Virtual Enactment Effect on Memory in Young and Aged Populations: A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Method
2.1. Search Strategy
2.2. Selection Criteria
2.3. Quality Assessment and Data Abstraction
3. Results
3.1. What Populations Have Been Included?
3.2. What Virtual Apparatus Have Been Used?
3.3. What are the Navigation Conditions in the Included Studies?
3.4. How Has Memory Performance Been Measured?
3.5. Do “Virtual” Actions Have a “Real” Effect on Spatial and Episodic Memory?
3.6. What Are the Cognitive Factors Mediating Active Navigation and Memory Performance?
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Robin, J. Spatial scaffold effects in event memory and imagination. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2018, 9, e1462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moscovitch, M.; Rosenbaum, R.S.; Gilboa, A.; Addis, D.R.; Westmacott, R.; Grady, C.; McAndrews, M.P.; Levine, L.; Black, S.; Winocur, G.; et al. Functional neuroanatomy of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory: A unified account based on multiple trace theory. J. Anat. 2005, 207, 35–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- American Psychological Association. APA Dictionary of Psychology; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Burgess, N. Spatial cognition and the brain. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1124, 77–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vann, S.D.; Aggleton, J.P.; Maguire, E.A. What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 792–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chrastil, E.R.; Warren, W.H. Active and passive contributions to spatial learning. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2012, 19, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tulving, E. Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2002, 53, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Jebara, N.; Orriols, E.; Zaoui, M.; Berthoz, A.; Piolino, P. Effects of enactment in episodic memory: A pilot virtual reality study with young and elderly adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2014, 6, 338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Laurent, X.; Ensslin, A.; Marí-Beffa, P. An Action to an Object Does Not Improve Its Episodic Encoding but Removes Distraction. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2016, 42, 494–507. [Google Scholar]
- Plancher, G.; Gyselinck, V.; Nicolas, S.; Piolino, P. Age effect on components of episodic memory and feature binding: A virtual reality study. Neuropsychology 2010, 24, 379–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piolino, P.; Desgranges, B.; Clarys, D.; Guillery-Girard, B.; Taconnat, L.; Isingrini, M.; Eustache, F. Autobiographical memory, autonoetic consciousness, and self-perspective in aging. Psychol. Aging 2006, 21, 510–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Prebble, S.C.; Addis, D.R.; Tippett, L.J. Autobiographical memory and sense of self. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 815–840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Keefe, L.; Nadel, J. The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map; Clarendon Press: Oxford, UK, 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Hartley, T.; Lever, C.; Burgess, N.; O’Keefe, J. Space in the brain: How the hippocampal formation supports spatial cognition. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2014, 369, 20120510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Burgess, N.; Maguire, E.A.; O’Keefe, J. The human hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron 2002, 35, 625–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kühn, S.; Gallinat, J. Segregating cognitive functions within hippocampal formation: A quantitative meta-analysis on spatial navigation and episodic memory. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014, 35, 1129–1142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Manus, J.R.; Hopkins, R.O.; Reed, J.M.; Kitchener, E.G.; Squire, L.R. Recognition memory and the human hippocampus. Neuron 2003, 37, 171–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bergouignan, L.; Nyberg, L.; Ehrsson, H.H. Out-of-body–induced hippocampal amnesia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2014, 111, 4421–4426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eichenbaum, H. Time cells in the hippocampus: A new dimension for mapping memories. Nat. Rev. Neruosci. 2014, 15, 732–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glenberg, A.M.; Hayes, J. Contribution of embodiment to solving the riddle of infantile amnesia. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ritchey, M.; Wing, E.A.; Labar, K.S.; Cabeza, R. Neural Similarity Between Encoding and Retrieval is Related to Memory Via Hippocampal Interactions. Cereb. Cortex 2012, 23, 2818–2828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nadel, L.; Winocur, G.; Ryan, L.; Moscovitch, M. Systems consolidation and hippocampus: Two views. Debates Neurosci. 2007, 1, 55–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gomez, A.; Rousset, S.; Baciu, M. Egocentric-updating during navigation facilitates episodic memory retrieval. Acta Psychol. 2009, 132, 221–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burgess, N. The hippocampus, space, and view points in episodic memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2002, 55A, 1057–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gomez, A.; Rousset, S.; Charnallet, A. Spatial deficits in an amnesic patient with hippocampal damage: Questioning the multiple trace theory. Hippocampus 2012, 22, 1313–1324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tromp, D.; Dufour, A.; Lithfous, S.; Pebayle, T.; Després, O. Episodic memory in normal aging and Alzheimer disease: Insights from imaging and behavioral studies. Ageing Res. Rev. 2015, 24, 232–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wheeler, M.A.; Stuss, D.T.; Tulving, E. Toward a theory of episodic memory: The frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychol. Bull. 1997, 121, 331–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Habib, R.; Nyberg, L.; Tulving, E. Hemispheric asymmetries of memory: The HERA model revisited. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003, 7, 241–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lepage, M.; Ghaffar, O.; Nyberg, L.; Tulving, E. Prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval mode. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2000, 97, 506–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burgess, N.; Maguire, E.A.; Spiers, H.J.; O’Keefe, J. A temporoparietal and prefrontal network for retrieving the spatial context of lifelike events. Neuroimage 2001, 14, 439–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spreng, R.N.; Mar, R.A.; Kim, A.S.N. The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008, 21, 489–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, A. An embodied cognitive science? Trends Cogn. Sci. 1999, 3, 345–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, M. Six views of embodied cognition. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2002, 9, 625–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Glenberg, A.M. Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2010, 1, 586–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dijkstra, K.; Post, L. Mechanisms of embodiment. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vallet, G.T. Embodied cognition of aging. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spackman, J.S.; Yanchar, S.C. Embodied Cognition, Representationalism, and Mechanism: A Review and Analysis. J. Theory Soc. Behav. 2014, 44, 46–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Braak, H.; Braak, E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991, 82, 239–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fjell, A.M.; Westlye, L.T.; Amlien, I.; Espeseth, T.; Reinvang, I.; Raz, N.; Agartz, I.; Salat, D.H.; Greve, D.N.; Fischl, B.; et al. High consistency of regional cortical thinning in aging across multiple samples. Cereb. Cortex 2009, 19, 2001–2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Braak, H.; Braak, E. Staging of alzheimer’s disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Neurobiol. Aging 1995, 16, 271–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brooks, B.M.; Attree, E.A.; Rose, F.D.; Clifford, B.R.; Leadbetter, A.G. The Specificity of Memory Enhancement During Interaction with a Virtual Environment. Memory 1999, 7, 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engelkamp, J. Memory for Actions; Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis: London, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Plancher, G.; Barra, J.; Orriols, E.; Piolino, P. The influence of action on episodic memory: A virtual reality study. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2013, 66, 895–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tulving, E. Ecphoric process in episodic memory. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 1983, 1110, 361–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rugg, M.D.; Wilding, E.L. Retrieval processing and episodic memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 108–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kent, C.; Lamberts, K. The encoding-retrieval relationship: Retrieval as mental simulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 92–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meyer, K.; Damasio, A. Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends Neurosci. 2009, 32, 376–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, J.D.; Price, M.H.; Leiker, E.K. Episodic retrieval involves early and sustained effects of reactivating information from encoding. Neuroimage 2015, 106, 300–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rugg, M.D.; Johnson, J.D.; Park, H.; Uncapher, M.R. Encoding-retrieval overlap in human episodic memory: A functional neuroimaging perspective. Prog. Brain Res. 2008, 169, 339–352. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Danker, J.F.; Anderson, J.R. The ghosts of brain states past: Remembering reactivates the brain regions engaged during encoding. Psychol. Bull. 2010, 136, 87–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nyberg, L.; Habib, R.; McIntosh, A.R.; Tulving, E. Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during memory retrieval. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2000, 97, 11120–11124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wheeler, M.; Petersen, S.; Buckner, R. Memory’s Echo: Vivid Remembering Reactivates Sensory-Specific Cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2000, 97, 11125–11129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, P.N.; Foreman, N.; Gillett, R.; Stanton, D. Active Versus Passive Processing of Spatial Information in a Computer- Simulated Environment. Ecol. Psychol. 1997, 9, 37–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Riva, G. From virtual to real body: Virtual reality as embodied technology. J. Cyber Ther. Rehabil. 2008, 1, 7–22. [Google Scholar]
- Repetto, C.; Serino, S.; Macedonia, M.; Riva, G. Virtual Reality as an Embodied Tool to Enhance Episodic Memory in Elderly. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G.; Grp, P. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement (Reprinted from Annals of Internal Medicine). Phys. Ther. 2009, 89, 873–880. [Google Scholar]
- Tuena, C.; Serino, S.; Gaston-Bellegarde, A.; Orriols, E.; Makowski, D.; Riva, G.; Piolino, P. How virtual embodiment affects episodic memory functioning: A proof-of-concept study. Annu. Rev. Cyberther. Telemed. 2017, 15, 98–103. [Google Scholar]
- Carassa, A.; Geminiani, G.; Morganti, F.; Varotto, D. Active and passive spatial learning in a complex virtual environment: The effect of effcient exploration. Cogn. Process. 2002, 3, 65–81. [Google Scholar]
- Sandamas, G.; Foreman, N. Drawing maps and remembering landmarks after driving in a virtual small town environment. J. Maps 2007, 3, 35–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wallet, G.; Sauzéon, H.; Larrue, F.; N’Kaoua, B. Virtual/real transfer in a large-scale environment: Impact of active navigation as a function of the viewpoint displacement effect and recall tasks. Adv. Hum. Comput. Interact. 2013, 2013, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meade, M.E.; Meade, J.G.; Sauzéon, H.; Fernandes, M.A. Active Navigation in Virtual Environments Benefits Spatial Memory in Older Adults. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gaunet, F.; Vidal, M.; Kemeny, A.; Berthoz, A. Active, passive and snapshot exploration in a virtual environment: Influence on scene memory, reorientation and path memory. Cogn. Brain Res. 2001, 11, 409–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taillade, M.; Sauzéon, H.; Pala, P.A.; Déjos, M.; Larrue, F.; Gross, C.; N’Kaoua, B. Age-Related Wayfinding Differences in Real Large-Scale Environments: Detrimental Motor Control Effects during Spatial Learning Are Mediated by Executive Decline? PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e67193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalgarno, B.; Bennett, S.; Harper, B. The Importance of Active Exploration, Optical Flow, and Task Alignment for Spatial Learning in Desktop 3D Environments. Hum. Comput. Interact. 2010, 25, 25–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- von Stülpnagel, R.; Steffens, M.C. Active route learning in virtual environments: Disentangling movement control from intention, instruction specificity, and navigation control. Psychol. Res. 2013, 77, 555–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, P.N. Active exploration of a virtual environment does not promote orientation or memory for objects. Environ. Behav. 1999, 31, 752–763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peruch, P.; Vercher, J.; Gauthier, G.M. Acquisition of Spatial Knowledge Through Visual Exploration of Simulated Environments. Ecol. Psychol. 1995, 7, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sauzéon, H.; N’Kaoua, B.; Arvind Pala, P.; Taillade, M.; Guitton, P. Age and active navigation effects on episodic memory: A virtual reality study. Br. J. Psychol. 2016, 107, 72–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sauzéon, H.; Pala, P.A.; Larrue, F.; Wallet, G.; Déjos, M.; Zheng, X.; Guitton, P.; N’Kaoua, B. The use of Virtual Reality for episodic memory assessment: Effects of active navigation. Exp. Psychol. 2012, 59, 99–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plancher, G.; Tirard, A.; Gyselinck, V.; Nicolas, S.; Piolino, P. Using virtual reality to characterize episodic memory profiles in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: Influence of active and passive encoding. Neuropsychologia 2012, 50, 592–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plancher, G.; Nicolas, S.; Piolino, P. Apport de la réalité virtuelle en neuropsychologie de la mémoire: Étude dans le vieillissement. Psychol. Neuropsychiatr. Vieil. 2008, 6, 7–22. [Google Scholar]
- Wallet, G.; Sauzéon, H.; Rodrigues, J.; Larrue, F.; N’Kaoua, B. Virtual/real transfer of spatial learning: Impact of activity according to the retention delay. Stud. Health Technol. Inform. 2010, 154, 145–149. [Google Scholar]
- Hahm, J.; Lee, K.; Lim, S.-L.; Kim, S.-Y.; Kim, H.-T.; Lee, J.-H. Effects of active navigation on object recognition in virtual environments. CyberPsychol. Behav. 2007, 10, 305–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cutmore, T.R.H.; Hine, T.J.; Maberly, K.J.; Langford, N.M.; Hawgood, G. Cognitive and gender factors influencing navigation in a virtual environment. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2000, 53, 223–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sandamas, G.; Foreman, N. Active Versus Passive Acquisition of Spatial Knowledge While Controlling a Vehicle in a Virtual Urban Space in Drivers and Non-Drivers. SAGE Open 2015, 5, 2158244015595443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farrell, M.J.; Arnold, P.; Pettifer, S.; Adams, J.; Graham, T.; MacManamon, M. Transfer of route learning from virtual to real environments. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2003, 9, 219–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wallet, G.; Sauzéon, H.; Pala, P.A.; Larrue, F.; Zheng, X.; N’Kaoua, B. Virtual/real transfer of spatial knowledge: Benefit from visual fidelity provided in a virtual environment and impact of active navigation. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2011, 14, 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allison, C.; Redhead, E.S. Factors in fl uencing orientation within a nested virtual environment: External cues, active exploration and familiarity. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017, 51, 158–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palermo, E.; Laut, J.; Nov, O.; Cappa, P.; Porfiri, M. Spatial memory training in a citizen science context. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 73, 38–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, I.; Levy, R.M.; Barton, J.J.S.; Iaria, G. Age and gender differences in various topographical orientation strategies. Brain Res. 2011, 1410, 112–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pacheco, D.; Verschure, P.F.M.J. Long-term spatial clustering in free recall. Memory 2018, 26, 798–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pettijohn, K.A.; Radvansky, G.A. Walking through doorways causes forgetting: Active and passive interaction. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2018, 30, 771–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barry, C.; Burgess, N. Neural Mechanisms of Self-Location. Curr. Biol. 2014, 24, R330–R339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chrastil, E.R.; Warren, W.H. Active and Passive Spatial Learning in Human Navigation: Acquisition of Graph Knowledg. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2015, 41, 1162–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greenwood, P.M. The frontal aging hypothesis evaluated. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2000, 6, 705–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Charness, G.; Gneezy, U.; Kuhn, M.A. Experimental methods: Between-subject and within-subject design. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2012, 81, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delis, D.C.; Kramer, J.H.; Kaplan, E.; Thompkins, B.A.O. CVLT: California Verbal Learning Test-Adult Version: Manual; Psychological Corporation: London, UK, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Madan, C.R.; Singhal, A. Using actions to enhance memory: Effects of enactment, gestures, and exercise on human memory. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dijkstra, K.; Kaschak, M.P.; Zwaan, R.A. Body posture facilitates retrieval of autobiographical memories. Cognition 2007, 102, 139–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guterstam, A.; Bjornsdotter, M.; Bergouignan, L.; Gentile, G.; Li, T.-Q.; Ehrsson, H.H. Decoding illusory self-location from activity in the human hippocampus. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steffens, M.C.; Von Stülpnagel, R.; Schult, J.C. Memory Recall After ‘Learning by Doing’ and ‘Learning by Viewing’: Boundary Conditions of an Enactment Benefit. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Potter, M.C. Recognition and memory for briefly presented scenes. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Migo, E.M.; Mayes, A.R.; Montaldi, D. Measuring recollection and familiarity: Improving the remember/know procedure. Conscious. Cogn. 2012, 21, 1435–1455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nyberg, L.; Persson, J.; Nilsson, L.G. Individual differences in memory enhancement by encoding enactment: Relationships to adult age and biological factors. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2002, 26, 835–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmer, H.D.; Cohen, R.L. Memory for Action: A Distinct Form of Episodic Memory? Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. WAIS-R Manual: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised; Psychological Corporation: London, UK, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Stroop, J.R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandenberg, S.G.; Kuse, A.R. Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Percept. Mot. Skills 1978, 47, 599–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reitan, R.M. Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 1958, 8, 271–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- García-Betances, R.I.; Arredondo Waldmeyer, M.T.; Fico, G.; Cabrera-Umpiérrez, M.F. A succinct overview of virtual reality technology use in Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2015, 7, 80. [Google Scholar]
- Serino, S.; Pedroli, E.; Tuena, C.; Leo, G.D.; Stramba-Badiale, M.; Goulene, K.; Mariotti, N.G.; Riva, G. A Novel Virtual Reality-Based Training Protocol for the Enhancement of the ‘Mental Frame Syncing’ in Individuals with Alzheimer’ s Disease: A Development-of-Concept Trial. Serino, Silvia et al. “A Novel Virtual Reality-Based Training Protocol for the Enhancement of the “Mental Frame Syncing” in Individuals with Alzheimer’s Disease: A Development-of-Concept Trial.”. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2017, 9, 240. [Google Scholar]
- Serino, S.; Riva, G. Getting lost in Alzheimer’s disease: A break in the mental frame syncing. Med. Hypotheses 2013, 80, 416–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Triberti, S.; Riva, G. Being present in action: A theoretical model about the ‘interlocking’ between intentions and environmental affordances. Front. Psychol. 2016, 6, 2052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Münzer, S.; Zimmer, H.D.; Schwalm, M.; Baus, J.; Aslan, I. Computer-assisted navigation and the acquisition of route and survey knowledge. J. Environ. Psychol. 2006, 26, 300–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Doeller, C.F.; King, J.A.; Burgess, N. Parallel striatal and hippocampal systems for landmarks and boundaries in spatial memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 5915–5920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kontaxopoulou, D.; Beratis, I.N.; Fragkiadaki, S.; Pavlou, D.; Yannis, G.; Economou, A.; Papanicolaou, A.C.; Papageorgiou, S.G. Incidental and Intentional Memory: Their Relation with Attention and Executive Functions. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2017, 32, 19–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guderian, S.; Dzieciol, A.M.; Gadian, D.G.; Jentschke, S.; Doeller, C.F.; Burgess, N.; Mishkin, M.; Vargha-Khadem, F. Hippocampal Volume Reduction in Humans Predicts Impaired Allocentric Spatial Memory in Virtual-Reality Navigation. J. Neurosci. 2015, 35, 14123–14131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Serino, S.; Repetto, C. New Trends in Episodic Memory Assessment: Immersive 360 Ecological Videos. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salthouse, T.A. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol. Aging 2009, 30, 507–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hedden, T.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Insights into the ageing mind: A view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 5, 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kinugawa, K.; Schumm, S.; Pollina, M.; Depre, M.; Jungbluth, C.; Doulazmi, M.; Mariani, J. Aging-related episodic memory decline: Are emotions the key? Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robitsek, R.J.; Fortin, N.J.; Koh, M.T.; Gallagher, M.; Eichenbaum, H. Cognitive Aging: A Common Decline of Episodic Recollection and Spatial Memory in Rats. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 8945–8954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolbers, T.; Dudchenko, P.A.; Wood, E.R. Spatial memory—A unique window into healthy and pathological aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2014, 6, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colombo, D.; Serino, S.; Tuena, C.; Pedroli, E.; Dakanalis, A.; Cipresso, P.; Riva, G. Egocentric and allocentric spatial reference frames in aging: A systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 80, 605–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alexander, A.S.; Nitz, D.A. Retrosplenial cortex maps the conjunction of internal and external spaces. Nat. Neurosci. 2015, 18, 1143–1151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dubois, B.; Feldman, H.H.; Jacova, C.; DeKosky, S.T.; Barberger-Gateau, P.; Cummings, J.; Delacourte, A.; Galasko, D.; Gauthier, S.; Jicha, G.; et al. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Revising the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2007, 6, 734–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nordahl, C.W.; Ranganath, C.; Yonelinas, A.P.; DeCarli, C.; Reed, B.R.; Jagust, W.J. Different mechanisms of episodic memory failure in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 2005, 43, 1688–1697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dunn, C.J.; Duffy, S.L.; Hickie, I.B.; Lagopoulos, J.; Lewis, S.J.; Naismith, S.L.; Shine, J.M. Deficits in episodic memory retrieval reveal impaired default mode network connectivity in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. NeuroImage Clin. 2014, 4, 473–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Petersen, R.C.; Doody, R.; Kurz, A.; Mohs, R.C.; Morris, J.C.; Rabins, P.V.; Ritchie, K.; Rossor, M.; Thal, L.; Winblad, B. Current Concepts in Mild Cognitive Impairment. Arch. Neurol. 2011, 58, 1985–1992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hafkemeijer, A.; van der Grond, J.; Rombouts, S.A.R.B. Imaging the default mode network in aging and dementia. Biochim. Biophys. Acta Mol. Basis Dis. 2012, 1822, 431–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buckner, R.L.; Snyder, A.Z.; Shannon, B.J.; LaRossa, G.; Sachs, R.; Fotenos, A.F.; Sheline, Y.I.; Klunk, W.E.; Mathis, C.A.; Morris, J.C.; et al. Molecular, Structural, and Functional Characterization of Alzheimer’s Disease: Evidence for a Relationship between Default Activity, Amyloid, and Memory. J. Neurosci. 2005, 25, 7709–7717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Buckner, R.L.; Andrews-Hanna, J.R.; Schacter, D.L. The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1124, 1–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Serino, S.; Morganti, F.; Di Stefano, F.; Riva, G. Detecting early egocentric and allocentric impairments deficits in Alzheimer’s disease: An experimental study with virtual reality. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2015, 7, 88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruggiero, G.; Iavarone, A.; Iachini, T. Allocentric to egocentric spatial switching: Impairment in amci and Alzheimer’s disease patients? Curr. Alzheimer Res. 2018, 15, 229–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piolino, P.; Desgranges, B.; Belliard, S.; Matuszewski, V.; Lalevée, C.; De La Sayette, V.; Eustache, F. Autobiographical memory and autonoetic consciousness: Triple dissociation in neurodegenerative diseases. Brain 2003, 126, 2203–2219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rauchs, G.; Piolino, P.; Mézenge, F.; Landeau, B.; Lalevée, C.; Pélerin, A.; Viader, F.; De La Sayette, V.; Eustache, F.; Desgranges, B. Autonoetic consciousness in Alzheimer’s disease: Neuropsychological and PET findings using an episodic learning and recognition task. Neurobiol. Aging 2007, 28, 1410–1420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Ref. | Sample (s) | Conditions | Design (Navigation) | Virtual Apparatus | Memory Assessment | Primary Outcomes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[62] | 48 YA (age range: 21–38; 24 males) | Effect of active (no decisional level) vs. passive (prerecorded travel) vs. snapshot exploration (static condition) on scene recognition and memory of displacements. Intentional encoding. | Within | Non-immersive (CaTS driving simulator); input device: joystick. | Scene recognition (route snapshots); Pointing toward the origin test using the joystick; drawing test (shape of the path). | Path shape task benefitted from active condition, whereas recognition and pointing task were not affected by the exploring conditions. |
[41] Exp. 1 | 30 YA (mean age = 27.1; 14 males) | Effect of active vs. passive (recorded navigation) navigation on spatial memory. No intentional encoding. Participants could freely navigate the apartment. | Between (yoked) | Non-immersive; input device: joystick; house apartment navigation. | Spatial layout test (spatial layout drawing of the VE); recall test (location and objects name on VE map). | Active group showed better spatial layout scores. No effect on recall test. |
[41] Exp. 2 | 40 YA (mean age = 26; 18 males) | Effect of active vs. passive (recorded navigation) navigation on spatial memory. No intentional encoding. Participants could freely navigate the apartment. | // | Non-immersive; input device: joystick; house apartment navigation. | Spatial layout test (spatial layout drawing of the VE); recognition task (objects); object location test. | Spatial layout recall replicated for active condition. No effect on other tasks. |
[63] | 30 YA (age range = 18–30) and 30 OA (age range = 58–72) | Role of active vs. passive (pre-recorded video) motor exploration in spatial memory and wayfinding. Intentional encoding. No decision-making. | Between | Non-immersive; input device: joystick; virtual replica of Bordeaux. | Wayfinding task (replication of the path; use of spatial representation, errors and stops were calculated); spatial memory task (map drawing + picture classification; route and survey representations). | Active condition worsened survey knowledge (spatial map) in both groups, led to better wayfinding scores in YA and worsened in OA. Executive functions have a crucial role during active navigation. |
[64] Exp. 1 | 22 undergraduates (14 males) | Exploring the role of motion control vs. passive condition (VE tour) on spatial learning. Intentional encoding (learning phase before test phase). Navigation instructions were given. | Between | Non-immersive; input device: keyboard; VE of a research lab. | Spatial learning test (indicate position and direction of egocentric pictures on a lab map; object location test). | View positioning test was better for active participants, no significant difference between the conditions was observed for object location task. Active navigation contributes partially to survey representation. |
[64] Exp. 2 | 80 undergraduates (49 males) | Exploring the role of optical flow (action with object in active condition) vs. passive navigation vs. static condition on spatial learning between active. Intentional encoding (learning phase before test phase). Navigation instructions were given. | // | Non-immersive; input device: keyboard; VE of a research lab. | Spatial learning test (indicate position and direction of egocentric pictures on a lab map; object location test). | Active participants performed better in object locations task and passive condition performed better than static condition. No difference among the conditions was observed for the first task. |
[65] Exp. 1 | 82 university students (age range: 19–33; 43 males) | Effect of intentional vs. incidental encoding vs. active vs. passive navigation (observing the participant navigating) on spatial memory. Auditory route instructions were given (no decision-making). | Between (yoked) | Non-immersive; input devices: keyboard and mouse; virtual city. | Spatial memory test (landmark recognition task, pointing task and path-sketching, route navigation task; respectively, landmark, survey and route knowledge). | Active navigation led to better landmark and route knowledge performances. No effect on survey knowledge. No effect of encoding. |
[65] Exp. 2 | 88 university students (age range: 18–33; 10 males) | Effect of movement (active navigation vs. passive) vs. instruction control (instructing vs. listening) vs. instruction specificity (landmark information vs. layout information) on spatial memory. Navigation instructions were written (no decision-making). | // | Non-immersive; input devices: keyboard and mouse; apartment with rooms. | Spatial memory test (landmark recognition task, tour integration task, route navigation task). | Landmark knowledge, tour integration and route knowledge benefited from self-contained condition. Effect on performance was mediate by instruction specificity and control in the latest task. |
[65] Exp. 3 | 102 students (age range: 19–41; 21 males) | Effect of active vs. passive navigation vs. decision-making (map) vs. less decision-making (map with suggested path) vs. no-map condition on spatial memory. Participants were asked to find the shortest possible route. | // | Non-immersive; input devices: keyboard and mouse; apartment with rooms. | Spatial memory test (landmark recognition task and tour integration task; route navigation task). | Active navigation led to better landmark recognition performance. Decision-making helped participants in observed movement condition and less decision-making worsened route knowledge. |
[66] | 24 undergraduates (age range: 18–21; 7 males) | Effect of active free navigation (with decision-making) vs. passive on object-memory. Intentional encoding. | Between (yoked) | Non-immersive; input device: keyboard; virtual city. | Object task (locate objects) and recognition task. | No difference between the two conditions in the tasks. |
[53] Exp. 1 | 72 undergraduates (age range: 18–27; 22 males) | Effect of psychological activity (decision-making vs. no decision-making on directions) and physical activity (motor control vs. no motor control on keyboard) vs. control group on spatial performance. No intentional encoding (explore VE). | Between | Non-immersive; input device: keyboard; virtual city. | Orientation task (direction test + map drawing). | No difference was observed between the conditions manipulated. |
[53] Exp. 2 | 36 undergraduates (age range: 18–42) | Effect of active exploration vs. passive observation of navigation vs. control (no exploration of VE) on wayfinding. No intentional encoding (explore VE). | Between | Non-immersive; input device: keyboard; virtual arena. | Wayfinding task. | No difference was observed between the conditions manipulated. |
[67] | 18 YA (age range: 20–39; 9 males) | Effect of active (with decision-making) vs. passive dynamic (recorded video) vs. passive static (slide-like scenes) free exploration on spatial layout performances. Intentional encoding. | Within | Non-immersive; input device: joystick; virtual arena with cubes. | Target location test (8 trials; score, time, orientation and verbal or drawing description of the strategies used to reach a given target were calculated). | Active participants performed better than the two passive conditions in the task (scores, time, verbal and layout descriptions but no orientation task). Active motor behaviour with active perception is crucial to extract invariants in the VE. |
[68] | 30 YA (age range = 18–25)and 30 OA (age range = 60–81) | Active (with decision-making) vs. passive (computer-guided tour) free navigation effects on memory for everyday objects. Intentional encoding. | Between | Non-immersive; input devices: keyboard and mouse; VR-based Human Objects Memories from Everyday Scenes (HOMES). | Free recall and recognition (learning, proactive interference, semantic clustering, recognition hits, and false recognitions). VE 1A followed by free recall task + VE 2A followed by free recall task + VE B followed by recognition task. | Active navigation had a beneficial effect on recognition hits only, in both YA and OA compared with passive mode. Active mode reduced false recognitions in YA but increased these in OA. Active navigation enhanced memory in older adults when to not demanding. |
[69] | 44 students (mean age = 21.94, SD = 2.13; 21 males) | Active (with decision-making) vs. passive (computer-guided tour) free navigation effects on memory for everyday objects. Intentional encoding. | Between | Non-immersive; input devices: keyboard and mouse; VR-based HOMES. | Free recall and recognition (learning, proactive interference, semantic clustering, recognition hits, and false recognitions). VE 1A followed by free recall task + VE 2A followed by free recall task + VE B followed by recognition task. | Active navigation led to better recognition hits performances compared to passive condition. Active participants had less source-based false recognition compared with passive participants. Active navigation was useful to enrich visuomotor details of episodic memory traces but had no effect on semantic relational processing. |
[9] Exp. 3 | 41 participants (age range: 18–34; 12 males) | Active vs. passive (recorded actions; no motor response) selection affects memory for object. Intentional encoding. Instructions. | Between | Non-immersive; input device: keyboard; WWW (what–where–when) variation built with Second Life arena. | Object name cued recall (full episodic recall: what + where + when; non-episodic: where + what or when + what or what only). | Active condition reduced distractor encoding compared to the passive viewing of the action of the avatar. |
[43] | 72 Psychology students (mean age = 22.23, SD = 3.94; 36 males) | Interaction condition (motor trace in memory, no decision on itinerary) vs. planning condition (no control of the vehicle; decisional level) vs. passive (recorded video). Intentional encoding. | Between | Non-immersive; input devices: steering wheel and pedals; virtual city. | Free recall of elements; visuospatial memory test (draw map + locate elements); visuospatial cued recall (locate elements on a prepared map); recognition test (elements, locations and navigation directions after seeing the elements). | Interaction enhanced memory recall, in particular spatial memory test (no effect on influence on visuospatial cued recall or recognition); however, interaction worsened elements recognition compared with passive condition; planning condition boosted visuospatial recalls. Both interaction and planning had an effect on episodic memory. |
[70] | 21 healthy OA (4 males), 15 aMCI (7 males) and 15 AD (2 males) | Active vs. passive (recorded video) encoding influences episodic memory. Intentional encoding. Predetermined route. | Between | Non-immersive; input devices: steering wheel and pedals; two virtual cities. | Immediate free recall (what, details, when, egocentric where, allocentric where, binding); recognition (elements, spatial and temporal relations between elements; remember/know paradigm); delayed free recall (same as immediate free recall). | Active exploration led in OA, aMCI, and AD groups to better recall of elements, allocentric spatial information and binding. Procedural skills and self-involvement may be crucial for episodic performances in aMCI and AD patients. |
[71] | 113 psychology students (mean age = 21.57, SD = 2.99) and 45 OA | Effect of active vs. passive navigation and intentional vs. incidental encoding on episodic memory. Predetermined route. | Between | Non-immersive; input devices: steering wheel and pedals; virtual city. | Free recall (what, verbal where, visuospatial where, when, details); recognition test (elements). | Encoding conditions affect differently episodic features in YA and OA. However, any effect due to sensorimotor implication emerged in the study. |
[8] | 64 YA and 64 OA (32 males) | HNC (real-life driving conditions) vs. LNC (only pedals; no enactment associated with direction) vs. IC (verbal instructions without driving; decisional level only) vs. passive (no driving no decision) effect on episodic memory performance. Intentional encoding. | Between | Non-immersive; input devices: steering wheel and pedals; virtual city. | Immediate free recall (what and details; binding: what + where + when; remember/know paradigm); visuospatial recall test (what, where, when on real map); delayed free recall test (what, details, where, and when); recognition test (elements). | Binding, regardless of age-groups, was enhanced by LNC and IC; HNC and passive conditions did not help episodic memory performance in both groups. Interestingly, Remember responses were boosted in older adults by IC condition. Active condition may be helpful when do not overload cognitive resources. |
[72] | 90 students (average age of 20; 45 males) | Passive VE (recorded route) vs. active VE vs. real environment (navigate the environment with instructions) and immediate vs. 48-h recall. Predetermined route. | Between | Non-immersive; input device: joystick; virtual replica of the Bordeaux area. | Immediate or 48-h recall task: real world wayfinding (replication of the real route), freehand sketch (directional changes) and photograph classification (picture in chronological order). | Transfer and sketch task are efficient after 48 h of retention and it is efficient for the two paper-pencil tasks. Active navigation led to benefits in wayfinding task, irrespective of the delay retention. |
[73] | 59 YA (age range: 19–29; 19 males) | Effect of active vs. passive free exploration on object recognition in VEs. | Between | Immersive; input device: keyboard; virtual rooms with objects. | Recognition task (objects). | Active navigation led to higher hit and lower miss responses than the passive condition. Active navigation has an important role in landmark recognition. |
[74] Exp. 1 | 32 YA (age range: 18–34; 16 males) | Active vs. passive free navigation with four trials with different virtual maze. | Between | Non-immersive with static navigation; input devices: keyboard and mouse; virtual maze. | “Active” test: number of moves and time. | Navigational knowledge is represented regardless the kind of exploration condition. |
[58] | 20 male students (age range: 20–26) | Active (self-governed) vs. passive (avatar-guided) free exploration. Four exploring sessions. Intentional encoding. | Between | Immersive (HMD); input device: joystick; virtual school. | Wayfinding task (short route to starting point); pointing task (orienting to the starting point); sketch-map (local accuracy or survey-type organization). | Self-governed explorers were better in completing the wayfinding task. Sketch-map accuracy was similar in both groups, whereas self-governed group had better survey-type organization. No differences were shown in pointing task. Self-governed participants organize their knowledge in survey mode. |
[75] | 34 YA (age range: 18–38; 7 males) | Active vs. passive (passenger condition) condition. Participants were before divided in driver and non-drivers. Intentional encoding and decision-making. | Between | Non-immersive: input devices: steering wheel and pedals; virtual city. | Survey knowledge: pointing error scores (street-level view) and map placement error scores (bird’s eye view); route knowledge: route scores (shortest route). | Driver had better route scores during active navigation compared with drivers in passive and non-driver in active conditions. Drivers showed better map scores (no condition effect). Active navigators do not learn more spatial layout knowledge and dual task effect may affect scores in non-drivers. |
[59] | 54 students (9 males) | Active vs. passive (passenger condition). Three exposures (3, 10, or 15 times). No intentional encoding. Predetermined path. | Between (yoked) | Non-immersive: input devices: steering wheel and pedals; virtual city. | Survey knowledge (map sketch drawing and map rates’ score), route knowledge (travel directions) and landmark knowledge (landmarks recall). | Passengers recalled more landmarks across exposure conditions. Survey errors reduced between 5 and 15 times in both conditions. Exposure led to better map reliability especially for the passive condition. Attentional resources could have led to worst performance in active drivers. |
[76] Exp. 3 | 41 undergraduates (age range: 18–24; 20 males) | VE active vs. VE passive (watch experimenter navigation) vs. VE + line (active with path to follow; no free exploration) vs. control (no VE training; real -world wayfinding). | Between | Non-immersive. Input device: keyboard; virtual replica of an office. | Real world transfer task (balloons wayfinding times and errors from virtual to real places); training task (wayfinding time and errors in VE conditions). | Times for active condition were lower compared with control condition and active and VE + line led to fewer errors than control condition. Virtual real transfer occurs thanks to virtual interaction. |
[60] | 64 students (average age of 20; 32 males) | Ground vs. areal point of view and active vs. passive navigation. Predetermined route | Between | Non-immersive; input device: joystick; virtual replica of the Bordeaux area. | Real world wayfinding task (replication of the real route; error scores), sketch-drawing task (directional changes; errors and omissions scores) and scene-sorting task (errors). | Active navigation boosted sketch-mapping task and worsened wayfinding and picture-sorting scores. Grounded-level condition improved performance in wayfinding and picture-sorting tasks, whereas aerial-level in sketch-mapping task. Active navigation and grounded-level interaction had a positive effect in the wayfinding and picture-sorting tasks, whereas passive and aerial-level condition improved sketch-mapping scores. Egocentric information and motor information create a correct perception-action coupling. |
[77] | 64 students (average age of 20; 32 males) | Detailed vs. undetailed visual fidelity and active vs. passive navigation. Predetermined route. | Between | Non-immersive; input device: joystick; virtual replica of the Bordeaux area. | Real-world wayfinding task (replication of the real route; errors and hesitations scores), sketch-mapping task (directional changes; errors and omissions scores), and scene-sorting task (errors). | Results highlighted better performance for each spatial task in both active and detailed condition. Interaction effect (active and detailed) led to better scores for sketch task and active condition combined with undetailed VE worsened scene-sorting task. Perceptual-motor information is crucial in spatial knowledge. Visual fidelity has positive effect for allocentric representation but not for route knowledge. |
[78] Exp. 1 & 2 | 28 students (age range: 18–23; 9 males) | Active exploration (experiment 1) vs. passive exploration (experiment 2; video of active exploration). Predetermined route of familiar environment. | Between | Non-immersive; input device: keyboard; university building. | Orientation test trials of external cues from four virtual rooms (internal visited and unvisited, external visited and unvisited). | There is no difference in the two conditions. |
[78] Exp. 3 | 54 visitors (mean age = 17.55, SD = 1.14; 19 males) | Active exploration vs. passive exploration. Predetermined route of unfamiliar environment. | Between | Non-immersive; input device: keyboard; university building. | Orientation test trials of external cues from four virtual rooms (internal visited and unvisited, external visited and unvisited). | No effect of unfamiliarity for active participants. Passive participants had greater error for the internal unvisited room. Active exploration enhances survey knowledge for unfamiliar environments. |
[79] | 60 adults (mean age = 25.2, SD = 4.5; males 49) | Active exploration vs. passive exploration (video of passive exploration) and immersive vs. computer screen. Predetermined rout of Gowanus canal. Intentional encoding. An allocentric map of the canal was provided in all conditions. | Within | Immersive (Emotiv EPOC headset) and non-immersive; input device: mouse and headset gyroscope; Gowanus Canal. | Elements recognition task | No difference between the two navigation conditions. However, active navigation with the mouse has higher level of engagement. |
[80] Exp. 1 and 2 | 3D active exploration (Exp. 1) vs. 2D passive snapshots presentation (Exp. 2). Free exploration and intentional encoding. | Between | Immersive (nVisor SX111) and non-immersive; input device: Wiimote; virtual apartment. | Search trials of geometric and contextual objects. | Search task improved in both conditions but in the immersive condition initial fixations and time spent in the incorrect rooms and better selection of the correct room indicate higher use of memory. | |
[81] | 28 YA (mean age = 25.6, SD = 5.4; 17 males) | Active navigation vs. passive navigation (video). Immediate (intentional encoding) and 24 h delayed (naïve) recall. Participants could freely navigate the environment. | Between | Non-immersive; input device: keyboard and mouse; virtual city. | Immediate and delayed free recall of semantically linked images of 3D objects placed in the town. | No effect of navigation types on spatial memory. |
[57] | 14 YA (mean age = 22, SD = 2.08; 7 males) | Full condition (full control over the navigation) vs. medium condition (participants move but do not control pre-recorded navigation) vs. low condition (watch pre-recorded navigation). | Within | Immersive (Oculus Rift DK2); input device: Kinect for legs and arms movement detection; virtual city. | Immediate free recall (what, egocentric where, details, when, binding) and item recognition (source memory, remember/know/guess paradigm), egocentric, allocentric and temporal recognition. | Any significance was found among the conditions. However, the full and medium (virtual embodiment) conditions were more immersive than the passive one. |
[82] | 16 students (females = 16) | Active navigation vs. passive (watching the navigation of the active participant). Free exploration. | Between (yoked) | Non-immersive (46-inch touchscreen monitor); input device: joystick; virtual rooms | Immediate memory recognition for objects manipulated in each room). | Both passive and active navigation had a significant negative effect on memory of object, with active navigation having a greater effect compared to passive. |
[61] | 22 YA (mean age = 19.71, SD = 2.19; females = 11) and 22 OA (mean age = 74.55, SD = 7.82; females = 10) | Active navigation vs. passive. Free exploration. | Within (yoked) | Immersive (cardboard) mobile application (input device: button headset and head movements); VE (city, park, mall) | Encoding-Retrieval route overlap accuracy | Active encoding leads to better spatial memory in OA; accuracy is predicted by age, active exploration and visuospatial abilities. |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Tuena, C.; Serino, S.; Dutriaux, L.; Riva, G.; Piolino, P. Virtual Enactment Effect on Memory in Young and Aged Populations: A Systematic Review. J. Clin. Med. 2019, 8, 620. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8050620
Tuena C, Serino S, Dutriaux L, Riva G, Piolino P. Virtual Enactment Effect on Memory in Young and Aged Populations: A Systematic Review. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2019; 8(5):620. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8050620
Chicago/Turabian StyleTuena, Cosimo, Silvia Serino, Léo Dutriaux, Giuseppe Riva, and Pascale Piolino. 2019. "Virtual Enactment Effect on Memory in Young and Aged Populations: A Systematic Review" Journal of Clinical Medicine 8, no. 5: 620. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8050620
APA StyleTuena, C., Serino, S., Dutriaux, L., Riva, G., & Piolino, P. (2019). Virtual Enactment Effect on Memory in Young and Aged Populations: A Systematic Review. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 8(5), 620. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8050620