42, SE = 0.45, pbonf < 0.001; vs. morbidly obese, mean diff. = 3.35, SE = 0.45, pbonf < 0.001) subjects were more educated than obese and morbidly obese patients.
Finally, about the CIRS scores, we found a statistically significant difference on both CIRS-morbidity [F(3, 300) = 235.25, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.56] and CIRS-severity scores [F(3, 300) = 346.47, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.69], with a progressive increase of multimorbidity and severity from normal-weight to morbid obesity (all pbonf < 0.001).

3.2. Gender Differences in Each BMI Subgroup

In the normal-weight group, female participants were older [F(1, 95) = 10.99, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.10; female, M age = 32.00 years, SD = 7.68 vs. male, M age = 27.64 years, SD = 4.51] and more educated [F(1, 95) = 11.31, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.11; female, M education = 13.00 years, SD = 0.05 vs. male, M education = 12.09 years, SD = 1.95] than male participants. As for cognitive scores and morbidity indexes, no gender differences were detected (all ps > 0.09).
In the overweight group, females were younger as compared with males [F(1, 50) = 6.16, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.11; female, M age = 30.00 years, SD = 6.71 vs. male, M age = 37.12 years, SD = 1.67]. Instead, no difference was found on the education variable (p = 0.99). As concerns performance on executive tasks, males got higher scores than females on the FAB15 [F(1, 50) = 21.41, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.36; female, M = 12.50, SD = 1.71 vs. male, M = 14.17, SD = 0.38]; conversely, females performed better than males on both Stroop-E [F(1, 50) = 6.43, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.11; female, M = 0.20, SD = 0.25 vs. male, M = 0.94, SD = 1.28] and Stroop-T [F(1, 50) = 5.78, p = 0.02, η2 = 0.10; female, M = 9.45, SD = 5.40 vs. male, M = 13.87, SD = 7.03]. We did not find sex differences on FAS and DSST scores, not even on morbidity indexes (all ps > 0.06).
In the obese group, female participants were older [F(1, 74) = 8.10, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.10; female, M age = 41.89 years, SD = 10.10 vs. male, M age = 34.50 years, SD = 12.27] and more multimorbid than males [F(1, 74) = 10.57, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.20; female, M = 2.35, SD = 0.71 vs. male, M age = 1.71, SD = 0.56]. No additional differences were observed on the other variables of interest (all ps > 0.09).
In the morbidly obese group, females were older than males [F(1, 78) = 9.36, p = 0.003, η2 = 0.11; female, M age = 38.80 years, SD = 13.20 vs. male, M age = 28.40 years, SD = 13.08] but no sex differences emerged with regard to formal schooling (p = 0.83). In terms of executive functioning, gender differences were found on verbal fluency and inhibition domains, with females getting higher scores on the FAS [F(1, 78) = 52.66, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.74; female, M = 35.33, SD = 0.49 vs. male, M = 27.50, SD = 3.74] but performed worse than males on both Stroop-E [F(1, 78) = 4.89, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.06; female, M = 0.82, SD = 1.00 vs. male, M = 0.25, SD = 0.45] and Stroop-T [F(1, 78) = 14.42, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.16; female, M = 22.63, SD = 12.59 vs. male, M = 11.20, SD = 8.13]. ANOVA did not reveal sex differences on FAB15 and DSST scores, not even on morbidity indexes (all ps > 0.15).

3.3. Correlation Analysis

The Spearman’s correlation coefficients are tabulated and reported in Table 2. BMI strongly correlated with CIRS-morbidity (ρs = 0.892, p < 0.001) and CIRS-severity (ρs = 0.888, p < 0.001); it also showed a negative moderate correlation with year of education (ρs = −0.468, p < 0.001). Still, BMI showed weak and negative correlations with DSST (ρs = −0.293, p < 0.001), FAS (ρs = −0.273, p < 0.001) and FAB15 (ρs = −0.173, p < 0.05); conversely, positive correlations were found with Stroop-E (ρs = 0.188, p < 0.01), Stroop-T (ρs = 0.180, p < 0.01) and age (ρs = 0.136, p < 0.05). Age correlated moderately, in negative direction, with DSST (ρs = −0.532, p < 0.001) but positively with Stroop-T (ρs = 0.575, p < 0.001) and Stroop-E (ρs = 0.411, p < 0.001). In addition, weak positive correlations were found with CIRS-severity (ρs = 0.251, p < 0.001) and CIRS-morbidity (ρs = 0.231, p < 0.001). Finally, moderate negative correlations were shown between years of education and CIRS (CIRS-severity, ρs = −0.492, p < 0.001; CIRS-morbidity, ρs = −0.451, p < 0.001); moreover, education correlated positively with FAB15 (ρs = 0.531, p < 0.001), DSST (ρs = 0.456, p < 0.001) and FAS (ρs = 0.449, p < 0.001). A negative moderate correlation was found with Stroop-T (ρs = −0.362, p < 0.001) and Stroop-E (ρs = −0.362, p < 0.001).

3.4. Analysis of Variance for Executive Scores

Between-group differences were found on FAB15 [F(3, 300) = 27.78, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.30], FAS [F(3, 300) = 21.61, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.24], DSST [F(3, 300) = 26.76, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.21], and Stroop-T scores [F(3, 300) = 10.93, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.10]; no difference was detected on the Stroop-E [F(3, 300) = 0.85, p = 0.47] (see Table 1). Multiple comparisons post-hoc analysis revealed that both obese and morbidly obese patients reported lower performance on FAB15 than normal-weight and overweight subjects (all pbonf < 0.001); moreover, patients with morbid obesity got lower FAB15 scores than obese patients (mean diff. = −1.91, SE = 0.47, pbonf < 0.001). As concerns FAS, DSST, and Stroop-T scores, both obese and morbidly obese patients obtained lower scores than normal-weight and overweight participants (all pbonf < 0.001); however, no differences were detected between the two obese subgroups (FAS, mean diff. = |2.60|, SE = 2.49; DSST, mean diff. = |2.60|, SE = 2.40; Stroop-T, mean diff. = |1.74|, SE = 2.49).

3.5. Multivariate Analysis of Covariance

Results of MANCOVA (Table 3) showed a significant effect of BMI [F(15, 348) = 5.54, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.19] even after adjusting for the significant contribution of sociodemographic variables [sex, F(5, 114) = 3.80, p = 0.003, η p 2 = 0.14; age, F(5, 114) = 20.91, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.48; education, F(5, 114) = 13.45, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.37] and CIRS-severity score [F(5, 114) = 2.64, p = 0.03, η p 2 = 0.10]; the CIRS-morbidity score was not a significant covariate [F(5, 114) = 1.35, p = 0.24]. More specifically, between-group differences were found on the FAB15 [F(3, 160) = 20.58, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.28], FAS [F(3, 160) = 11.51, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.18], DSST [F(3, 160) = 6.18, p = 0.001, η p 2 = 0.10], and Stroop-T [F(3, 160) = 5.66, p = 0.001, η p 2 = 0.10]; conversely, no difference between BMI subgroups on the Stroop-E [F(3, 160) = 0.86, p = 0.46] were shown.
Pairwise comparisons based on Bonferroni’s correction showed no significant difference between obese patients and normal- and over- weight subjects in any of the executive scores (pbonf range = 0.08–0.99). As concerns patients with morbid obesity, they got lower FAB15 scores than normal-weight (mean diff. = −3.75, SE = 0.52, pbonf < 0.001), overweight (mean diff. = −4.99, SE = 0.54, pbonf < 0.001), and obese participants (mean diff. = −3.00, SE = 0.53, pbonf < 0.001). Still, morbidly obese patients obtained lower FAS scores as compared with normal-weight (mean diff. = −10.94, SE = 2.87, pbonf = 0.001) and overweight subjects (mean diff. = −9.60, SE = 3.01, pbonf = 0.01); however, no difference was found between obese and morbidly obese patients on this test (mean diff. = −2.33, SE = 2.95, pbonf = 0.25). Similarly, patients with morbid obesity reported lower DSST and higher Stroop-T scores as compared with normal-weight (DSST, mean diff. = −11.38, SE = 3.54, pbonf < 0.001; Stroop-T, mean diff. = 8.62, SE = 2.19, pbonf = 0.001) and overweight subjects (DSST, mean diff. = −10.99, SE = 3.75, pbonf = 0.001; Stroop-T, mean diff. = 7.09, SE = 2.29, pbonf = 0.01) while no difference was detected if compared with obese participants (DSST, mean diff. = 1.92, SE = 4.47, pbonf = 0.14; Stroop-T, mean diff. = 5.77, SE = 2.24, pbonf = 0.09).

4. Discussion

The current study was designed to compare the performance of morbidly obese, obese, overweight, and normal-weight participants on neuropsychological tasks exploring general executive functioning and specific (i.e., inhibitory control, verbal fluency, and psychomotor speed) executive subdomains. Our study encompassed the simultaneous contribution of sociodemographic variables (sex, age, and formal schooling) and comorbidity/severity indexes as covariates in a GLM.
In a scientific context in which the link between EFs and obesity needs to be further investigated [7,25,51,52,53,54,81], our findings showed that morbidly obese individuals got poor executive scores regardless of the significant effects of the above-mentioned covariates. Notably, in line with previous studies [50], we found no significant differences between obese, overweight, and normal-weight subjects on the executive domains explored. This evidence suggests that severe obesity, more than obesity, may represent a clinical condition that impacts on cognition. Although a few contributions are available on this topic [34,65,66], their results converge on the idea that morbidly obese patients, net of the presence of eating disorders, would show weaknesses in tasks assessing motor planning, verbal fluency, processing speed, attention/vigilance, and inhibition.
Our results showed that morbidly obese patients got lower FAB15 scores (i.e., lower general executive functioning) than normal-weight, overweight, and obese participants. Therefore, the FAB15 may be sensitive enough to detect executive blunting in morbid obesity and to differentiate morbidly obese from obese patients in terms of executive functioning. Morbidly obese patients scored worse on FAS, DSST and Stroop-T as compared with normal-weight and overweight subjects, although they got similar performance than obese patients.
As concerns the executive subdomains explored, poor performance on the FAS test underlies low self-monitoring/set-shifting capabilities. Reduced psychomotor speed (i.e., DSST) highlights some difficulties in detecting and responding appropriately to rapid changes in the environment. Finally, longer latency time in the Stroop Test suggests inadequate inhibitory control and thus decreased ability to voluntarily suppress interfering information or prepotent habitual responses.
From an anatomofunctional standpoint, the integrity of these domains seems to depend on a large frontal network including (i) anterior and posterior cingulate cortices, inferior frontal gyrus and medial frontal areas for verbal fluency, (ii) dorsolateral, ventromedial, orbital prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices for inhibitory control, and (iii) the frontoparietal network, especially the middle frontal gyrus and the posterior parietal cortex, for psychomotor speed. Great interest is held by the functional connectivity within the PFC, as well as between PFC and subcortical structures (e.g., basal ganglia, subthalamic nucleus, hippocampal formation) in patients with and without eating disorders [82,83,84,85,86,87,88]. The projections from the PFC towards hypothalamic circuits appear to be involved in the modulation of hunger and satiety signals, whereas the striatal and ventral midbrain circuits are relevant for reward processing [83,89,90]. In morbidly obese patients, likely also due to the more complicated clinical profile (e.g., metabolic syndrome) [91,92,93], abnormal activity within these neural circuits might affect EFs and lead to difficulties in planning regular eating patterns, inability to delay gratification, or inhibit prepotent responses to highly palatable foods [53].
To better understand the relationship between obesity and cognition, there is a need to examine the interaction between obesity and brain physiology. Many studies have found that obesity in middle age correlated with an increased risk of cognitive decline in later life [94]. Indeed, excessive body fat seems to be associated with reduced brain volume in cognitively healthy older adults, but in patients with cognitive deficits, it exerts an additional detrimental effect [95]. More specifically, greater body adiposity and elevated waist-hip ratio were found to be related with generalized structural alterations involving orbital frontal cortex, temporal and parietal cortices, and hippocampus [96,97,98]. In a structural brain mapping cohort study of patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s disease (AD), a higher BMI value was found to be correlated with brain volume reduction in the frontal, temporal, and parietal cortices (0.5–1.5%) per one unit increase in BMI [44]. In line with this vein, longitudinal studies have highlighted a significant increase in temporal lobe atrophy between 13 and 16% for each one-unit increment in BMI [45]. Still, brain scanning evidence has suggested that obese individuals showed lower gray matter density in the middle frontal gyrus, primary somatosensory cortex, and putamen than normal-weight controls [99,100].
Although the mechanisms underlying the relationship between obesity and cognitive functions need to be further investigated, physiology research has identified a reliable mechanism of action in the activity of the neuro-immune-endocrine systems. Adipose tissue fulfills neuro-immune-endocrine functions, participating in the homeostasis [101]. In obesity, adipose tissue produces cytokines such as interleukin (specifically IL1ß and IL6), interferon γ (IFNγ), Tumor Necrosis Factor α (TNFα), and Monocyte Chemoattractant Protein 1 (MCP1), which promote chronic inflammation [102,103,104,105,106,107]. Hypertrophy of adipocytes induces a state of chronic inflammation that causes changes in the brain by inducing neuroinflammation. For instance, some studies have shown that inflammatory processes, especially in the hypothalamus, often follow high-fat feeding [15,108,109]. Moreover, chronic low-grade inflammation alters the blood-brain barrier (BBB) due to endothelial dysfunction, generating neuroinflammation and increasing oxidative stress, often resulting in cognitive impairment [110,111]. Since the BBB interfaces the periphery with the central nervous system, it could represent the bridge between peripheral inflammation and obesity-related differences in cognitive processes. Studies on obese animal models and human obese patients have shown a relationship between diet and cognitive functioning, especially in working memory and learning [110,111,112].
Furthermore, systemic inflammation could predict cognitive decline and dementia [113,114,115,116]. Adipose tissue may also affect β-amyloid metabolism [117,118]. Indeed, some neuropathological features of AD (e.g., amyloid plaques, neurofibrillary tangles) appear to be most observed in obese compared with normal-weight older adults [119]. In addition, a higher concentration of amyloid plaques has been detected in the hippocampal formation of cognitive intact morbidly obese patients when compared with non-obese subjects [120,121]. A high-fat diet may increase either body weight or cognitive impairment [122]. Longitudinal data have shown that higher caloric intake was related to an increased risk of AD at 6.3 years follow-up [123]. In addition, high-fatty acids diets, or diets rich in simple sugars, may undermine brain physiology, harming the integrity of the blood–brain barrier (BBB) [124], protecting the central nervous system from bloodborne toxins. AD and vascular dementia are linked to BBB dysfunction [125] and longitudinal studies sustained a significant relationship between midlife obesity and lower BBB functioning [45].
This study supports the hypothesis that patients with morbid obesity show inefficiencies in executive functioning, including when the contributions of sociodemographic factors and comorbidity indexes are taken into account. The main limitation of the present study is that we used BMI as the sole indicator of obesity, although previous research has shown that it is not a good indicator of obesity, besides that its relationship to EFs is notoriously mixed. BMI is unable to discriminate between muscle and adipose tissues and cannot assess regional adiposity [126]. Conversely, waist circumference or waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) could be superior to BMI as measures of obesity since they showed higher correlations with obesity-related risk factors [127,128,129]. Furthermore, robust evidence is available on the predictive role of WHR for executive performance in obese patients [49]. An additional limitation, in terms of external validity, is that we tested treatment-seeking overweight, obese, and morbidly obese patients; therefore, our results are not generalizable to patients not requiring a dietary intervention. Finally, one should consider that, although some cognitive tasks assessing EFs are based on a verbal response set (e.g., verbal fluency, Stroop test), most executive tests require hand motor responses to be properly performed (e.g., the DSST) [130,131]. Thus, it could be interesting to test the mediation effects exerted by hand motor function in the relationship between EFs and obesity.

5. Conclusions

Our results support the relationship between severe/morbid obesity and poor EFs, also considering the contribution of sociodemographic variables and the severity of concurrent diseases. Specifically, results from the present study suggest that: (1) patients with morbid obesity demonstrate decreased general executive functioning compared with normal-weight, overweight, and obese participants; (2) morbidly obese patients score worse than normal-weight and overweight subjects on cognitive tasks exploring selective executive subdomains, namely, cognitive flexibility, psychomotor speed, and inhibition processes; (3) EFs need to be accounted into the management of obese patients due to the non-negligible clinical relevance in diagnostic, therapeutic, and prognostic terms; (4) the direction of the relationship between obesity and executive defects needs to be further investigated. The study of this matter remains a strong challenge for future research.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.L.M., C.R.I., I.V. and A.M.; methodology, M.L.M., C.R.I., A.I. and S.C.; software, C.R.I., G.D.M.; formal analysis, M.L.M., C.R.I. and G.D.M.; investigation, M.C., M.S., R.P. and A.S.; resources, G.M. and V.M.; writing—original draft preparation, M.L.M., C.R.I. and I.V.; writing—review and editing, M.C., M.S., R.P. and A.S.; supervision, A.I., S.C., G.M. and A.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Campania “Luigi Vanvitelli”.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. WHO. Obesity: Preventing and Managing the Global Epidemic. Report of a WHO Consultation. World Health Organ. Tech. Rep. Ser. 2000, 894, 1–253. [Google Scholar]
  2. Collins, J.; Meng, C.; Eng, A. Psychological Impact of Severe Obesity. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2016, 5, 435–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Abdelaal, M.; le Roux, C.W.; Docherty, N.G. Morbidity and Mortality Associated with Obesity. Ann. Transl. Med. 2017, 5, 161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Blüher, M. Obesity: Global Epidemiology and Pathogenesis. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 2019, 15, 288–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Hruby, A.; Hu, F.B. The Epidemiology of Obesity: A Big Picture. PharmacoEconomics 2015, 33, 673–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Polito, R.; Scarinci, A.; Ambrosi, A.; Tartaglia, N.; Tafuri, D.; Monda, M.; Messina, A.; Cimmino, F.; Catapano, A.; Sessa, F.; et al. The Beneficial Effects of Physical Activity and Weight Loss on Human Colorectal Carcinoma Cell Lines. In Proceedings of the Journal of Human Sport and Exercise—2020—Winter Conferences of Sports Science, Alicante, Spain, 24 April 2020; Universidad de Alicante: Alicante, Spain, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  7. Fitzpatrick, S.; Gilbert, S.; Serpell, L. Systematic Review: Are Overweight and Obese Individuals Impaired on Behavioural Tasks of Executive Functioning? Neuropsychol. Rev. 2013, 23, 138–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Donofry, S.D.; Stillman, C.M.; Erickson, K.I. A Review of the Relationship between Eating Behavior, Obesity and Functional Brain Network Organization. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2020, 15, 1157–1181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Gustafson, D.; Rothenberg, E.; Blennow, K.; Steen, B.; Skoog, I. An 18-Year Follow-up of Overweight and Risk of Alzheimer Disease. Arch. Intern. Med. 2003, 163, 1524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Rosengren, A. Body Mass Index, Other Cardiovascular Risk Factors, and Hospitalization for Dementia. Arch. Intern. Med. 2005, 165, 321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Panza, F.; D’Introno, A.; Colacicco, A.M.; Capurso, C.; Pichichero, G.; Capurso, S.A.; Capurso, A.; Solfrizzi, V. Lipid Metabolism in Cognitive Decline and Dementia. Brain Res. Rev. 2006, 51, 275–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Whitmer, R.A.; Gunderson, E.P.; Barrett-Connor, E.; Quesenberry, C.P.; Yaffe, K. Obesity in Middle Age and Future Risk of Dementia: A 27 Year Longitudinal Population Based Study. BMJ 2005, 330, 1360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Beydoun, M.A.; Beydoun, H.A.; Wang, Y. Obesity and Central Obesity as Risk Factors for Incident Dementia and Its Subtypes: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Obes. Rev. 2008, 9, 204–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Whitmer, R.A.; Gustafson, D.R.; Barrett-Connor, E.; Haan, M.N.; Gunderson, E.P.; Yaffe, K. Central Obesity and Increased Risk of Dementia More than Three Decades Later. Neurology 2008, 71, 1057–1064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Miller, A.A.; Spencer, S.J. Obesity and Neuroinflammation: A Pathway to Cognitive Impairment. Brain Behav. Immun. 2014, 42, 10–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bischof, G.N.; Park, D.C. Obesity and Aging: Consequences for Cognition, Brain Structure, and Brain Function. Psychosom. Med. 2015, 77, 697–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Dye, L.; Boyle, N.B.; Champ, C.; Lawton, C. The Relationship between Obesity and Cognitive Health and Decline. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2017, 76, 443–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Leigh, S.-J.; Morris, M.J. Diet, Inflammation and the Gut Microbiome: Mechanisms for Obesity-Associated Cognitive Impairment. Biochim. Biophys. Acta (BBA)-Mol. Basis Dis. 2020, 1866, 165767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Ma, W.; Zhang, H.; Wu, N.; Liu, Y.; Han, P.; Wang, F.; Wang, J.; Xie, F.; Niu, S.; Hu, H.; et al. Relationship between Obesity-Related Anthropometric Indicators and Cognitive Function in Chinese Suburb-Dwelling Older Adults. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0258922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Appelhans, B.M.; French, S.A.; Pagoto, S.L.; Sherwood, N.E. Managing Temptation in Obesity Treatment: A Neurobehavioral Model of Intervention Strategies. Appetite 2016, 96, 268–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Higgs, S.; Williamson, A.C.; Attwood, A.S. Recall of Recent Lunch and Its Effect on Subsequent Snack Intake. Physiol. Behav. 2008, 94, 454–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Dahl, A.K.; Löppönen, M.; Isoaho, R.; Berg, S.; Kivelä, S.-L. Overweight and Obesity in Old Age Are Not Associated with Greater Dementia Risk: (See Editorial Comments by Dr. David S. Knodman, pp 2349–2350). J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2008, 56, 2261–2266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Cserjési, R.; Luminet, O.; Poncelet, A.-S.; Lénárd, L. Altered Executive Function in Obesity. Exploration of the Role of Affective States on Cognitive Abilities. Appetite 2009, 52, 535–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Gunstad, J.; Lhotsky, A.; Wendell, C.R.; Ferrucci, L.; Zonderman, A.B. Longitudinal Examination of Obesity and Cognitive Function: Results from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neuroepidemiology 2010, 34, 222–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Gluck, M.E.; Viswanath, P.; Stinson, E.J. Obesity, Appetite, and the Prefrontal Cortex. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2017, 6, 380–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Dubois, B.; Slachevsky, A.; Litvan, I.; Pillon, B. The FAB: A Frontal Assessment Battery at Bedside. Neurology 2000, 55, 1621–1626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Ardila, A. On the Evolutionary Origins of Executive Functions. Brain Cogn. 2008, 68, 92–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Burgess, P.W.; Simons, J.S. Theories of Frontal Lobe Executive Function: Clinical Applications. In The Effectiveness of Rehabilitation for Cognitive Deficits; Halligan, P.W., Wade, D.T., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2005; pp. 211–231. ISBN 978-0-19-852654-4. [Google Scholar]
  29. Gilbert, S.J.; Burgess, P.W. Executive Function. Curr. Biol. 2008, 18, R110–R114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Diamond, A. Executive Functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Chieffi, S.; Messina, A.; Villano, I.; Valenzano, A.A.; Nigro, E.; La Marra, M.; Cibelli, G.; Monda, V.; Salerno, M.; Tafuri, D.; et al. The Use of Velocity Information in Movement Reproduction. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Chieffi, S.; Castaldi, C.; Di Maio, G.; La Marra, M.; Messina, A.; Monda, V.; Villano, I. Attentional Bias in the Radial and Vertical Dimensions of Space. Comptes Rendus Biol. 2019, 342, 97–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Toto, G.A.; Limone, P. Research on a massive open online course (MOOC): A Rapid Evidence Assessment of online courses in physical education and sport. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 2019, 19, 2328–2333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Ribeiro, O.; Carmo, I.D.; Paiva, T.; Figueira, M.L. Neuropsychological Profile, Cognitive Reserve and Emotional Distress in a Portuguese Sample of Severely Obese Patients. Acta Med. Port. 2020, 33, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Dassen, F.C.M.; Houben, K.; Allom, V.; Jansen, A. Self-Regulation and Obesity: The Role of Executive Function and Delay Discounting in the Prediction of Weight Loss. J. Behav. Med. 2018, 41, 806–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Lyke, J.A.; Spinella, M. Associations among Aspects of Impulsivity and Eating Factors in a Nonclinical Sample. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2004, 36, 229–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Steward, T.; Mestre-Bach, G.; Vintró-Alcaraz, C.; Lozano-Madrid, M.; Agüera, Z.; Fernández-Formoso, J.A.; Granero, R.; Jiménez-Murcia, S.; Vilarrasa, N.; García-Ruiz-de-Gordejuela, A.; et al. Food Addiction and Impaired Executive Functions in Women with Obesity. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2018, 26, 574–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Powell, D.J.H.; McMinn, D.; Allan, J.L. Does Real Time Variability in Inhibitory Control Drive Snacking Behavior? An Intensive Longitudinal Study. Health Psychol. 2017, 36, 356–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Riggs, N.; Chou, C.-P.; Spruijt-Metz, D.; Pentz, M.A. Executive Cognitive Function as a Correlate and Predictor of Child Food Intake and Physical Activity. Child Neuropsychol. 2010, 16, 279–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Sims, R.C.; Bennett, N.-K.; Mwendwa, D.T.; Ali, M.K.; Levy, S.-A.T.; Callender, C.O.; Campbell, A.L. Executive Function and Negative Eating Behaviors in Severely Obese African Americans. Ethn. Dis. 2014, 24, 328–334. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  41. Beth Spitznagel, M.; Galioto, R.; Limbach, K.; John, G.; Leslie, H. Cognitive Function Is Linked to Adherence to Bariatric Postoperative Guidelines. Surg. Obes. Relat. Dis. 2013, 9, 580–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Witbracht, M.G.; Laugero, K.D.; Van Loan, M.D.; Adams, S.H.; Keim, N.L. Performance on the Iowa Gambling Task Is Related to Magnitude of Weight Loss and Salivary Cortisol in a Diet-Induced Weight Loss Intervention in Overweight Women. Physiol. Behav. 2012, 106, 291–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. O’Brien, P.D.; Hinder, L.M.; Callaghan, B.C.; Feldman, E.L. Neurological Consequences of Obesity. Lancet Neurol. 2017, 16, 465–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Ho, A.J.; Raji, C.A.; Becker, J.T.; Lopez, O.L.; Kuller, L.H.; Hua, X.; Lee, S.; Hibar, D.; Dinov, I.D.; Stein, J.L.; et al. Obesity Is Linked with Lower Brain Volume in 700 AD and MCI Patients. Neurobiol. Aging 2010, 31, 1326–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Gustafson, D.; Lissner, L.; Bengtsson, C.; Bjorkelund, C.; Skoog, I. A 24-Year Follow-up of Body Mass Index and Cerebral Atrophy. Neurology 2004, 63, 1876–1881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Banack, H.R.; Stokes, A. The ‘Obesity Paradox’ May Not Be a Paradox at All. Int. J. Obes. 2017, 41, 1162–1163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Monda, V.; La Marra, M.; Perrella, R.; Caviglia, G.; Iavarone, A.; Chieffi, S.; Messina, G.; Carotenuto, M.; Monda, M.; Messina, A. Obesity and Brain Illness: From Cognitive and Psychological Evidences to Obesity Paradox. Diabetes Metab. Syndr. Obes. Targets Ther. 2017, 10, 473–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Boeka, A.; Lokken, K. Neuropsychological Performance of a Clinical Sample of Extremely Obese Individuals. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2008, 23, 467–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Hartanto, A.; Yong, J.; Toh, W. Bidirectional Associations between Obesity and Cognitive Function in Midlife Adults: A Longitudinal Study. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. La Marra, M.; Villano, I.; Ilardi, C.R.; Carosella, M.; Staiano, M.; Iavarone, A.; Chieffi, S.; Messina, G.; Polito, R.; Porro, C.; et al. Executive Functions in Overweight and Obese Treatment-Seeking Patients: Cross-Sectional Data and Longitudinal Perspectives. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Vainik, U.; Dagher, A.; Dubé, L.; Fellows, L.K. Neurobehavioural Correlates of Body Mass Index and Eating Behaviours in Adults: A Systematic Review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 279–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Emery, R.L.; Levine, M.D. Questionnaire and Behavioral Task Measures of Impulsivity Are Differentially Associated with Body Mass Index: A Comprehensive Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 143, 868–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Gettens, K.M.; Gorin, A.A. Executive Function in Weight Loss and Weight Loss Maintenance: A Conceptual Review and Novel Neuropsychological Model of Weight Control. J. Behav. Med. 2017, 40, 687–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Yang, Y.; Shields, G.S.; Guo, C.; Liu, Y. Executive Function Performance in Obesity and Overweight Individuals: A Meta-Analysis and Review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018, 84, 225–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Volkow, N.D.; Wang, G.-J.; Fowler, J.S.; Telang, F. Overlapping Neuronal Circuits in Addiction and Obesity: Evidence of Systems Pathology. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 2008, 363, 3191–3200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Monica, D.; Paulo, M.; Appolinário, J.C.; de Freitas, S.R.; Coutinho, G.; Santos, C.; Coutinho, W. Assessment of Executive Functions in Obese Individuals with Binge Eating Disorder. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 2010, 32, 381–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Favieri, F.; Forte, G.; Casagrande, M. The Executive Functions in Overweight and Obesity: A Systematic Review of Neuropsychological Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Studies. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Lezak, M.D. Neuropsychological Assessment, 5th ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2012; ISBN 978-0-19-539552-5. [Google Scholar]
  59. Strauss, E.; Sherman, E.M.S.; Spreen, O.; Spreen, O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary, 3rd ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK; New York, NY, USA,, 2006; ISBN 978-0-19-515957-8. [Google Scholar]
  60. Bauer, C.C.C.; Moreno, B.; González-Santos, L.; Concha, L.; Barquera, S.; Barrios, F.A. Child Overweight and Obesity Are Associated with Reduced Executive Cognitive Performance and Brain Alterations: A Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study in Mexican Children: Child BMI, Brain Structure and Function. Pediatric Obes. 2015, 10, 196–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Gonzales, M.M.; Tarumi, T.; Miles, S.C.; Tanaka, H.; Shah, F.; Haley, A.P. Insulin Sensitivity as a Mediator of the Relationship Between BMI and Working Memory-Related Brain Activation. Obesity 2010, 18, 2131–2137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Cournot, M.; Marquie, J.C.; Ansiau, D.; Martinaud, C.; Fonds, H.; Ferrieres, J.; Ruidavets, J.B. Relation between Body Mass Index and Cognitive Function in Healthy Middle-Aged Men and Women. Neurology 2006, 67, 1208–1214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Ariza, M.; Garolera, M.; Jurado, M.A.; Garcia-Garcia, I.; Hernan, I.; Sánchez-Garre, C.; Vernet-Vernet, M.; Sender-Palacios, M.J.; Marques-Iturria, I.; Pueyo, R.; et al. Dopamine Genes (DRD2/ANKK1-TaqA1 and DRD4-7R) and Executive Function: Their Interaction with Obesity. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e41482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Skinner, J.S.; Abel, W.M.; McCoy, K.; Wilkins, C.H. Exploring the “Obesity Paradox” as a Correlate of Cognitive and Physical Function in Community-Dwelling Black and White Older Adults. Ethn. Dis. 2017, 27, 387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Galioto, R.; Spitznagel, M.B.; Strain, G.; Devlin, M.; Cohen, R.; Paul, R.; Crosby, R.D.; Mitchell, J.E.; Gunstad, J. Cognitive Function in Morbidly Obese Individuals with and without Binge Eating Disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 2012, 53, 490–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Sargénius, H.L.; Lydersen, S.; Hestad, K. Neuropsychological Function in Individuals with Morbid Obesity: A Cross-Sectional Study. BMC Obes. 2017, 4, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Yoshitake, T.; Kiyohara, Y.; Kato, I.; Ohmura, T.; Iwamoto, H.; Nakayama, K.; Ohmori, S.; Nomiyama, K.; Kawano, H.; Ueda, K.; et al. Incidence and Risk Factors of Vascular Dementia and Alzheimer’s Disease in a Defined Elderly Japanese Population: The Hisayama Study. Neurology 1995, 45, 1161–1168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Segura, B.; Jurado, M.Á.; Freixenet, N.; Albuin, C.; Muniesa, J.; Junqué, C. Mental Slowness and Executive Dysfunctions in Patients with Metabolic Syndrome. Neurosci. Lett. 2009, 462, 49–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Ilardi, C.R.; Chieffi, S.; Scuotto, C.; Gamboz, N.; Galeone, F.; Sannino, M.; Garofalo, E.; La Marra, M.; Ronga, B.; Iavarone, A. The Frontal Assessment Battery 20 Years Later: Normative Data for a Shortened Version (FAB15). Neurol. Sci. 2022, 43, 1709–1719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Golden, C.J. Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses; Stoelting Company: Wood Dale, IL, USA, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  71. Kane, M.J.; Engle, R.W. Working-Memory Capacity and the Control of Attention: The Contributions of Goal Neglect, Response Competition, and Task Set to Stroop Interference. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2003, 132, 47–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Scarpina, F.; Tagini, S. The Stroop Color and Word Test. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Patterson, J. F-A-S Test. In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology; Kreutzer, J.S., DeLuca, J., Caplan, B., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 1024–1026. ISBN 978-0-387-79947-6. [Google Scholar]
  74. Spreen, O.; Risser, A.H. Assessment of Aphasia; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2003; ISBN 978-0-19-514075-0. [Google Scholar]
  75. Boake, C. From the Binet–Simon to the Wechsler–Bellevue: Tracing the History of Intelligence Testing. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2002, 24, 383–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Jaeger, J. Digit Symbol Substitution Test: The Case for Sensitivity Over Specificity in Neuropsychological Testing. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2018, 38, 513–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Conwell, Y.; Forbes, N.T.; Cox, C.; Caine, E.D. Validation of a Measure of Physical Illness Burden at Autopsy: The Cumulative Illness Rating Scale. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 1993, 41, 38–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Newman, D.A. Missing Data: Five Practical Guidelines. Organ. Res. Methods 2014, 17, 372–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Sidi, Y.; Harel, O. The Treatment of Incomplete Data: Reporting, Analysis, Reproducibility, and Replicability. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018, 209, 169–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Ilardi, C.R.; Garofalo, E.; Chieffi, S.; Gamboz, N.; La Marra, M.; Iavarone, A. Daily exposure to digital displays may affect the clock-drawing test: From psychometrics to serendipity. Neurol. Sci. 2020, 41, 3683–3690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Limone, P.; Toto, G.A. The psychological constructs and dimensions applied to sports performance: A change of theoretical paradigms. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 2018, 5, 2034–2038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Chambers, C.D.; Garavan, H.; Bellgrove, M.A. Insights into the Neural Basis of Response Inhibition from Cognitive and Clinical Neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2009, 33, 631–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Foldi, C.J.; Morris, M.J.; Oldfield, B.J. Executive Function in Obesity and Anorexia Nervosa: Opposite Ends of a Spectrum of Disordered Feeding Behaviour? Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2021, 111, 110395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Hurtado-Pomares, M.; Terol-Cantero, M.C.; Sánchez-Pérez, A.; Leiva-Santana, C.; Peral-Gómez, P.; Valera-Gran, D.; Navarrete-Muñoz, E.M. Measuring Executive Dysfunction in Parkinson’s Disease: Reliability and Validity of the Spanish Version of Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB-E). PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0207698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Rodríguez-Aranda, C.; Waterloo, K.; Johnsen, S.H.; Eldevik, P.; Sparr, S.; Wikran, G.C.; Herder, M.; Vangberg, T.R. Neuroanatomical Correlates of Verbal Fluency in Early Alzheimer’s Disease and Normal Aging. Brain Lang. 2016, 155–156, 24–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Villano, I.; Ilardi, C.R.; Arena, S.; Scuotto, C.; Gleijeses, M.G.; Messina, G.; Messina, A.; Monda, V.; Monda, M.; Iavarone, A.; et al. Obese Subjects without Eating Disorders Experience Binge Episodes Also Independently of Emotional Eating and Personality Traits among University Students of Southern Italy. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Chieffi, S.; Iavarone, A.; La Marra, M.; Messina, G.; Villano, I.; Ranucci, S.; Messina, A.; Piombino, L.; Dalia, C.; Monda, M. Memory for proprioceptive targets in bulimia nervosa. J. Psychiatry 2015, 18, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Monda, V.; Valenzano, A.; Moscatelli, F.; Salerno, M.; Sessa, F.; Triggiani, A.I.; Viggiano, A.; Capranica, L.; Marsala, G.; De Luca, V.; et al. Primary Motor Cortex Excitability in Karate Athletes: A Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation Study. Front. Physiol. 2017, 8, 695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. La Marra, M.; Caviglia, G.; Perrella, R. Using Smartphones When Eating Increases Caloric Intake in Young People: An Overview of the Literature. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 587886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Messina, A.; Monda, V.; Avola, R.; Moscatelli, F.; Valenzano, A.; Villano, I.; Ruberto, M.; Monda, E.; La Marra, M.; Messina, G.; et al. Role of the orexin system on arousal, attention, feeding behaviour and sleep disorders. Acta Med. Mediterr. 2017, 33, 645–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Fergenbaum, J.H.; Bruce, S.; Lou, W.; Hanley, A.J.G.; Greenwood, C.; Young, T.K. Obesity and Lowered Cognitive Performance in a Canadian First Nations Population. Obesity 2009, 17, 1957–1963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Manasse, S.M.; Juarascio, A.S.; Forman, E.M.; Berner, L.A.; Butryn, M.L.; Ruocco, A.C. Executive Functioning in Overweight Individuals with and without Loss-of-Control Eating: Executive Function and Loss-of-Control Eating. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2014, 22, 373–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Manasse, S.M.; Espel, H.M.; Forman, E.M.; Ruocco, A.C.; Juarascio, A.S.; Butryn, M.L.; Zhang, F.; Lowe, M.R. The Independent and Interacting Effects of Hedonic Hunger and Executive Function on Binge Eating. Appetite 2015, 89, 16–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Debette, S.; Seshadri, S.; Beiser, A.; Au, R.; Himali, J.J.; Palumbo, C.; Wolf, P.A.; DeCarli, C. Midlife Vascular Risk Factor Exposure Accelerates Structural Brain Aging and Cognitive Decline. Neurology 2011, 77, 461–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Raji, C.A.; Ho, A.J.; Parikshak, N.N.; Becker, J.T.; Lopez, O.L.; Kuller, L.H.; Hua, X.; Leow, A.D.; Toga, A.W.; Thompson, P.M. Brain Structure and Obesity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 31, 353–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Jagust, W.; Harvey, D.; Mungas, D.; Haan, M. Central Obesity and the Aging Brain. Arch Neurol. 2005, 62, 1545–1548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Ward, M.A.; Carlsson, C.M.; Trivedi, M.A.; Sager, M.A.; Johnson, S.C. The Effect of Body Mass Index on Global Brain Volume in Middle-Aged Adults: A Cross Sectional Study. BMC Neurol. 2005, 5, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Gorospe, E.C.; Dave, J.K. The Risk of Dementia with Increased Body Mass Index. Age Ageing 2006, 36, 23–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Pannacciulli, N.; Del Parigi, A.; Chen, K.; Le, D.S.N.T.; Reiman, E.M.; Tataranni, P.A. Brain Abnormalities in Human Obesity: A Voxel-Based Morphometric Study. NeuroImage 2006, 31, 1419–1425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Taki, Y.; Kinomura, S.; Sato, K.; Inoue, K.; Goto, R.; Okada, K.; Uchida, S.; Kawashima, R.; Fukuda, H. Relationship Between Body Mass Index and Gray Matter Volume in 1428 Healthy Individuals. Obesity 2008, 16, 119–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Salas-Venegas, V.; Flores-Torres, R.P.; Rodríguez-Cortés, Y.M.; Rodríguez-Retana, D.; Ramírez-Carreto, R.J.; Concepción-Carrillo, L.E.; Pérez-Flores, L.J.; Alarcón-Aguilar, A.; López-Díazguerrero, N.E.; Gómez-González, B.; et al. The Obese Brain: Mechanisms of Systemic and Local Inflammation, and Interventions to Reverse the Cognitive Deficit. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2022, 16, 798995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Murray, P.J. Macrophage Polarization. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 2017, 79, 541–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Guillemot-Legris, O.; Muccioli, G.G. Obesity-Induced Neuroinflammation: Beyond the Hypothalamus. Trends Neurosci. 2017, 40, 237–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Ouchi, N.; Parker, J.L.; Lugus, J.J.; Walsh, K. Adipokines in Inflammation and Metabolic Disease. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 2011, 11, 85–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Messina, A.; Monda, M.; Valenzano, A.; Messina, G.; Villano, I.; Moscatelli, F.; Cibelli, G.; Marsala, G.; Polito, R.; Ruberto, M.; et al. Functional Changes Induced by Orexin A and Adiponectin on the Sympathetic/Parasympathetic Balance. Front. Physiol. 2018, 9, 259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Villano, I.; La Marra, M.; Di Maio, G.; Monda, V.; Chieffi, S.; Guatteo, E.; Messina, G.; Moscatelli, F.; Monda, M.; Messina, A. Physiological Role of Orexinergic System for Health. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 8353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Chieffi, S.; Villano, I.; Messina, A.; Monda, V.; La Marra, M.; Messina, G.; Monda, M. Involvement of orexin in sleep disorders and neurodegenerative diseases. Curr. Top. Pept. 2015, 16, 49–54. [Google Scholar]
  108. Polito, R.; Francavilla, V.C.; Ambrosi, A.; Tartaglia, N.; Tafuri, D.; Monda, M.; Messina, A.; Sessa, F.; Di Maio, G.; Ametta, A.; et al. The Orexin-A Serum Levels Are Strongly Modulated by Physical Activity Intervention in Diabetes Mellitus Patients. In Proceedings of the Journal of Human Sport and Exercise—2020—Winter Conferences of Sports Science, Alicante, Spain, 24 April 2020; Universidad de Alicante: Alicante, Spain, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  109. Di Maio, G.; Alessio, N.; Demirsoy, I.H.; Peluso, G.; Perrotta, S.; Monda, M.; Di Bernardo, G. Evaluation of Browning Agents on the White Adipogenesis of Bone Marrow Mesenchymal Stromal Cells: A Contribution to Fighting Obesity. Cells 2021, 10, 403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  110. Tucsek, Z.; Toth, P.; Tarantini, S.; Sosnowska, D.; Gautam, T.; Warrington, J.P.; Giles, C.B.; Wren, J.D.; Koller, A.; Ballabh, P.; et al. Aging Exacerbates Obesity-Induced Cerebromicrovascular Rarefaction, Neurovascular Uncoupling, and Cognitive Decline in Mice. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2014, 69, 1339–1352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Castanon, N.; Luheshi, G.; Layé, S. Role of Neuroinflammation in the Emotional and Cognitive Alterations Displayed by Animal Models of Obesity. Front. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Di Maio, G.; Monda, V.; Messina, A.; Polito, R.; Monda, M.; Asmundo, A.; Di Nunno, N.; Villano, N.; Francavilla, V.C. Nanocarriers As A Possible Neuropeptides Delivery Systems in the Brain. Acta Med. Mediterr. 2020, 4, 2287–2292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Odegaard, J.I.; Chawla, A. Pleiotropic Actions of Insulin Resistance and Inflammation in Metabolic Homeostasis. Science 2013, 339, 172–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  114. Bruun, J.M.; Pedersen, S.B.; Kristensen, K.; Richelsen, B. Effects of Pro-Inflammatory Cytokines and Chemokines on Leptin Production in Human Adipose Tissue in Vitro. Mol. Cell. Endocrinol. 2002, 190, 91–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Trayhurn, P.; Wood, I.S. Adipokines: Inflammation and the Pleiotropic Role of White Adipose Tissue. Br. J. Nutr. 2004, 92, 347–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Pradhan, A.D. C-Reactive Protein, Interleukin 6, and Risk of Developing Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus. JAMA 2001, 286, 327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Lee, Y.-H.; Martin, J.M.; Maple, R.L.; Tharp, W.G.; Pratley, R.E. Plasma Amyloid-β Peptide Levels Correlate with Adipocyte Amyloid Precursor Protein Gene Expression in Obese Individuals. Neuroendocrinology 2009, 90, 383–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Jahangiri, A.; Wilson, P.G.; Hou, T.; Brown, A.; King, V.L.; Tannock, L.R. Serum Amyloid A Is Found on ApoB-Containing Lipoproteins in Obese Humans with Diabetes: SAA Lipoprotein Distribution. Obesity 2013, 21, 993–996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  119. Serrano-Pozo, A.; Frosch, M.P.; Masliah, E.; Hyman, B.T. Neuropathological Alterations in Alzheimer Disease. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 2011, 1, a006189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Mrak, R.E. Alzheimer-Type Neuropathological Changes in Morbidly Obese Elderly Individuals. Clin. Neuropathol. 2009, 28, 40–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Moroz, N.; Tong, M.; Longato, L.; Xu, H.; de la Monte, S.M. Limited Alzheimer-Type Neurodegeneration in Experimental Obesity and Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2008, 15, 29–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Anstey, K.J.; Cherbuin, N.; Budge, M.; Young, J. Body Mass Index in Midlife and Late-Life as a Risk Factor for Dementia: A Meta-Analysis of Prospective Studies: BMI and Risk of Dementia. Obes. Rev. 2011, 12, e426–e437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Luchsinger, J.A.; Patel, B.; Tang, M.-X.; Schupf, N.; Mayeux, R. Measures of Adiposity and Dementia Risk in Elderly Persons. Arch. Neurol. 2007, 64, 392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Nguyen, J.C.D.; Killcross, A.S.; Jenkins, T.A. Obesity and Cognitive Decline: Role of Inflammation and Vascular Changes. Front. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  125. Skoog, I.; Wallin, A.; Fredman, P.; Hesse, C.; Aevarsson, O.; Karlsson, I.; Gottfries, C.G.; Blennow, K. A Population Study on Blood-Brain Barrier Function in 85-Year-Olds: Relation to Alzheimer’s Disease and Vascular Dementia. Neurology 1998, 50, 966–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Stevens, J.; McClain, J.E.; Truesdale, K.P. Selection of Measures in Epidemiologic Studies of the Consequences of Obesity. Int. J. Obes. 2008, 32, S60–S66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  127. Janssen, I.; Katzmarzyk, P.T.; Ross, R. Waist Circumference and Not Body Mass Index Explains Obesity-Related Health Risk. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2004, 79, 379–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Price, G.M.; Uauy, R.; Breeze, E.; Bulpitt, C.J.; Fletcher, A.E. Weight, Shape, and Mortality Risk in Older Persons: Elevated Waist-Hip Ratio, Not High Body Mass Index, Is Associated with a Greater Risk of Death. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2006, 84, 449–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  129. Zhu, S.; Heymsfield, S.B.; Toyoshima, H.; Wang, Z.; Pietrobelli, A.; Heshka, S. Race-Ethnicity–Specific Waist Circumference Cutoffs for Identifying Cardiovascular Disease Risk Factors. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2005, 81, 409–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Ilardi, C.R.; Iavarone, A.; La Marra, M.; Iachini, T.; Chieffi, S. Hand Movements in Mild Cognitive Impairment: Clinical Implications and Insights for Future Research. J. Integr. Neurosci. 2022, 21, 67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Chieffi, S.; Messina, G.; La Marra, M.; Iavarone, A.; Viggiano, A.; De Luca, V.; Monda, M. Distractor interference in visual motor tasks. Hor. Neurosci. Res. 2014, 13, 151–160. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
CharacteristicsNormal-Weight (n = 96)Overweight (n = 52)Obese (n = 76)Morbidly Obese (n = 80)p-Value
BMI, mean (SD)23.74 (1.55)27.25 (1.10)34.17 (2.20)46.18 (5.11)
Sex (f/m)52/4420/3236/4060/20
Age in years, mean (SD)30.00 (6.76)34.38 (10.57)38.00 (11.82)36.20 (13.85)<0.001
Education in years, mean (SD)12.58 (1.39)13.00 (0.86)10.58 (2.98)9.65 (3.67)<0.001
CIRS-morbidity, mean (SD)0.23 (0.43)1.11 (0.32)2.04 (0.71)3.43 (0.62)<0.001
CIRS-severity, mean (SD)0.05 (0.10)0.26 (0.07)1.38 (0.13)1.67 (0.43)<0.001
FAB15, mean (SD)13.00 (1.76)13.50 (1.38)11.41 (2.68)9.50 (0.59)<0.001
FAS, mean (SD)45.35 (6.93)44.75 (9.78)34.80 (13.23)32.20 (4.56)<0.001
DSST, mean (SD)59.04 (12.03)61.15 (1.42)45.55 (20.57)43.50 (13.82)<0.001
Stroop-T, mean (SD)13.43 (8.17)12.17 (6.75)18.04 (8.05)19.77 (12.61)<0.001
Stroop-E, mean (SD)0.52 (1.12)0.65 (1.07)0.78 (1.08)0.69 (0.94)0.47
CIRS: Cumulative Illness Rating Scale; BMI: Body Mass Index; FAB15: Frontal Assessment Battery–15; FAS: FAS Verbal Fluency Test; DSST: Digit Symbol Substitution Test; Stroop-T: Stroop Color-Word Test-Time; Stroop-E: Stroop Color-Word Test-Error.
Table 2. Correlation Matrix.
Table 2. Correlation Matrix.
BMIAgeEducationCIRS-MorbidityCIRS-SeverityFAB15FASDSSTStroop-EStroop-T
BMI
Age0.136 *
Education−0.468 ***−0.085
CIRS-morbidity0.892 ***0.231 ***−0.451 ***
CIRS-severity0.888 ***0.251 ***−0.492 ***0.944 ***
FAB15−0.173 *−0.1060.531 ***−0.128−0.165 *
FAS−0.273 ***−0.0300.449 ***−0.202 *−0.235 **0.563 ***
DSST−0.293 ***−0.532 ***0.456 ***−0.269 ***−0.280 ***0.490 ***0.378 ***
Stroop-E0.188 **0.411 ***−0.304 ***0.260 ***0.220 **−0.520 ***−0.258 ***−0.432 ***
Stroop-T0.180 **0.575 ***−0.362 ***0.304 ***0.299 ***−0.267 ***−0.268 ***−0.659 ***0.456 ***
BMI: Body Mass Index; FAB15: Frontal Assessment Battery–15; FAS: FAS Verbal Fluency Test; DSST: Digit Symbol Substitution Test; Stroop-E: Stroop Color-Word Test-Error; Stroop-T: Stroop Color-Word Test-Time. Note: * p <0.05; ** p<0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Analyses of Covariance.
Table 3. Analyses of Covariance.
Sum of SquareFp-Value
BMI × FAB15167.5120.58<0.001
Sex7.624.07<0.05
Age2.871.530.06
Education72.5838.76<0.001
CIRS-morbidity8.634.61<0.05
CIRS-severity7.544.02<0.05
BMI × FAS2881.9311.51<0.001
Sex2.680.050.77
Age36.160.710.95
Education72.5821.04<0.001
CIRS-morbidity25.830.510.48
CIRS-severity48.970.960.33
BMI × DSST3558.236.18<0.01
Sex9.940.120.73
Age4554.8855.73<0.001
Education3123.2838.21<0.001
CIRS-morbidity81.521.000.32
CIRS-severity115.701.420.24
BMI × Stroop-T820.615.66<0.01
Sex164.175.92<0.05
Age1599.9157.72<0.001
Education24.070.870.35
CIRS-morbidity2.950.110.74
CIRS-severity133.594.82<0.05
BMI × Stroop-E2.770.860.46
Sex1.574.12<0.05
Age22.1058.15<0.001
Education8.8223.20<0.001
CIRS-morbidity1.744.57<0.05
CIRS-severity4.1310.88<0.01
CIRS: Cumulative Illness Rating Scale; BMI: Body Mass Index; FAB15: Frontal Assessment Battery–15; FAS: FAS Verbal Fluency Test; DSST: Digit Symbol Substitution Test; Stroop-T: Stroop Color-Word Test-Time; Stroop-E: Stroop Color-Word Test-Error.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

La Marra, M.; Ilardi, C.R.; Villano, I.; Carosella, M.; Staiano, M.; Iavarone, A.; Chieffi, S.; Messina, G.; Polito, R.; Scarinci, A.; et al. Functional Relationship between Inhibitory Control, Cognitive Flexibility, Psychomotor Speed and Obesity. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1080. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12081080

AMA Style

La Marra M, Ilardi CR, Villano I, Carosella M, Staiano M, Iavarone A, Chieffi S, Messina G, Polito R, Scarinci A, et al. Functional Relationship between Inhibitory Control, Cognitive Flexibility, Psychomotor Speed and Obesity. Brain Sciences. 2022; 12(8):1080. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12081080

Chicago/Turabian Style

La Marra, Marco, Ciro Rosario Ilardi, Ines Villano, Mario Carosella, Maria Staiano, Alessandro Iavarone, Sergio Chieffi, Giovanni Messina, Rita Polito, Alessia Scarinci, and et al. 2022. "Functional Relationship between Inhibitory Control, Cognitive Flexibility, Psychomotor Speed and Obesity" Brain Sciences 12, no. 8: 1080. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12081080

APA Style

La Marra, M., Ilardi, C. R., Villano, I., Carosella, M., Staiano, M., Iavarone, A., Chieffi, S., Messina, G., Polito, R., Scarinci, A., Monda, V., Di Maio, G., & Messina, A. (2022). Functional Relationship between Inhibitory Control, Cognitive Flexibility, Psychomotor Speed and Obesity. Brain Sciences, 12(8), 1080. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12081080

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop