Next Article in Journal
Co-Occurring Methylenetetrahydrofolate Reductase (MTHFR) rs1801133 and rs1801131 Genotypes as Associative Genetic Modifiers of Clinical Severity in Rett Syndrome
Next Article in Special Issue
Impacts of Kinematic Information on Action Anticipation and the Related Neurophysiological Associations in Volleyball Experts
Previous Article in Journal
The Persistent Paradox of Rapid Eye Movement Sleep (REMS): Brain Waves and Dreaming
Previous Article in Special Issue
Fast and Stable Responses during Decision Making Require Strong Inhibitory Processes in Soccer Players
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Identifying the Qualities of Attention and the Attentional Style in Indoor Team Sports: A Gender Comparison

1
Department of Motor Performances, Faculty of Physical Educational and Mountain Sports, Transilvania University of Brasov, 500036 Brasov, Romania
2
Faculty of Sciences and Letters, “George Emil Palade” University of Medicine, Pharmacy, Sciences and Technology, 540142 Targu Mures, Romania
Brain Sci. 2024, 14(7), 623; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070623
Submission received: 3 June 2024 / Revised: 17 June 2024 / Accepted: 20 June 2024 / Published: 21 June 2024

Abstract

:
Attention is an essential psychological component in sports games, which conditions sports success. The purpose of this study was to identify the attention style (internal or external) and the weight of attention qualities depending on the practiced team sport (basketball, volleyball, or handball) and gender (female or male), in athletes aged 15–18. A total of 177 active athletes (87 female (mean age ± standard deviation: 16.07 ± 0.94 years) and 90 male (mean age ± standard deviation: 15.96 ± 0.82 years)) were involved in the study, including 62 handball players (28 female and 34 male), 58 volleyball players (30 female and 28 male) and 57 basketball players (29 female and 28 male). In the study, two questionnaires were applied: one implemented to identify the attentive style with the two dimensions internal and external (standardized), called questionnaire for the assessment of attentional style in athletes (QASA), and one designed by us, called questionnaire to identify the weights of attention qualities according to the characteristics of the practiced sport (QAQCS), aiming to identify the most relevant quality of attention, depending on the practiced sport. Cronbach’s alpha for both questionnaires was between 0.701 and 0.855. The results recorded in the present study reveal a variation in the attentional style between the groups of athletes and between genders. The results of the study highlight that in handball, girls have a predominantly external style with a total of 10.213 points; in boys’ handball, the predominant focus of attention is internal with a total score of 9.087 points. Girls’ volleyball focus of attention is predominantly external, with 8.999 points; in boys’ volleyball, the attention style is internal, registering a score of 9.713 points. In girls’ basketball, the predominant focus of attention is internal, with a total score of 8.516 points; in boys’ basketball, the external attention style is predominant, with 9.213 points. Looking at the weight of attention qualities, it was found that the most relevant for girls is concentration and mobility for handball players, stability was identified in volleyball, and distributiveness in basketball. In boys’ handball teams, mobility is the most essential, just like in basketball, and in volleyball it was found that stability has the biggest impact. ANOVA analysis highlighted statistically significant differences between groups of sports games by gender category, at both subscales of (QASA), as well as QAQCS, p < 0.05. The results of our study highlight differences between the attentional styles and their qualities in relation to the gender and the specific sport played, which determines differentiated approaches to these psychological components.

1. Introduction

Psychological preparation is an important component in the optimization of sport performances and identifying the psychological characteristics and peculiarities of athletes requires special attention in order to make the methodology of sports training more efficient. Sporting success requires the optimization of all an athletes’ potentials, namely physical, functional, psychological, technical, and tactical. Attention is an essential component in practicing team sports games and identifying the style and qualities of attention facilitates the optimization of sports training [1,2].
Practicing sports games requires the development of some psychological skills: concentration, attention, mental imagery, emotional control, will, etc. [3,4]. The sports training process is a multidimensional and dynamic process focused on the established performance objectives [5,6].
In the case of team sports games, the impact of sports psychology on the awareness and development of the athletes’ psychological skills, in which attention plays an essential role, can contribute to the improvement of individual performance, with connotations on team performance [7,8,9]. Depending on the complexity, the level of performance, the strategy approached and the technical–tactical level of the athletes, the cognitive demands during games or sports training require higher indices in information processing, as well as a high level of the ability to concentrate and pay attention [10,11,12]. The optimization of the mental processes determines the maximization of the performance of the players in the sports confrontations by adapting the sports behaviors on the anticipatory tactical bases of the action of the opponents and the game strategies of the opposing team [13,14].
Team sports games are characterized by diversity and specificities regarding the number of players, the playing surface, the game rules, etc. Athletes’ ability to concentrate is influenced by a series of specific, internal or external stimuli [15,16,17]. The mental pressure to which athletes are subjected in training and competitions is determined both by internal stimuli, such as anticipating the movements of teammates or opponents, the desire for success and victory, positive emotions, fear of failure, etc., as well as by external stimuli: the audience, the quality of the field, the characteristics of the equipment, etc. [18,19]. According to specialists in the field of sports psychology [20], the most demanding psychological component in sports games is attentional concentration, which represents a complex process based on two types of information processing: conscious and unconscious [21,22]. Controlled or conscious informational processing requires physical and intellectual efforts from athletes, while involuntary or unconscious informational processing involves the athletes execution of the task without making a conscious effort but with a precise and clear focus [13,22,23]. Conscious processing is specific to the stage of learning, repetition of a motor skill, and unconscious processing takes place mainly in the stage of consolidation, perfection of motor skills, which over time, through repetitions, becomes automatic [19,24].
To describe attentional processes from the perspective of cognitive science, several terms are used: attention, concentration, focus, etc. Attention refers to the ability to selectively process certain information by directing cognitive resources to specific stimuli or tasks [25]. Focusing attention aims to direct mental effort on a certain aspect or characteristic of a stimulus [26]. There are two types of attention: selective attention, which is the ability to select a stimulus for focus in the presence of distractions, and divided attention, which is the ability to simultaneously focus on two or more things, performing two skills simultaneously [27]. Nideffer (1998), a sports psychologist, considers that athletes have a tendency to change their attention style in relation to the sport practiced and depending on the targeted performance objective [28]. The focus of attention has two dimensions: direction (internal–external) and width (wide–narrow) [29]. The internal and external directions of the focus of attention aim at an introspective and extrospective perspective, respectively. The second dimension, width, has an integrative (expansive) orientation, the selective extreme. Previous studies found that at the level of sports initiation, the attentional external style predominates [20,30], and at the level of high performance, complexity and variance are predominant, depending on the experience in sports and social and biological factors [31,32].
Taking into account the difference in the focus of attention on gender, a series of studies did not highlight significant differences between individual sports and team sports [33,34]. Studies have found that female athletes have a predominance for internal attention, aiming at the motor task [35,36,37], and aesthetic motivation compared to male athletes, who are motivated by the competitive attitude, having a tendency to focus external attention [36,38].
The study of attentional mechanisms, in conjunction with the psychological factors underlying motor learning, has been a concern of specialists who have devised a series of specific theories. The theory of information processing is known as the constrained action hypothesis by Wulf et al. (2021) [39], in which it was considered that focusing internal attention on the biomechanics of a movement can disrupt automatic movement planning/execution. The OPTIMA theory (performance optimization through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning) of Wulf and Lewthwaite [39,40], focuses on a holistic approach, through which learning is a consequence of the interactions between attentional and motivational factors. It is suggested that in ideal sensorimotor and motivational conditions, aimed at associating the goal with the action, functional connections are more effective in the brain networks. In sports activities, the promotion of learning based on autonomy in correlation with performance expectations and with an external focus direction can facilitate dopaminergic responses and optimal involvement in the motor task [39,40]. A recent approach, called the ecological dynamics account of attentional focus [41], takes into account the characteristics of the environment in relation to the direction of attentional resources, evidences that environmental stimuli can contribute to the improvement in the following: motor self-organization; decision-making capacities; and awareness and predicting the movements of teammates and opponents [42,43]. The previously mentioned theories explain the dynamics of the focus of attention in the preparation process aimed at obtaining sports performances; thus, the internal attention is correlated with the automatic movements that become more effective if they are left unattended by an external focus [39]. The recent literature [44,45] shows some reticence regarding the theory of the constrained action hypothesis [39] and the OPTIMAL theory [40].
Among the main qualities of attention, we identify the following: stability, volume, intensity (concentration), mobility, distribution, and distraction [46,47]. Stability of attention comprises the long-term maintenance of orientation and concentration on the same object or on the same activity. The intensity consists of a focus of internal excitation, and the nearby areas are relatively inhibited and inaccessible to disturbing factors. The volume of attention represents the number of objects or phenomena that can be included simultaneously in the field of clear reflection. Mobility or flexibility of attention is the ability to move attention from one object to another in short time intervals. The distribution of attention is characterized by the number of activities that a person can perform simultaneously without one interfering too much with the others. Distracting attention targets stimuli with a certain intensity that causes external inhibition mechanisms in the activity in which the person is involved [48,49]. In the process of theoretical psychological training, an important focus is on the ability to ignore distractions, which will significantly involve the ability of mental attention and concentration [50].
The specialized literature that address the qualities of attention are limited. A study carried out by Stavrev and Ivanov (2019) [51] aimed at the qualities of stability and concentration of attention in the game of university basketball and volleyball, did not find significant differences between the sports. Some studies have focused on the impact of one or two qualities of attention in field tennis and in recreational aerobic gymnastics programs, among students from the academic environment [52,53], in boxing [54], and in combat sports [55]. Based on the study of the specialized literature, we have not identified any study that addresses the identification of the impact of attention qualities depending on the team sport practiced and gender, at the level of active athletes aged between 15–18 years. The purpose of this study was to identify the attention style (internal or external) and the weight of attention qualities depending on the team sport practiced (basketball, volleyball, or handball) and gender (female or male), in athletes aged 15–18.
H1—The hypothesis of the study started from the assumption that the focus of attention is influenced by the practiced team sport and gender, in athletes aged between 15 and 18 years.
H2—The hypothesis started from the assumption that the impact of attention qualities is different depending on the team sport practiced and the type of athletes.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Before starting the statistical procedures for verifying the formulated hypotheses, it is necessary to carry out some procedures regarding the power of the test and the size of the effect. Power analysis integrates a set of procedures aimed at identifying fundamental parameters regarding the scientific relevance of particular methodological approaches such as: effect size, alpha, type II error, sample size, variability of distributions and, of course, power [56,57]. The power analysis was performed with the program G*Power 3, offered free-of-charge to users (open-source) by the Institute of Experimental Psychology at the Heinrich Heine University in Dusseldorf, version 3.1.9.7.
From the perspective of the type of power analysis, the “A priori” method was selected, a method that provides relevant information regarding the volume of the sample/samples depending on the desired level of test power, the type 1 error level (alpha), and the effect size. The values of the input and output parameters can be seen in Figure 1.
It is observed that the critical F has a value of 2.267 at an approximate power of 0.70, which corresponds to a type II error of 0.3 and is minimally accepted [58,59,60], and a minimum sample size of 174 for the 6 groups entered. The volume of the sample included in the research is 177. The graph in the upper part of Figure 1 illustrates the ratio between the H0 and H1 distributions, projects the intersection of alpha and beta type errors, and indicates a limited possibility of rejecting the null hypothesis.
We believe that such minimum requirements can be acceptable under the conditions of this exploratory experimental approach. In future research, we aim to increase the power of the test and the sample size included in the research to significantly increase the quality of the research. The graph presented in Figure 2 shows the variation of power under the conditions of the input parameters for the different sample volumes and will represent a benchmark for increasing the quality of future research.
The present study included a total of 177 registered active athletes from the following team sports: handball, volleyball, and basketball. The data of the study participants, by groups, are presented in Table 1.
The age and duration of practicing different sports were collected from the official documents of the sports clubs, provided by the coaches. Athletes are members of the municipal clubs in Brasov and Targu Mures (Romania) that have indoor team sports sections (volleyball, handball, and basketball); all the subjects are domiciled and come from an urban environment. The participants voluntarily participated in this study and adhered to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Review Board of Physical Education and Sports, UMFST Targu Mures, Romania, no. 37/29.04.2023. Inclusion criteria: active athletes, clinically healthy, age between 15 and 18 years, full completion of the questionnaires, minimum 3 years of sports experience. In the preliminary stage of this study, all participants were informed of the objectives of the study. Before applying the questionnaires, verbal informed consent was obtained from the coaches, all participants signed an informed consent, and their parents or guardians also signed an informed consent.

2.2. Study Design and Procedures

Observational research with a cross-sectional strategy was carried out between May and July 2023, Figure 3. The two questionnaires were applied physically, in a technical session with the agreement of the coaches (day 1) and athletes (day 2). During the technical sessions, the athletes were presented with the most relevant theoretical aspects regarding the concept of attention, the typology of the attention style, the qualities of attention, and relevant aspects regarding attention in indoor sports games (day 3). After the theoretical information sessions, the two questionnaires were applied to the athletes (day 3), with preliminary information before starting the questionnaires.

2.3. Assessment Tools

The study included the application of two questionnaires to the athletes practicing team games (handball, volleyball and basketball):
  • The questionnaire for the assessment of attentional style in athletes (QASA) [18], a standardized questionnaire, includes 6 items and evaluates the attention skills in the direction dimension, namely internal or external. Items 1, 2, and 6 assess the internal dimension, referring to scale A, and items 3, 4, and 5, assess the external dimension, referring to scale B. The rating scale per item was from 0 to 4, where 0—never; 1—rarely; 2—sometimes; 3—frequently; and 4—always. Internal attention focused on the following aspects: the ability to perceive what is happening on the field and with teammates; the ability to focus only on a sporting action; the ability to listen to the trainer’s instructions with a quick focus, without being distracted by one’s own thoughts, ideas, etc. The external dimension of attention includes the following: focusing on a player but having the ability to look at things as a whole; the ability to imagine a game situation from other people’s information; and the ability to know the evolution of teammates at any time.
  • The questionnaire to identify the weights of attention qualities according to the characteristics of the practiced sport (QAQCS). This questionnaire was designed by us, based on the typology of attention qualities and aimed at the incidence of attention qualities in the opinion of team sports athletes. The questionnaire included 6 items related to the 6 qualities of attention, namely: volume; stability; intensity (concentration); distributivity; flexibility (mobility); and distraction. Each item consisted in the appreciation of the qualities of attention according to the characteristics of the practiced sports game. The Likert rating scale from 1 to 5 was used for each item, thus 1—very little, and 5—very much.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

The statistical analysis was carried out with the SPSS 22 program, calculating the following statistical parameters: arithmetic mean, minimum (Min), maximum (Max), standard deviation (SD), and Cronbach’s alpha value (α) for the validity of the questionnaires. We used the Students t-test for the differences between groups according to gender (male and female); significance threshold selected for the study p < 0.05; and confidence interval for the mean (CI–95%) with lower and upper benchmarks. The mean difference between the sports game groups was analyzed using an ANOVA analysis of variance: Fisher test and multiple comparisons (LSD). To calculate the size of the effect, we used the statistical parameter Cohen’s d, where = 0.2 very small, 0.2–0.5 small, 05–0.8 medium, and >0.8 large. The percentage of variance was also calculated, which measures the proportion in which a mathematical model takes into account the variation (dispersion) of a given data set. The Kurtosis indicator was also used, which highlights the probability of the distribution of a random variable with a real value. A confirmatory factorial analysis of the newly designed questionnaire was also carried out using statistical parameters: correlation, KMO, and Bartlett’s test, effect size, and total variance explained. Power analysis was performed with the G*Power 3 program that provides relevant information on sample size in correlation with test power, type 1 error level (alpha), and effect size.

3. Results

3.1. Validity and Reliability—Questionnaire to Identify the Weights of Attention Qualities According to the Characteristics of the Practiced Sport (QAQCS)

The purpose of initiating the exploratory factor analysis procedure is to detect structure in the relationships between variables, resolve the collinearity problem, and validate the scale construct [57]. Due to the fact that the instruments were created on the occasion of this research and the factorial structure was not analyzed, the demersal is exploratory and not confirmatory. Since the wording items aim to assess the same construct, we expect a unifactorial structure in the instrument. Analyzing the correlations between the qualities of attention, according to Table 2, it is observed that the strongest correlation was registered between the qualities of attention flexibility and stability of 0.523 and flexibility with distraction of 0.420.
The value of the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin index (0.753), as well as the significance level of the test of sphericity (185.709; sig < 0.001), suggests the existence of one or more common factors, which justifies the initiation of the factor reduction procedure (Table 3).
Adequate communality values, which represent the multiple correlation coefficients for each variable, indicate an adequate factorial model (Table 4).
According to Table 5, it can be seen that although six factors were generated, only one manages to reach the selection criterion (Eigenvalue = 1). The variance explained by the factor that reaches the selection criterion is 41.727.
Table 6, component matrix, presents the list of variables and their contribution to the factor loading. So, we are discussing a one-factor solution to the statistical approach. Due to the fact that only one factor was extracted, no further component rotation was performed.
The reliability of the two questionnaires, by calculating the statistical index Cronbach’s alpha, highlights that the reliability of the two questionnaires was high and very high, for all study groups (Table 7).
After collecting the applied questionnaires and analyzing the results, we will present the most relevant statistical indicators recorded in the study. For the assessment of attentional style in athletes (QASA) questionnaire, the analysis of the data on the two internal and external subscales, depending on the arithmetic mean recorded for the specific items, reveals the following: Girls’ handball is a predominant external style with a total of 10.213 points compared to the internal style, where only 7.998 points were registered, the difference being 2.215 points. In boys’ handball, the predominant attention style is internal, with a total score of 9.087 points; the external score registered a value of 8.44 points, with a difference of 0.647. In girls’ volleyball, the attentional style is predominantly external with 8.999 points at a difference of 0.466 points compared to the external focus of attention, which registered a total score of 8.533 points; in boys’ volleyball, the attention style is internal with a score of 9.713 points compared to the external one of 9.571 points. In girls’ basketball, the predominant focus of attention is internal, with a total score of 8.516 points compared to the score recorded for the external style of 7.688 points, the difference being 0.828 points. In boys’ basketball, the external focus of attention predominates with 9.213 points, with a very small difference of 0.072 points, from the internal style, which recorded a total score of 9.141 points. In conclusion, in handball and volleyball for girls, the external focus of attention predominates, and in basketball, the internal style. For boys, in handball and volleyball, the internal attention style predominates, and in basketball, the external one. The Kurtosis index, regarding the distribution of the data, falls within the limits of normality for all targeted items for all study groups, being between −2.160 and 1.182 (Table 8).

3.2. Questionnaire for the Assessment of Attentional Style in Athletes (QASA)

The ANOVA analysis of variance (Table 9) allowed us to identify the differences between the arithmetic means of the two categories of girls and boys subjects according to the internal and external attentional style, in the questionnaire applied for the assessment of the attentional style. Analyzing the ANOVA results, we find statistically significant differences between groups of sports games by gender category, of both subscales (internal attention and external attention) of the questionnaire for the assessment of attentional style in athletes (QASA), p < 0.05.
Making multiple comparisons (Table 10), the groups of subjects were statistically significant, p < 0.05. The differences of the arithmetic averages on the groups of sports games fell between the lower and upper limits specific to the 95% CI confidentiality interval, for both subscales of the attention style questionnaire. In the girls’ groups, the biggest differences in arithmetic means were recorded for Ai between volleyball and basketball, with a value of 0.328 points, and for Ae at 2.524 points. In the groups of boys, the biggest difference in averages between the groups of athletes was recorded between volleyball and basketball, with Ai at 0.642 points and Ae at 0.357 points.

3.3. Questionnaire to Identify the Weights of Attention Qualities According to the Characteristics of the Practiced Sport

In the analysis of the results of the questionnaire for identifing the weights of attention qualities according to the characteristics of the practiced sport, taking into account the arithmetic mean, we find that in the groups of girls handball the most representative qualities of attention are concentration and mobility, registering a value of 4.071, and the least representative is distraction with 2.353. In volleyball, the girls consider that stability is the most relevant and the least relevant was the distraction, due to the regulation that does not allow making any form of noise during certain phases of the game. In basketball, the most relevant is distributiveness, and the least appreciated is distraction. In boys handball teams, it was found that mobility is the most relevant; in volleyball, it is stability; and in basketball, it is mobility. The least significant in all groups of sports games was distraction. The Kurtosis index, regarding the data distribution, falls within the limits of normality for all targeted items for all study groups, being between −1.956 and 0.420 (Table 11).
The application of the ANOVA analysis of variance (Table 12), allowed us to identify the differences for p < 0.05, between the arithmetic means of the two categories of subjects, girls and boys, regarding the qualities of attention. The ANOVA analysis highlights that the differences between the female groups from the three sports were statistically significant (F = 12.430, p < 0.01); also for the male groups, the differences between the handball, volleyball, and basketball groups were statistically significant (F = 78.991, p = 0.02).
By performing multiple comparisons (Table 13), the groups of subjects were statistically significant. The differences of the arithmetic averages on the groups of sports games, fell within the limits of the confidentiality interval 95% CI, for all the qualities of attention included in the questionnaire. In the girls’ groups, the biggest differences in arithmetic means were recorded between handball and volleyball at 4.361 points, and in boys between handball and basketball at 3.129 points. The effect size calculated between the two groups according to gender recorded a value of d = 0.67, which reflects a medium size of the effect.

4. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to identify the attention style (internal or external) and the weight of attention qualities depending on the practiced team sport (basketball, volleyball, and handball) and gender (female and male) in athletes aged 15–18. The results of the study reveal the fact that in female groups, attention has an internal dimension in handball and volleyball and an external one in basketball, while in boys, handball players present an internal dimension and in volleyball and basketball an external one. Identifying the type of attention depending on the team sports game practiced and depending on gender, we consider that it contributes to the understanding of the role that attention plays in the optimization of sports performance. The identification of the internal and external attentional types facilitates the understanding by coaches, psychologists, and athletes of the psychic mechanisms that can affect psychological and sports training. Our study substantiates other previous studies that highlighted the role and importance of attention in sports games.
In a previous study carried out by Biscaia et al. (2021) [61], on samples of girls playing handball, Biscaia et al. applied the Nideffer attentional and interpersonal style test (AIST) questionnaire (1976), targeting the attentional style in the age category of 15–16 years, and found that they presented an external dimension registering a score of 5.00 ± 1.09 compared to the internal dimension of 4.81 ± 0.91. In the 17–18-year-old age category, the external and internal dimensions were equal with a score of 4.38. These results are in line with the results of the present study [61], where an external dimension of attention to handball girls was also recorded. A previous study regarding the attention style of senior handball players, depending on the position in the game, found that it varies, but the external dimension of attention predominates [62], a finding that differs from the results registered by us; we consider that this fact is due to the difference in experience, our subjects being juniors (with a more limited sporting experience) and not seniors.
In accordance with our study, Summers et al. (1991) found that in basketball there is a variation in the dimensions of attention from internal to external according to age and experience in both girls and boys [63]. In the case of basketball, a difference in attentional style was recorded between genders, with boys registering an external dimension and girls registering an internal. These results are different from previous findings [64,65,66]. In volleyball, a previous study by Fontani et al. (2006) [67], on a sample of boys aged 17–18 years, found a dimension of the external attention style, a result that aligns with our result; the difference was in the experience of the game, which, in the mentioned study, was under 3 years. Previous studies found that in volleyball, the predominance of attention is external, findings that align with the results of our study [21,67,68].
A review on the focus of attention in team sports found that the external dimension is predominant, having a beneficial role in obtaining performances [66], compared to the internal dimension, which affects performance [69]. Previous studies regarding the determination of attentional style were carried out in both individual and team sports, in different age categories, such as basketball, athletics, handball, shooting, golf, badminton, etc. [13,70,71,72], having the practical–methodical objective of understanding and improving the effectiveness of coaching [73]. Other studies have focused on the correlations between visual attention and reaction speed in association with the type of sport practiced; the results being statistically significant in team sports [74,75]. Attention to athletes during sports performances is dynamic; they show changes due to the variation of focus points [41,42,76,77]. Taking into account the differences between genders, aiming at the dimensions of the focus of attention, in the learning process of sports skills, Wulf et al. (2003) [78] consider that female athletes show a greater motor learning advantage when they are provided with externally focused instructions, compared to boys. The results of our study highlight that in handball and volleyball, the attention style is external; the only exception is with girls’ volleyball, and for boys, the domestic style prevails in volleyball and handball. A series of studies have focused on highlighting different aspects of attention; thus, a study conducted on basketball players found that there are no significant differences between genders regarding visual attention [79]. Reigal et al. (2022) found that the attention span of athletes who practice open sports (which varies depending on a number of factors such as the movement of opponents and teammates, etc.) is better compared to those who practice closed sports (in which the external environment does not influence performance). This finding was also replicated in the evaluation by gender [80].
Based on the analysis of the present study, we found that the qualities of attention vary depending on the sport practiced and the gender of the athletes. Thus, for the female groups, the most relevant qualities of attention were very different, which are as follows: in handball, concentration and mobility recorded an identical score; in volleyball, stability was the most relevant; for basketball, distributiveness was considered the most important quality of attention. In the male groups, handball and basketball players appreciated the mobility of attention the most, and volleyball players considered the stability of attention to be the most important.
Regarding the relevance of attention qualities depending on the practiced sport and the identification of studies that align with our results, there are very few. Stavrev and Ivanov (2019) analyzed two of the qualities of attention, namely stability and concentration, starting from the assumption that they are most representative in boys’ basketball and volleyball games, they found that concentration is significant in both groups, in both sports, while the stability in the basketball game did not present a good homogeneity [54]. Another study conducted on 30 girls who play handball (mean age = 14.33 years, SD = 0.48), analyzed the mobility and concentration of attention in correlation with sports performance by applying a linguistic intervention program, and found that there are significant positive correlations between the investigated intellect dimensions (analogous transfer and attention mobility) and preferential status index values [55,56]. Flexibility of attention, according to our study, is the quality with the greatest impact on handball in both genders, as well as in boys basketball. Flexibility (mobility) is conditioned by an optimal level of activation in order to dispose of all attentional resources, and motor skills have a high degree of automation [18,81], aspects that correlate with the experience of our subjects. Hutterman et al., in a study on motor response, found that players in team sports are more concerned with improving their attentional concentration and making correct decisions compared to those who practice individual sports, a finding that aligns with the results of our study regarding the flexibility and stability of attention [82]. Analyzing the specialized literature, we found that most of the studies carried out on the qualities of attention are focused on medical fields, targeting mental deficiencies, and were not focused on healthy people or on athletes. In this frame of thought, we consider that the present study presents an essential contribution to the understanding in the way in which the qualities of attention are appreciated by athletes from different team games. Based on the results of our study, we believe that specialists can better understand the mechanisms of attention depending on the characteristics of the practiced sport and will be able to optimally guide the psychological preparation process with new scientific information. Distraction of attention in all groups of subjects recorded the lowest values. We consider that this aspect is due to the game experience that offers the athletes the ability to pay attention to the phases of the game and the dynamics of the game, processed both in the preparation process and in the official matches.
Limitations in the study include the following: a relatively small number of subjects by sports and gender categories; a lack of an interventional program; the study included only subjects between the ages of 15 and 18, and athletes in other categories of greater or smaller age were not taken into account; athletes who practice outdoor team sports (e.g., football and rugby) were not included in the study; the influence and dynamics of environmental factors that could have modified the identification of focus of attention and qualities of attention were not targeted. Since the study is cross-sectional, the dynamics of the focus of attention over a certain period of time were not identified, which would have facilitated the identification of the variation in the quality of attention and the changes in the focus of attention. The application of questionnaires with evaluation scales only allows the identification of individual answers regarding the defined concepts without allowing descriptive arguments, which could highlight changes in concept and construct. Another limitation of the study is the fact that the socio-economic, educational, and geographical level of the subjects was not taken into account because, in Romania, the training of athletes registered at municipal clubs is free; school education is compulsory and free until the completion of high school studies (18–19 years).
Based on the findings of this study, future research could be directed towards identifying the dynamics of attentional focus depending on the competition stages. Another possibility would be to identify if the relevance of attention qualities changes depending on the performance objectives, as well as identify the qualities of attention relevant to the preparation process and apply some interventional strategies in order to better understand the characteristics of the sports context in order to obtain more effective individual results. Moreover, qualitative investigations provide us with details regarding the role of intra-personal variables, such as attentional focus and qualities of attention, in the context of team sports and environmental and social factors. It is an economical and reliable research method.

5. Conclusions

The results recorded in the present study reveal a variation in the focus of attention between groups of athletes and between genders. In the age category of 15–17 years for girls, an external dimension of attention was recorded in handball and volleyball and an internal one in the game of basketball. For boys, the dimension of attention was internal in handball and volleyball and external in basketball. Looking at the weight of the qualities of attention, it was found that the most relevant for girls are concentration and mobility for handball players; stability was identified in volleyball; and distributiveness in basketball. In boys handball teams, the most essential is mobility, just like in basketball, and in volleyball, it was found that stability has the biggest impact. The interdisciplinary approach to sports performance is a condition and a current trend of scientific research. The theoretical and practical implications found in the study will allow the optimization of the sports training methodology from the perspective of psychological training, with direct implications for physical, technical, and tactical training, in order to obtain relevant performance. Sports performance requires specialists and athletes to focus their training on all components of sports training, and the knowledge of psychological skills facilitates sports success.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The present study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved in 29 April 2023 by the Review Board of Physical Education and Sports, UMFST Targu Mures, Romania, no. 37/29.04.2023.

Informed Consent Statement

The participants voluntarily participated in this study and adhered to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are not publicly available for reasons of privacy.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

QASAquestionnaire for the assessment of focus of attention in athletes
QAQCSquestionnaire to identify the weights of attention qualities according to the characteristics of the practiced sport
HGhandball group girls
HBhandball group boys
VGvolleyball group girls
VBvolleyball group boys
BGbasketball group girls
BBbasketball group boys

References

  1. Rooney, D.; Jackson, R.C.; Heron, N. Differences in the attitudes to sport psychology consulting between individual and team sport athletes. BMC Sports Sci. Med. Rehabil. 2021, 13, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Shoxrux, S. Studying Sports Psychology. Am. J. Soc. Sci. Humanit. Res. 2013, 3, 176–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Evans, M.B.; Eys, M.A.; Bruner, M.W. Seeing the “we” in “me” sports: The need to consider individual sport team environments. Can. Psychol./Psychol. Can. 2012, 53, 301–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Miftari, F.; Selimi, M.; Badau, D.; Kelmendi, D. Identifying Differences Between Basketball, Handball and Volleyball Players in Increasing Explosive Force and Agility. Челoвек. Спoрт. Медицина 2021, 21, 144–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Jones, G.; Hanton, S.; Connaughton, D. A framework of mental toughness in the world’s best performers. Sport Psychol. 2007, 21, 243–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. van de Pol, P.K.C.; Kavussanu, M. Achievement motivation across training and competition in individual and team sports. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 2012, 1, 91–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Dosil, J. Psicologia de la Actividad Fiscia y del Deporte; McGraw-Hill: Madrid, Spain, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  8. Gu, Q.; Zou, L.; Loprinzi, P.D.; Quan, M.; Huang, T. Effects of Open Versus Closed Skill Exercise on Cognitive Function: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 1707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Joksimovic, M.; Goranovic, K.; Petkovic, J.; Badau, D.; Hantanu, C.G. Morphological Characteristics of Elite Female Volleyball Players Under 19. Int. J. Morphol. 2023, 41, 1203–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chiu, C.N.; Chen, C.Y.; Muggleton, N.G. Sport, time pressure, and cognitive performance. Prog. Brain Res. 2017, 234, 85–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Moradi, J.; Bahrami, A.; Dana, A. Motivation for Participation in Sports Based on Athletes in Team and Individual Sports. Phys. Cult. Sport Stud. Res. 2020, 85, 14–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Olteanu, M.; Oancea, B.M.; Badau, D. Improving Effectiveness of Basketball Free Throws through the Implementation of Technologies in the Technical Training Process. Appl. Sci. 2023, 13, 2650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Zhuravleva, T.; Aiken, C.A.; Becker, K.A.; Lin, P.C.; Sampson, J.J. The use of a holistic focus of attention to improve standing long jump performance among NCAA track and field athletes. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 2023, 18, 1074–1081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Rahimi, A.; Roberts, S.D.; Baker, J.R.; Wojtowicz, M. Attention and executive control in varsity athletes engaging in strategic and static sports. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0266933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Meng, F.W.; Yao, Z.F.; Chang, E.C.; Chen, Y.L. Team sport expertise shows superior stimulus-driven visual attention and motor inhibition. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0217056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Moreau, D.; Clerc, J.; Mansy-Dannay, A.; Guerrien, A. Enhancing spatial ability through sport practice: Evidence for an effect of motor training on mental rotation performance. J. Individ. Differ. 2012, 33, 83–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Nimmerichter, A.; Weber, N.J.R.; Wirth, K.; Haller, A. Effects of Video-Based Visual Training on Decision-Making and Reactive Agility in Adolescent Football Players. Sports 2015, 4, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Craciun, M. Psihologia Sportului Pentru Antrenori; RisoPrint: Cluj Napoca, Romania, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  19. Nechifor, C.; Sandovici, A. Modele explicative ale motivaţiei sportive din perspectiva performanţei. Psihol. Rev. Ştiinţifico-Pract. 2010, 4, 86–94. [Google Scholar]
  20. Voss, M.W.; Kramer, A.F.; Basak, C.; Prakash, R.S.; Roberts, B. Are expert athletes ‘expert’ in the cognitive laboratory? A meta-analytic review of cognition and sport expertise. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2010, 24, 812–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Gu, S.; Xue, L. Relationships among Sports Group Cohesion, Psychological Collectivism, Mental Toughness and Athlete Engagement in Chinese Team Sports Athletes. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 4987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Nica, S. Enhancement of Sport Performance with the Help of Psychoterapy. Stud. Univ. “Vasile Goldis” Ser. Educ. Fiz. si Kinetoterapie 2014, 3, 60–69. [Google Scholar]
  23. Boutcher, S.H. Attentional processes and sports performance. In Advances in Sports Psychology; Horn, T., Ed.; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  24. Kramer, A.F.; Erickson, K.I. Capitalizing on cortical plasticity: Influence of physical activity on cognition and brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 342–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Liu, C.; Mao, Z.; Zhang, T.; Liu, A.A.; Wang, B.; Zhang, T. Focus Your Attention: A Focal Attention for Multimodal Learning. IEEE Trans. Multimed. 2022, 24, 103–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Treinkman, M. Focus of Attention in Voice Training. J. Voice 2022, 36, 733.e1–733.e8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Hill, B.D.; Taylor, S.E.; Beach, J.D. Attention in sport. In APA Handbook of Sport and Exercise Psychology: Sport Psychology; Anshel, M.H., Petrie, T.A., Steinfeldt, J.A., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2009; pp. 259–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Nideffer, R.M.; Sagal, M.S. Concentration and attention control training. In Applied Sport Psychology: Personal Growth to Peak Performance, 3rd ed.; Williams, J.M., Ed.; Mayfield: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1998; pp. 296–315. [Google Scholar]
  29. Muntianu, V.A.; Abalașei, B.A.; Nichifor, F.; Dumitru, I.M. The Correlation between Psychological Characteristics and Psychomotor Abilities of Junior Handball Players. Children 2022, 9, 767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Zhuravleva, T.A.; Aiken, C.A.; Partridge, J.A. Reflections from expert throws coaches on the use of attentional focus cues during training. Braz. J. Mot. Behav. 2022, 16, 26–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Aditya, R.S.; Rahmatika, Q.T.; Solikhah, F.K.; Al Mutairi, R.I.; Alruwaili, A.S.; Astuti, E.S.; Fadila, R. Mental Toughness May Have an Impact on Athlete’s Performance: Systematic Review. Retos 2024, 56, 328–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Fairbrother, N.; Janssen, P.; Antony, M.M.; Tucker, E.; Young, A.H. Perinatal anxiety disorder prevalence and incidence. J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 200, 148–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Kumartasli, M.; Baştuğ, G. Examination of Attention Levels of Athletes Who do Taekwondo, Karate and Muaythai. Ovidius Univ. Ann. Ser. Phys. Educ. Sport/Sci. Mov. Health 2010, 2, 521–524. [Google Scholar]
  34. Harry, J.R.; Lanier, R.; Nunley, B.; Blinch, J. Focus of attention effects on lower extremity biomechanics during vertical jump landings. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2019, 68, 102521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lind, E.; Welch, A.S.; Ekkekakis, P. Do ‘mind over muscle’ strategies work? Examining the effects of attentional association and dissociation on exertional, affective and physiological responses to exercise. Sports Med. 2009, 39, 743–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Duncan, L.R.; Hall, C.R.; Wilson, P.M.; Jenny, O. Exercise motivation: A cross-sectional analysis examining its relationships with frequency, intensity, and duration of exercise. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2010, 7, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Emad, M.; Neumann, D.L.; Abel, L. Attentional focus strategies used by regular exercisers and their relationship with perceived exertion, enjoyment, and satisfaction. J. Hum. Sport Exerc. 2017, 12, 106–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Ziv, G.; Meckel, Y.; Lidor, R.; Rotstein, A. The effects of external and internal focus of attention on physiological responses during running. J. Hum. Sport Exerc. 2012, 7, 608–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Wulf, G.; Lewthwaite, R. Translating thoughts into action: Optimizing motor performance and learning through brief motivational and attentional influences. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2021, 30, 535–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Wulf, G.; Lewthwaite, R. Optimizing performance through intrinsic motivation and attention for learning: The OPTIMAL theory of motor learning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2016, 23, 1382–1414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Gottwald, V.; Davies, M.; Owen, R. Every story has two sides: Evaluating information processing and ecological dynamics perspectives of focus of attention in skill acquisition. Front. Sports Act. Living 2023, 5, 1176635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Gottwald, V.M.; Owen, R.O.; Lawrence, G.P. An internal focus of attention is optimal when congruent with afferent proprioceptive task information. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2020, 47, 101634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Aiken, C.A.; Becker, K.A. Utilising an internal focus of attention during preparation and an external focus during execution may facilitate motor learning. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2022, 23, 259–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Sakizlian, E.R.; Mihăilescu, L.N. Theoretical and methodological concepts that subsidize the scientific direction of sports training in handball. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 2017, 17, 2333–2338. Available online: https://www.efsupit.ro/images/stories/5November2017/Art%20255.pdf (accessed on 16 April 2024). [CrossRef]
  45. Singh, H.; Wulf, G. The distance effect and level of expertise: Is the optimal external focus different for low-skilled and high-skilled performers? Hum. Mov. Sci. 2020, 73, 102663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Tanasă, A.R.; Abalaşei, B.A.; Dumitru, I.M.; Popescu, L.; Ene-Voiculescu, C.; Ene-Voiculescu, V.; Moraru, C.E. Investigating the influence of personalized training on the optimisation of some psychomotor behaviours among junior gymnasts in the training process (Moldavia, Romania). Broad Res. Artif. Intell. Neurosci. 2024, 15, 459–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Moro, R.; Auday, M. Conscious focus of attention and sports skills. Analysis of the main lines of experimental research. Retos 2024, 51, 1364–1374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Fetean, G.D.; Monea, D.; Roșca, M. The role of attention in the football game for children and juniors. Discobolul-Phys. Educ. Sport Kinetotherapy J. 2021, 60, 197–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Ohuruogu, B.; Jonathan, U.I.; Ikechukwu, U.J. Psychological preparation for peak performance in sport competition. J. Educ. Pract. 2016, 7, 47–50. [Google Scholar]
  50. Marin, M.; Popa, C.; Sandu, A.; Miron, D.; Sakizlian, E.R.; Rosca, A.; Rusu, L. Comparative analysis of core muscle behavior on ingression and egression in driving position. Acta Tech. Napoc. Ser. Appl. Math. Mech. Eng. 2021, 64. Available online: https://atna-mam.utcluj.ro/index.php/Acta/article/view/1534 (accessed on 7 April 2024).
  51. Stavrev, S.; Ivanov, P. Comparison of qualities of attention between students from basketball and volleyball teams in the unwe -Sofia. Bulg. Knowl. Int. J. 2019, 35, 2143–2147. [Google Scholar]
  52. Bozhkova, A.; Dyakova, G.; Stavrev, S. Distinctiveness of some characteristics of attention and effective thinking, with students from both higherschools. Stud. UBB Educ. Artis Gymn 2013, LVIII, 41–46. [Google Scholar]
  53. Moneva, E.; Barova, I. Comparative analysis of some psychological indicators for students from the unwe table tennis and mass aerobics classes. Int. J. Sci. KNOWLEDGE-Pap. 2019, 30, 473–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hernández, M.C.; Prieto, Y.S.; García, J.D.; Rodríguez, M.S. Factors affecting concentration of attention in boxing athletes in combat situations. Rev. PODIUM 2020, 15, 5–21. [Google Scholar]
  55. Soler, P.Y.; Suárez Rodríguez, M. La atención en los deportes de combate: Punto de partida hacia el óptimo rendimiento. EFDeportes.com, Rev. Digit. Buenos Aires 2015, 9. Available online: https://www.efdeportes.com/efd202/la-atencion-en-los-deportes-de-combate.htm (accessed on 17 March 2024).
  56. Popa, M. Statistici Multivariate Aplicate in Psihologie; Polirom: Iasi, Romania, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  57. Moldovan, T. Satisfacția Studenților din Cadrul Universității de Arte din Târgu Mureș; UartPress: Târgu Mureș, Romania, 2022; Available online: https://uartpress.ro/press/index.php/uap/catalog/view/13/19/8 (accessed on 14 May 2024).
  58. Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 1992, 1, 98–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods. 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods. 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Biscaia, P.; Coelho, E.; Vicente, P.J.; Monteiro, D.; Mendo, A.H.; Alves, J. Which cognitive and perceptual skills best discriminate elite female handball players. Kinesiology 2021, 53, 104–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Blecharz, J.; Wrześniewski, K.; Siekańska, M.; Ambroży, T.; Spieszny, M. Cognitive Factors in Elite Handball: Do Players’ Positions Determine Their Cognitive Processes? J. Hum. Kinet. 2022, 82, 213–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Summers, J.J.; Miller, K.; Ford, S. Attentional Style and Basketball Performance. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 1991, 13, 239–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Oudejans, R.R.D.; Koedijker, J.M.; Bleijendaal, I.; Bakker, F.C. The education of attention in aiming at a far target: Training visual control in basketball jump shooting. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2005, 3, 197–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Moeinirad, S.; Abdoli, B.; Farsi, A.; Ahmadi, N. Training visual attention improves basketball three-point shot performance under pressure. Sport Sci. Health. 2022, 18, 853–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Sighinolfi, L. Sport Psychology in Basketball: Performance Under Pressure. In Basketball Sports Medicine and Science; Laver, L., Kocaoglu, B., Cole, B., Arundale, A.J.H., Bytomski, J., Amendola, A., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Fontani, G.; Lodi, L.; Felici, A.; Migliorini, S.; Corradeschi, F. Attention in Athletes of High and Low Experience Engaged in Different Open Skill Sports. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2006, 102, 791–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Yu, M.; Liu, Y.B.; Yang, G. Differences of attentional networks function in athletes from open-skill sports: An functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Neuroreport 2019, 30, 1239–1245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Hahn, S.; Kramer, A. Further evidence for the division of attention among non-contiguous locations. Vis. Cogn. 1998, 5, 217–256. [Google Scholar]
  70. Poolton, J.; Maxwell, J.; Masters, R.; Raab, M. Benefits of an external focus of attention: Common coding or conscious processing? J. Sports Sci. 2006, 24, 89–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Bahrami, A.; Moradi, J.; Rasouli, P. The Relationship between Cognitive Styles, Attention and Performance of Shooting Skill. Int. J. Motor Control Learn. 2020, 2, e146521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Kashani, V.; Nik Ravan, A.; Azari, M. Identifying Especial Skills for Air Gun Shooting in Skilled Male and Female Shooters. Ann. Appl. Sport Sci. 2016, 4, 59–67. Available online: http://aassjournal.com/article-1-317-en.html (accessed on 22 April 2024). [CrossRef]
  73. Bernier, M.; Codron, R.; Thienot, E.; Fournier, J.F. The attentional focus of expert golfers in training and competition: A naturalistic investigation. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 2011, 23, 326–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Burris, K.; Liu, S.; Appelbaum, L. Visual-motor expertise in athletes: Insights from semiparametric modelling of 2317 athletes tested on the Nike SPARQ Sensory Station. J. Sports Sci. 2020, 38, 320–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Yu, M.; Liu, Y. Differences in executive function of the attention network between athletes from interceptive and strategic sports. J. Mot. Behav. 2021, 53, 419–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Abedanzadeh, R.; Becker, K.; Mousavi, S.M.R. Both a holistic and external focus of attention enhance the learning of a badminton short serve. Psychol. Res. 2022, 86, 141–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Zhang, S.; Owen, R. Bridging attentional control and reinvestment: A test of the interactionist hypothesis in an E-sport context. New Ideas Psychol. 2023, 70, 101031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Wulf, G.; Wächter, S.; Wortmann, S. Attentional Focus in Motor Skill Learning: Do Females Benefit from an External Focus? Women Sport Phys. Act. J. 2003, 12, 37–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Peng, J.; Zhigang, G.; Tieming, F. Team ball sport experience minimizes sex difference in visual attention. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 987672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Reigal, R.E.; Enríquez-Molina, R.; Herrera-Robles, S.; Juárez-Ruiz de Mier, R.; Pastrana Brincones, J.L.; Hernández-Mendo, A.; Morales-Sánchez, V. Attentional Span is Determined by Sport Discipline. Sustainability 2022, 14, 2524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Predoiu, A.; Predoiu, R.; Grigore, V.; Mitrache, G. Analogical reasoning, mobility of attention and their link with psi in junior handball teams. Discobolul-Phys. Educ. Sport Kinetotherapy J. 2017, 50, 5–10. [Google Scholar]
  82. Hüttermann, S.; Ford, P.R.; Williams, A.M.; Varga, M.; Smeeton, N.J. Attention, Perception, and Action in a Simulated Decision-Making Task. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2019, 41, 230–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Power analysis—input and output parameters.
Figure 1. Power analysis—input and output parameters.
Brainsci 14 00623 g001
Figure 2. Variation of research power as a function of input parameters.
Figure 2. Variation of research power as a function of input parameters.
Brainsci 14 00623 g002
Figure 3. Research design.
Figure 3. Research design.
Brainsci 14 00623 g003
Table 1. Data on the distribution of subjects, age, and experience.
Table 1. Data on the distribution of subjects, age, and experience.
GroupsNumber of SubjectsAge—YearsThe Experience—Years
(X ± SD)Min. Max. (X ± SD)Min.Max.
Girls handball (HG)2816.64 ± 1.5115185.78 ± 2.0539
Boys handball (HB)3415.94 ± 0.6615185.41 ± 1.8339
Girls volleyball (VG)3016.67 ± 0.9815186.20 ± 1.2648
Boys volleyball (VB)2816.43 ± 0.9415184.92 ± 0.9958
Girls basketball (BG)2915.00 ± 0.7715185.08 ± 1.4647
Boys basketball (BB)2815.57 ± 0.7515186.57 ± 0.6458
X—mean; SD—standard deviation; Min.—minimum; Max.—maximum.
Table 2. Correlation matrix—questionnaire to identify the weights of attention qualities according to the characteristics of the practiced sport (QAQCS).
Table 2. Correlation matrix—questionnaire to identify the weights of attention qualities according to the characteristics of the practiced sport (QAQCS).
VolumeStabilityConcentrationDistributivityFlexibilityDistraction
Correlationvolume-0.3190.2340.2370.4680.175
stability0.319-0.2900.2580.5230.225
concentration0.2340.290-0.1960.2910.167
distributivity0.2370.2580.196-0.3060.270
mobility0.4680.5230.2910.306-0.420
distraction0.1750.2250.1670.2700.420-
pvolume-0.0000.0010.0010.0000.010
stability0.000-0.0000.0000.0000.001
concentration0.0010.000-0.0040.0000.013
distributivity0.0010.0000.004-0.0000.000
mobility0.0000.0000.0000.000-0.000
distraction0.0100.0010.0130.0000.000-
p—Sig. (One-tailed).
Table 3. KMO and Bartlett’s test.
Table 3. KMO and Bartlett’s test.
Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy0.753
Bartlett’s Test of SphericityApprox. Chi-Square185.709
df15
p0.000
df—degree of freedom and p—level of statistical probability.
Table 4. Communalities (extraction method: principal component analysis).
Table 4. Communalities (extraction method: principal component analysis).
ComponentInitialExtraction
volume1.0000.413
stability1.0000.497
concentration1.0000.282
distributivity1.0000.314
mobility1.0000.674
distraction1.0000.324
Table 5. Total variance explained (extraction method: principal component analysis).
Table 5. Total variance explained (extraction method: principal component analysis).
ComponentInitial EigenvaluesExtraction Sums of Squared Loadings
Total% of VarianceCumulative %Total% of VarianceCumulative %
volume2.50441.72741.7272.50441.72741.727
stability0.89214.86256.589
concentration0.79813.29569.884
distributivity0.75312.54982.433
mobility0.66611.10593.538
distraction0.3886.462100.000
Table 6. Component matrix a.
Table 6. Component matrix a.
ItemsComponent 1
volume0.642
stability0.705
concentration0.531
distributivity0.561
mobility0.821
distraction0.569
a Components extracted.
Table 7. The reliability of questionnaires on groups of subjects—Cronbach’s alpha.
Table 7. The reliability of questionnaires on groups of subjects—Cronbach’s alpha.
SportsGroupsQuestionnaire for the Assessment of Attentional Style in Athletes (α Cronbach’s)Questionnaire to Identify the Weights of Attention Qualities According to the Characteristics of the Practiced Sport (α Cronbach’s)
Whole questionnaireTotal sample (177 subjects)0.7870.709
Handball Girls0.7420.730
Boys0.7210.701
Volleyball Girls0.7300.717
Boys0.8400.805
BasketballGirls0.8550.798
Boys0.7330.739
Table 8. Descriptive statistics of the results of the questionnaire for the assessment of attentional style in athletes (QASA).
Table 8. Descriptive statistics of the results of the questionnaire for the assessment of attentional style in athletes (QASA).
GroupScaleItemiNMin.Max.MeanSDKurtosis
HGAi I1. I am able to “read” what is happening on the field and “feel” the mood of my teammates 282.003.002.9280.2621.183
I2. I am able to stop my thoughts while focusing on a certain event, game situation or discussion 282.004.003.2850.712−0.830
I6. When I listen to the coach’s instructions, I am not quickly distracted from my own thoughts or ideas 280.003.001.7850.956−1.070
AeI3. Even if I focus on what a certain player is doing, I manage to look at things as a whole 283.004.003.5000.509−2.160
I4. I manage to get an overview of the game situation from the information received from teammates and the coach 283.004.003.2850.460−1.076
I5. I know at any moment what the other players are doing on the field 282.004.003.4280.634−0.438
HBAiI1. I am able to “read” what is happening on the field and “feel” the mood of my teammates 342.004.003.2350.553−0.144
I2. I am able to stop my thoughts while focusing on a certain event, game situation or discussion 342.004.003.3520.773−0.921
I6. When I listen to the coach’s instructions, I am not quickly distracted from my own thoughts or ideas 340.000.002.5001.0510.276
AeI3. Even if I focus on what a certain player is doing, I manage to look at things as a whole 341.004.002.6470.848−0.667
I4. I manage to get an overview of the game situation from the information received from teammates and the coach 342.004.003.1760.796−1.328
I5. I know at any moment what the other players are doing on the field 340.004.002.6171.101−0.531
VGAiI1. I am able to “read” what is happening on the field and “feel” the mood of my teammates 302.003.002.8000.4060.527
I2. I am able to stop my thoughts while focusing on a certain event, game situation or discussion 302.004.002.8000.664−0.634
I6. When I listen to the coach’s instructions, I am not quickly distracted from my own thoughts or ideas 301.004.002.9330.7840.993
AeI3. Even if I focus on what a certain player is doing, I manage to look at things as a whole 302.004.003.1330.628−0.321
I4. I manage to get an overview of the game situation from the information received from teammates and the coach 302.004.003.2660.691−0.770
I5. I know at any moment what the other players are doing on the field 302.004.002.6000.621−0.534
VBAiI1. I am able to “read” what is happening on the field and “feel” the mood of my teammates 282.004.003.1420.5240.705
I2. I am able to stop my thoughts while focusing on a certain event, game situation or discussion 282.004.003.3570.621−0.554
I6. When I listen to the coach’s instructions, I am not quickly distracted from my own thoughts or ideas 282.004.003.2140.686−0.749
AeI3. Even if I focus on what a certain player is doing, I manage to look at things as a whole 282.004.003.0000.860−1.678
I4. I manage to get an overview of the game situation from the information received from teammates and the coach 283.004.003.3570.487−1.732
I5. I know at any moment what the other players are doing on the field 282.004.003.2140.568−0.062
BGAiI1. I am able to “read” what is happening on the field and “feel” the mood of my teammates 292.004.003.0000.845−1.615
I2. I am able to stop my thoughts while focusing on a certain event, game situation or discussion 292.004.003.2410.786−1.198
I6. When I listen to the coach’s instructions, I am not quickly distracted from my own thoughts or ideas 290.004.002.2750.996−0.003
AeI3. Even if I focus on what a certain player is doing, I manage to look at things as a whole 291.004.002.4820.737−0.089
I4. I manage to get an overview of the game situation from the information received from teammates and the coach 291.004.002.8621.059−0.958
I5. I know at any moment what the other players are doing on the field 291.004.002.3440.973−1.193
BBAiI1. I am able to “read” what is happening on the field and “feel” the mood of my teammates 282.004.003.1420.650−0.486
I2. I am able to stop my thoughts while focusing on a certain event, game situation or discussion 282.004.003.1420.848−1.566
I6. When I listen to the coach’s instructions, I am not quickly distracted from my own thoughts or ideas 281.004.002.8570.848−0.034
AeI3. Even if I focus on what a certain player is doing, I manage to look at things as a whole 281.004.003.0710.8990.132
I4. I manage to get an overview of the game situation from the information received from teammates and the coach 282.004.003.0710.813−1.463
I5. I know at any moment what the other players are doing on the field 281.004.003.0710.978−0.658
HG—handball group girls; HB—handball group boys; VG—volleyball group girls; VB—volleyball group boys; BG—basketball group girls; BB—basketball group boys; N—numar subiecti; Min.—minimim; Max.—maximum, SD—standard deviation, Ai—Internal attention; Ae—External attention.
Table 9. ANOVA (analysis of variance) between groups for the questionnaire for the assessment of attentional style in athletes (QASA).
Table 9. ANOVA (analysis of variance) between groups for the questionnaire for the assessment of attentional style in athletes (QASA).
GroupAttentional StyledfMSFp
GirlsInternal attention10.76325.3823.6190.031
External attention19.89529.4473.4490.000
Boys Internal attention12.64326.3213.2640.043
External attention22.149211.0743.9520.023
∑—sum of squares; df—degrees of freedom; MS—mean square; F—test value; p—probability level.
Table 10. Multiple comparisons LSD—at questionnaire for the assessment of attentional style in athletes (QASA).
Table 10. Multiple comparisons LSD—at questionnaire for the assessment of attentional style in athletes (QASA).
GenderAttentional StyleSportsSportsΔXSEp95% CI
Lower Upper
FemaleAiHandballVolleyball−0.533 *0.3200.021−1.1700.103
Basketball−0.862 *0.3230.009−1.504−0.219
VolleyballBasketball−0.328 *0.3170.034−0.9600.302
AeHandballVolleyball1.214 *0.4120.0040.3942.034
Basketball2.524 *0.4150.0001.6973.351
VolleyballBasketball1.310 *0.4080.0020.4972.123
MaleAiHandballVolleyball−0.890 *0.3550.014−1.597−0.184
Basketball−0.248 *0.3550.048−0.9530.458
VolleyballBaschet0.642 *0.3720.037−0.0961.382
AeHandballVolleyball−1.159 *0.4270.008−2.008−0.310
Basketball−0.802 *0.4270.044−1.6510.046
VolleyballBasketball0.357 *0.4470.027−0.5321.246
* The mean difference is significant at the 0.05 level. ΔX—the difference of the arithmetic means between groups; SE—standard error, p—statistical significance values, CI—interval of confidence; Ai—internal attention; Ae—external attention.
Table 11. Descriptive statistics of the results of the questionnaire to identify the weights of the qualities of attention according to the characteristics of the practiced sport.
Table 11. Descriptive statistics of the results of the questionnaire to identify the weights of the qualities of attention according to the characteristics of the practiced sport.
GroupItemNMin.Max.MeanSDKurtosis
HGI1—volume282.005.003.4280.835−0.149
I2—stability282.005.003.6420.911−0.877
I3—concentration283.005.004.0710.813−1.463
I4—distributivity283.005.003.7140.712−0.830
I5—mobility283.005.004.0710.899−1.804
I6—distraction281.004.002.5350.6920.096
HBI1—volume343.005.003.7350.709−0.862
I2—stability342.005.003.3820.779−0.004
I3—concentration343.005.003.7350.709−0.862
I4—distributivity343.005.003.7940.640−0.523
I5—mobility343.005.004.1470.821−1.457
I6—distraction342.005.003.2050.977−0.482
VGI1—volume302.005.003.7001.055−1.396
I2—stability302.005.003.8000.924−1.336
I3—concentration302.005.003.3660.850−0.334
I4—distributivity302.005.003.3661.033−1.050
I5—mobility302.005.003.5330.860−0.629
I6—distraction301.004.002.6000.770−0.152
VBI1—volume282.005.003.8571.078−1.632
I2—stability283.005.004.0710.940−1.933
I3—concentration282.005.003.5710.959−0.919
I4—distributivity282.005.003.5711.033−1.140
I5—mobility283.005.003.7500.927−1.677
I6—distraction281.005.002.8210.8620.420
BGI1—volume293.005.004.0000.925−1.905
I2—stability292.005.003.5860.907−0.833
I3—concentration292.005.003.5170.949−0.833
I4—distributivity293.005.004.0340.944−1.956
I5—mobility292.005.003.9650.944−1.334
I6—distraction291.003.002.4130.732−0.566
BBI1—volume302.005.003.5331.008−1.010
I2—stability302.005.003.9331.014−0.879
I3—concentration302.005.003.3330.844−0.200
I4—distributivity302.005.003.8001.030−0.948
I5—mobility302.005.004.2331.040−0.082
I6—distraction301.005.002.8661.0080.173
HG—handball group girls; HB—handball group boys; VG—volleyball group girls; VB—volleyball group boys; BG—basketball group girls; BB—basketball group boys; N—numar subiecti; Min.—minimim; Max.—maximum; SD—standard deviation.
Table 12. ANOVA (analysis of variance) between groups in the questionnaire to identify the weights of attention qualities according to the characteristics of the practiced sport.
Table 12. ANOVA (analysis of variance) between groups in the questionnaire to identify the weights of attention qualities according to the characteristics of the practiced sport.
GroupdfMSFp
Girls279.1212139.56012.4300.000
Boys157.982278.9916.9680.002
∑—sum of squares; df—degrees of freedom; MS—mean square; F—test value; p—probability level.
Table 13. Multiple comparisons LSD –at questionnaire to identify the weights of attention qualities according to the characteristics of the practiced sport.
Table 13. Multiple comparisons LSD –at questionnaire to identify the weights of attention qualities according to the characteristics of the practiced sport.
GenderTeam SportsΔXSEp95% CI
LowerUpper
FemaleHandballVolleyball4.361 *0.8800.0002.6106.113
Basketball1.821 *0.8950.0450.0403.602
VolleyballBasketball−2.540 *0.8800.005−4.291−0.789
MaleHandballVolleyball1.779 *0.8590.0410.0723.486
Basketball3.129 *0.8430.0001.4534.805
VolleyballBasketball1.350 *0.8840.031−0.4073.107
* The mean difference is significant at the 0.05 level. ΔX—the difference of the arithmetic means; SE—standard error; p—statistical significance values.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Badau, A. Identifying the Qualities of Attention and the Attentional Style in Indoor Team Sports: A Gender Comparison. Brain Sci. 2024, 14, 623. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070623

AMA Style

Badau A. Identifying the Qualities of Attention and the Attentional Style in Indoor Team Sports: A Gender Comparison. Brain Sciences. 2024; 14(7):623. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070623

Chicago/Turabian Style

Badau, Adela. 2024. "Identifying the Qualities of Attention and the Attentional Style in Indoor Team Sports: A Gender Comparison" Brain Sciences 14, no. 7: 623. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070623

APA Style

Badau, A. (2024). Identifying the Qualities of Attention and the Attentional Style in Indoor Team Sports: A Gender Comparison. Brain Sciences, 14(7), 623. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci14070623

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop