1. Introduction
Ventilotherapy with continuous positive airway pressure (CPAP) represents the most effective treatment in the case of Obstructive Sleep Apnea Syndrome (OSAS), regardless of comorbidities and the severity of respiratory symptoms [
1]. Crucially, this treatment improves not only the respiratory symptoms, but it seems to restore those cognitive difficulties generally associated with OSAS [
2,
3,
4,
5,
6,
7].
The improvement of cognitive impairments after long-term CPAP treatment (i.e., three-to-six months) has been widely documented [
2,
3,
4,
7,
8,
9,
10,
11,
12]. On the contrary, the evidence about the short-term benefits (i.e., after less than two weeks of treatment) of CPAP on the cognitive difficulties in OSAS is more limited and quite dated. A short CPAP intervention (<15 days) seems especially related to improvement in vigilance (i.e., day-time sleep latency, as measured by the Multiple Sleep Latency Test [
13]) [
4,
6] and sustained attention (i.e., the ability to maintain attention while performing a specific task) [
3,
14,
15]. However, short-term and long-term memory and executive functions seem less sensitive to brief treatments [
14,
16]. Concerning psychomotor processing and visuospatial abilities, the evidence about a quick effect of CPAP treatment is controversial [
3,
14,
16].
To sum up, the previous evidence suggests that CPAP may have a short-term beneficial effect on certain cognitive abilities, whereas some other cognitive domains seemed not to improve. From a neuropsychological perspective, identifying the cognitive functions that can be shortly improved with the CPAP treatment and those that are not susceptible to a quick reversibility may enable the design of more efficient neuropsychological rehabilitative interventions, focused on those cognitive difficulties that cannot be improved with the CPAP use alone. This is especially valuable considering that neuropsychological rehabilitative treatments are usually effortful and time-consuming. From a clinical perspective, enhancing the short-term cognitive benefits of the CPAP therapy through informed communications may improve patients’ compliance. Many patients complain about the discomfort of the device, and adherence to the CPAP treatment is typically low [
1] (i.e., 8 to 15% of patients refuse CPAP treatment after the first usage and at least 50% of them do not use the device regularly within 1 year of treatment [
17]). Acknowledging the possible, upcoming advantages of the CPAP treatment on daily cognitive functioning, and thus quality of life, may facilitate the process of compliance to the treatment. Thus, we suggest that a more comprehensive and updated understanding of the possible short-term effect of CPAP treatment on the patients’ cognitive functioning is very relevant.
The aim of this research was to provide further and updated evidence about the possible association between a short-term (i.e., zero to 15 days) CPAP treatment in individuals affected by OSAS and cognitive functioning. To this purpose, we collected retrospective data concerning the cognitive profile of individuals with OSAS, as previously assessed with standard neuropsychological tests for other research purposes, who were about to start or had recently begun the CPAP therapy. The following cognitive domains were considered: verbal memory and learning; information processing speed; sustained, selective, and attentional executive control; and the executive functions of strategic reasoning, problem-solving, and mental planning. According to several pieces of evidence, these cognitive domains are severely affected by OSAS [
18,
19,
20,
21,
22,
23,
24] and they significantly impact individuals’ daily functioning and, possibly, the management of the CPAP. Additionally, in this research, the patients’ self-reported evaluation of psychological distress (i.e., depressive symptoms and anxiety) was recorded, given its possible confounding effect on cognitive functioning.
4. Discussion
In this retrospective study, we provided more recent evidence about the possible association between short-term CPAP therapy and cognitive functioning in OSAS, through a clinical approach.
Our results showed that as the time using the CPAP increased, the participants’ verbal learning abilities and verbal long-term memory improved. Hence, ventilotherapy might improve patients’ cognitive performance even after a few days of usage. However, this short-term benefit might be limited to certain cognitive functions, such as verbal learning and long-term verbal memory (i.e., here measured by the Selective Reminding Test [
36]). These results are consistent with previous studies reporting improving long-term verbal memory abilities after a prolonged CPAP treatment (3–6 months [
2,
8,
12,
51]), but not with those studies assessing the short-term benefit of ventilotherapy, which instead did not report any improvement in these cognitive domains [
6,
14]).
Nonetheless, the short CPAP therapy was not related to participants’ performance in the other cognitive domains investigated (i.e., information processing speed, sustained and selective attention, attentional executive control, and strategic reasoning). This evidence is especially unexpected concerning sustained attention, which was the cognitive function more consistently reported to benefit from short CPAP treatment (i.e., 1–15 days; [
3,
14,
15]). In our study, we used the computerized Attention Network Test [
38], which effectively evaluates three attentional networks simultaneously (alerting, orienting, and executive networks), providing separate, but interrelated, scores based on a precise recording of participants’ reaction times according to the patients’ behavior. This approach is hardly comparable with the traditional neuropsychological assessment of attention, as measured by pencil-and-paper tests, thus, possibly explaining the inconsistencies with previous findings. Then, we may encourage the use of computerized and easy-to-follow procedures to assess attention, instead using exclusively traditional pencil-and-paper tests. Because of the very simple instructions, the Attention Network Test [
38] is an open-source instrument that can be easily used across different age groups, educational levels, and pathological conditions [
52,
53,
54,
55]. Nonetheless, normative standardized data about this test should be necessary for its adoption also in clinical contexts. Additionally, our results suggest that the short-term use of CPAP was not related to the cognitive speed of information processing in our sample, as measured by the Symbol Digit Modalities Test [
37]; this evidence is in line with the results reported by Lim and colleagues (2007): the authors registered a benefit in cognitive functioning after two weeks of CPAP treatment. Overall, we may suggest that the possible dissimilar sensitivity of the measures adopted across different investigations may explain the inconsistencies found relative to previous studies, indicating that certain tests may be more suitable to detect short-term, subtle changes in cognitive functioning than others. Identifying those instruments more effective in detecting the possible impact of a (even short) treatment represents the first step to fruitful research in the field; hence, we suggest future studies should aim for this purpose.
It is interesting to note that that higher-order cognitive functions, such as strategic reasoning, problem-solving, mental planning (i.e., here measured by the Tower of London [
39]) and the attentional executive control (here measured by the Attention Network Test [
38]) were not related to a short CPAP treatment. Our results, which were in line with previous evidence [
3,
14,
16], may suggest that higher-order cognitive domains either require longer treatment to improve or they cannot be restored. The oxidative stress related to sleep apneas severely and negatively affects the brain structures and functioning, through different pathological processes such as the consequent vasogenic edema, the alteration of the extracellular/intracellular fluid balance, and neurons loss [
56]. The side-effect of nocturnal hypoxia on the brain has been reported in different and several cerebral areas [
57], explaining the heterogeneity of cognitive deficits observed in OSAS. However, these areas may not be equally sensitive to the restoring of the nocturnal oxygen intake. Likely, several nights of normal breathing are needed for the edema reabsorption, explaining the benefit of a prolonged use of the CPAP. On the other hand, residual cognitive deficits after long-term and even multidisciplinary (i.e., ventilotherapy and cognitive rehabilitation) treatments may reflect irreversible anoxic damage to the CNS [
5]. On the contrary, certain neural populations in the hippocampus, which is a key structure supporting memory and learning, are still capable of neurogenesis in adulthood, although this process seems inhibited by sleep fragmentation [
57]. We speculate that effectively reducing hypoxia while sleeping and improving sleep continuity may promote neurogenesis in the hippocampus, but not in other brain areas, rapidly counteracting the consequences of hypoxia. Eventually, this may explain why only learning and verbal memory were significantly associated with the days of CPAP treatment in our study. Nonetheless, it is conceivable that factors other than reduced hypoxia, which are secondary to ventilotherapy, possibly contributed to the association between the treatment and cognitive performance. Valencia Flores and colleagues (1996) observed that improvements in cognition (i.e., in verbal learning) after CPAP ventilotherapy were the result of improved vigilance, not of the treatment per se. Since in our study, we did not specifically assess the level of vigilance, we cannot exclude that higher scores in verbal learning, as well as in the long-term memory, may be due to increased vigilance or energy because of improved sleep quality. Indeed, it should be clarified whether a few days of CPAP use might be enough to counteract the side effect of hypoxia on the brain. From a clinical perspective, we may suggest assessing multiple cognitive domains in the neuropsychological assessment of OSAS, as well as managing rehabilitative cognitive interventions in parallel with the CPAP treatment, specifically addressing these cognitive domains which might be less susceptible to changes because of the ventilotherapy.
Other final considerations can be made about our results. Age and the body mass index (which is informative of the level of obesity) were related to the cognitive functioning in our sample. As both age and body mass index increase, the efficiency of executive control decreases (i.e., reaction time associated with executive control increases, here measured by the Attention Network Test [
38]); furthermore, aging was negatively related to the information processing speed (i.e., here measured by the Symbol Digit Modalities Test [
37]), verbal learning, and long-term memory (i.e., here measured by the Selective Reminding Test [
36]). This may not be surprising due to the documented detrimental role of aging and obesity on cognition [
58,
59,
60,
61,
62]. Indeed, aging is associated with an increased risk of many debilitating conditions (i.e., vascular factors, atherosclerosis, stroke, and inflammatory processes) as well as neurologic and psychiatric diseases, which all possibly affect cognition. Nonetheless, milder cognitive difficulties are also expected as a mere consequence of senescence (i.e., normative aging) [
63]. As for obesity, there is growing evidence that it is associated with structural changes in the brain, regardless of the presence of neurodegenerative diseases [
64,
65]. Conceivably, these anatomical alterations could cause the cognitive difficulties detected in patients suffering from obesity, mainly attributable to attention and executive function deficits, as well as to memory, decision-making, reasoning, and visuospatial difficulties [
33], with important consequences on individuals’ quality of life [
66] and eating behaviors (i.e., food-related impulsivity) [
67]. To sum up, aging and high body mass index might blunt the possible effect of the CPAP treatment, interfering with the interpretation of its effectiveness in the research setting and requiring extra care when tailoring complementary neuropsychological interventions in the clinical context. The possible concurrent effect of these components, at least on certain cognitive functions, should be carefully taken into consideration by both researchers and clinicians, especially since older age and obesity are both risk factors for the development of OSAS.
Finally, the level of anxiety and depression measured in our study was not linked to cognitive performance, in contrast with some previous evidence suggesting that psychological functioning may impact cognition [
41,
42]. However, it should be considered that in our case these variables were studied as possible concurrent predictors in the regression model (i.e., together with demographic and clinical indices), not as key independent variables. Moreover, the levels of anxiety and depressive symptoms in our sample were, on average, below the clinical cut-off, suggesting that our participants may suffer from very low levels of psychological distress.
Concerning the study’s limitations, the retrospective design represents a major drawback of this investigation. We could not plan pre- (i.e., baseline) and post-treatment evaluations at certain time points for all participants; indeed, we could not manage the variability or stratification of the number of days of CPAP treatment at the moment of testing. Furthermore, the retrospective and observational design adopted does not allow causal inferences about the effect of the CPAP treatment on cognition. Nevertheless, our purpose was to preliminarily explore the possible contribution of short-term CPAP to clarify whether an association would be found, acknowledging which cognitive domains would be involved the most. This preliminary knowledge may guide the design of randomized controlled trials or longitudinal investigations including pre- and post-treatment evaluations of patients’ cognitive profiles at certain time points, in comparison with individuals who did not receive any CPAP treatment. Additionally, the analyses performed relative to sustained attention, the strategic reasoning, and mental planning, were relatively underpowered, possibly explaining the absence of any significant predictive contribution of the CPAP treatment, and of the other independent variables considered, on these cognitive domains. However, considering the retrospective design of this study we could not increase the sample size.