Next Article in Journal
Translating Evidence from Clonal Hematopoiesis to Cardiovascular Disease: A Systematic Review
Next Article in Special Issue
Evidence of Mitochondrial Dysfunction in Fibromyalgia: Deviating Muscle Energy Metabolism Detected Using Microdialysis and Magnetic Resonance
Previous Article in Journal
Endometriosis Lowers the Cumulative Live Birth Rates in IVF by Decreasing the Number of Embryos but Not Their Quality
Previous Article in Special Issue
Reliability, Factor Structure and Predictive Validity of the Widespread Pain Index and Symptom Severity Scales of the 2010 American College of Rheumatology Criteria of Fibromyalgia
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of COMT Genotypes on Working Memory Performance in Fibromyalgia Patients

by
David Ferrera
1,
Francisco Gómez-Esquer
2,
Irene Peláez
1,
Paloma Barjola
1,
Roberto Fernandes-Magalhaes
1,
Alberto Carpio
1,
María E. De Lahoz
1,
Gema Díaz-Gil
2 and
Francisco Mercado
1,*
1
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Rey Juan Carlos University, 28922 Madrid, Spain
2
Department of Basic Health Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, Rey Juan Carlos University, 28922 Madrid, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9(8), 2479; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9082479
Submission received: 2 July 2020 / Revised: 28 July 2020 / Accepted: 29 July 2020 / Published: 1 August 2020

Abstract

:
Growing research has reported the presence of a clear impairment of working memory functioning in fibromyalgia. Although different genetic factors involving dopamine availability (i.e, the COMT gene) have been associated with the more severe presentation of key symptoms in fibromyalgia, scientific evidence regarding the influence of COMT genotypes on cognitive impairment in these patients is still lacking. To this end, 167 participants took part in the present investigation. Working memory performance was assessed by the application of the SST (Spatial Span Test) and LNST (Letter and Number Sequence Test) belonging to the Weschler Memory Scale III. Significant working memory impairment was shown by the fibromyalgia patients. Remarkably, our results suggest that performance according to different working memory measures might be influenced by different genotypes of the COMT gene. Specifically, fibromyalgia patients carrying the Val/Val genotype exhibited significantly worse outcomes for the span of SST backward, SST backward score, SST total score and the Working Memory Index (WMI) than the Val/Val healthy carriers. Furthermore, the Val/Val patients performed worse on the SST backward and SST score than heterozygotes. Our findings are the first to show a link between the COMT gene and working memory dysfunction in fibromyalgia, supporting the idea that higher COMT enzyme activity would contribute to more severe working memory impairment in fibromyalgia.

1. Introduction

Neurobiological evidence gathered from the past decade has established that dopamine plays a crucial role in neurotransmission mechanisms supporting mental operations involved in working memory processing [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9]. In line with this, whereas dopamine depletion in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) has been related to the appearance of deficits in working memory [10,11,12], the administration of dopamine agonist drugs (e.g., pergolide or bromocriptine) has been associated with an improvement in such processes [13,14,15,16,17].
Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) is an enzyme involved in the breakdown of several catecholamine neurotransmitters, such as dopamine, noradrenaline and serotonin [18,19,20]. Specifically, it has been reported that this enzyme also regulates the amount of dopamine in the PFC [20,21], amygdala, entorhinal cortex or hippocampus [22,23,24]—regions belonging to the neural network underlying working memory processing. One of the most studied single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) of the COMT gene is the rs4680 polymorphism, which arises by a substitution, in the 158 codon, from valine (Val) to methionine (Met) [25]. This change causes the enzyme encoded by the gene to have about 1/4 less activity [26], influencing, in turn, the availability of dopamine within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [27]. Several experimental studies have demonstrated that performance in working memory tasks may differ as a consequence of carrying different alleles of this gene [28,29,30,31,32,33,34]. It has been reported that those subjects carrying Met alleles showed better functioning in verbal and visuo-spatial working memory tasks [28,30,34], and fewer perseverative errors on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) than Val carriers [35,36]. These data also seem to have a neurophysiological correlate, since brain imaging studies have shown that Val carriers exhibited a decrease in dopamine levels within the PFC, reducing the blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) response of this brain region compared to that in the Met carriers [31,36], and also reflected by the poor performance of Val carriers in working memory tests [29]. These results have also been replicated in chronic patients, such as those suffering from posttraumatic stress disorder or schizophrenia [36,37,38,39], leading to the consideration of this gene as a therapeutic target for improving cognitive symptoms [40].
The COMT gene has been widely studied with respect to the pain-related and affective symptoms of chronic pain syndromes, such as fibromyalgia [41,42,43,44]. Experimental evidence has suggested that fibromyalgia patients carrying the two Met alleles of the COMT gene had a higher number of tender points [42], greater sensibility to painful stimulation [41,43] and, also, a higher severity of depressive and anxious symptomatology than Val carriers [43,44,45].
Along with affective and physical symptoms, subjective complaints about cognitive functioning (encompassed under the term fibrofog [46]) are one of the main symptoms in fibromyalgia [47,48]. The cognitive impairments have been proposed as more disabling than the pain itself [49]. Systematically, neuropsychological evidence has shown that fibromyalgia patients exhibit significant cognitive dysfunction compared to healthy control participants in different domains, such as attention [50,51,52], processing speed [53], executive functioning [54,55,56,57,58] and theory of mind [59]. Remarkably, it has been suggested that these impairments might be better explained by alterations in working memory processes [60,61], where these findings were particularly solid [62,63].
However, despite the frequently reported relationship between working memory impairment and fibromyalgia, the effect of biological indices (i.e., the COMT gene) related to dopamine availability on cognitive resources in these patients has not been explored to date. Previous experimental evidence coming from voxel-based morphometry studies [64] has shown clear decrements in cerebral grey matter density in fibromyalgia patients within regions related to working memory processing, such as the hippocampal gyrus and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) [65,66]. Furthermore, these changes in cerebral grey matter have been linked to the presence of dopamine metabolism abnormalities in fibromyalgia [64]. In line with these findings, some authors have recently pointed out the need to explore the relationship between dopamine and cognitive processes in chronic pain [67]. This accumulated evidence leads us to suggest that COMT, as a genetic marker involved in dopamine availability, might be a useful target to better characterise cognitive dysfunction in fibromyalgia.
In summary, this investigation aimed to explore the relationship between the different genotypes of COMT and working memory dysfunction in fibromyalgia. To this end, we analysed the rs4680 SNP of the COMT gene in fibromyalgia patients and healthy participants and measured their performance in neuropsychological tests involving working memory processing. Based on previous findings, we expected to confirm the presence of cognitive impairment in fibromyalgia. Additionally, we hypothesised that this altered neuropsychological functioning regarding working memory processing would be even worse in those patients carrying the Val/Val genotype of the SNP rs4680 than in the Met allele carriers.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample Size Calculation

In order to calculate the number of participants required for this study, the G*Power software was used [68]. This software allows one to determine the sample size necessary to obtain a certain statistical power. In this study, the sample size was calculated using the a priori statistical power of 1 − β = 0.80 for a medium effect size. The total sample size obtained for this statistical power was 158 subjects. We expected that 10% of participants’ data would be lost during the experimental procedure; therefore, we decided to increase the total sample size to 173. Data from six subjects were lost because they did not complete all the tests; thus, one hundred and sixty-seven Caucasian women took part in the experiment. Eighty-six were fibromyalgia patients, and eighty-one were healthy control participants (HC).

2.2. Participants

Fibromyalgia patients were recruited from the Fibromyalgia and Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Association (AFYSYFACRO) and Fibromyalgia Association of Pinto (AFAP) of Comunidad de Madrid (Spain). All fulfilled the diagnosis criteria proposed by the American College of Rheumatology (ACR) in 2010 [47]. The diagnosis of fibromyalgia was carried out by different rheumatologists among different public hospitals of the Comunidad de Madrid. HC participants were recruited by email, by advertisements published along the Faculty of Health Science at the Rey Juan Carlos University and among the friends of the fibromyalgia patients. The whole sample was aged between 35 and 67 years old. The HC participants were selected in such a way that the age and educational level were matched with those of the patients. The HC participants did not suffer from any chronic pain conditions. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal eyesight and the ability to read and write in Spanish at the equivalent of eighth grade level in English. No differences were found in terms of age (F (1165) = 0.001, p = 0.975) or educational level (χ2 (3) = 3.305, p = 0.347) between the two groups of participants. They had no history of psychiatric disorders (i.e., psychosis, personality disorders or substance abuse/dependence) or neurological diseases. The HC participants did not suffer from any chronic pain conditions. Patients who took medications kept doing so for both medical prescription and ethical considerations. Most patients were taking analgesics, antidepressants and benzodiazepines.

2.3. Procedure

Data for whole sample were collected in the Faculty of Health Science of Rey Juan Carlos University (Madrid). Once in the laboratory, participants gave written informed consent for their involvement in the experiment. Subsequently, we collected the demographic data, clinical information and drug consumption information. Once all this information was collected, the neuropsychological evaluation was always carried out in the same order by an expert psychologist. Finally, saliva samples were collected according to the manufacturer’s protocol from all participants. Each session lasted approximately 45 to 60 min.

2.4. Self-Reported Measurements and Psychological Pain Assessment

Participants provided written informed consent for their involvement in the experiment. The Rey Juan Carlos Research Ethics Board approved this study (ref: 201000100011550), which followed all the requirements of this committee. Subsequently, participants completed some self-report instruments in their validated Spanish version. The whole sample fulfilled the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) [69]. The STAI test is a self-report questionnaire composed of 20 items for measuring Trait Anxiety and 20 items for State Anxiety ranging from 0 to 60 (subsequently assigning a centile score). The reported values of Cronbach’s α were 0.93 for the Trait Anxiety and 0.87 for State Anxiety subscales [69]. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) [70] was also administered to all participants for measuring depressive symptoms. This scale has scores ranging from 0 to 63. The values of Cronbach’s α were 0.87 [70]. Additionally, fibromyalgia patients completed the Spanish version of the Fibromyalgia Impact Questionnaire (FIQ-S) [71], a specific questionnaire to assess the current health and functional status of fibromyalgia patients with a score ranging between 0 and 80. Just before starting the experiment, a visual analogue scale (VAS) ranging from 0 (no pain at all) to 10 (worse imaginable pain) was applied for monitoring the level of pain suffered by the participants in the previous week, including the day of testing. The reported Cronbach’s α for this test was 0.82 [71]. The fibromyalgia patients showed higher scores for depression (F (1165) = 111.431, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.403), state anxiety (F (1165) = 11.534, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.065), trait anxiety (F (1165) = 51.981, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.240) and VAS pain (F (1165) = 329.208, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.666) than the control participants. Data regarding the psychological variables are shown in Table 1, along with the information on drug consumption and sociodemographic conditions.

2.5. Neuropsychological Testing of Working Memory

Working memory functioning was assessed by two different neuropsychological tests. Both tests are part of the Working Memory Index (WMI) belonging to the Spanish version of the Wechsler Memory Scale III (WMS-III) [72]. This index allows one to obtain a measure of the individual’s performance in different tasks that require attending to, retaining and processing information in memory, and subsequently generating a response according to that information [72]. The WMI is calculated from the scores obtained in the Spatial Span Test (SST) and the Letter and Number Sequence Test (LNST). These tests were selected because they have been considered as validated and standardized measures of verbal and visual–spatial functions of working memory functioning. Values of Cronbach’s α between 0.85 and 0.99 were reported [73].
The SST of the WMS-III evaluates immediate spatial working memory [74]. This task is a modified version of the classical Corsi Block-Tapping Test [75] and has been previously used in chronic patients with working memory impairments [76,77]. Participants were presented with a board on which a set of ten cubes or blocks were fixed. On the board, all the cubes had written on them numbers from 1 to 10 that only the examiner could see. The SST test was applied twice, one forwards and the other backwards (see Figure 1). In the forward application, participants were asked to repeat in the same order the tapping sequence previously made by the examiner. The first sequence presented to the participants started with a tapping sequence of three blocks. Two trials were applied for each sequence length. The SST finished when the participants failed to reproduce both trials of the same sequence. For the backward application, participants were asked to repeat the tapping sequence but in reverse order (i.e., backwards). Whereas performing the forward version requires the setting in motion of several processes such as encoding, memory delay and the implementation of response alternatives, the backward application requires the activation of executive processes such as manipulating information [78]. Performance in this test was measured by recording the following raw scores: span of SST forward (number of correct trials performed in the forward application; score ranging from 0 to 9), SST forward score (sum of all the points obtained by the subject in direct order with a score ranging from 0 to 16), span of SST backward (number of correct trials performed in the backward application; in the same way as in the forward application, the score range was 0 to 9), SST backward score (sum of all the points obtained by the subject in the backward application with a score ranging from 0 to 16) and SST total score (sum of SST forward score and SST backward score, whose range of scores was 0 to 32).
The LNST was used for assessing verbal working memory. A series of sequences consisting of disordered letters and numbers were verbally presented by the examiner to the participants, who were instructed to reproduce them in a specific order: first, the numbers from the smallest to the largest, followed by the letters in alphabetic order. The lengths of the LNST series were gradually increased, starting from lengths of two elements and finishing with series including eight elements (e.g., two elements: L-2 (2-L); eight elements: 7-M-2-T-6-F-9-A (2-6-7-9-A-F-M-T). The same criterion applied in the SST test for stopping its application was also adopted for the LNST test (i.e., it finished when the participants failed two consecutive elements belonging to the same sequence length). This scale has been shown to be sensitive for exploring working memory disorders in different chronic syndromes [79,80]. Two different memory measures were computed for this test: the span of LNST (maximum number of elements correctly remembered; score ranging from 0 to 8) and LNST score (sum of all the points obtained by the subject in this task; score ranging from 0 to 21). Finally, the total score of the WMI (score ranging between 50 and 150) was also calculated by computing the total scores of the LNST and SST tests.

2.6. COMT Genotyping

Unstimulated whole saliva samples were collected into collection tubes according to standardized procedures. Subjects were asked not to eat, drink or chew gum for 1 hour before sample collection. Immediately after collection, the samples were stored at −20 °C until analysis. Saliva, as compared to blood collection, has the following advantages: it requires no specialized personnel for collection, allows for remote collection by the patient, is painless, is well accepted by participants, has decreased risks of disease transmission, does not clot, can be frozen before DNA extraction and possibly has a longer storage time.
Genomic DNA was extracted from 5 mL of saliva using a REALPURE Saliva RBMEG06 Kit (Durviz, Valencia, Spain) according to the manufacturer’s protocol. The resulting DNA was diluted to 100–1000 ng/μL, using 1×Tris-EDTA (TE) buffer (Sigma-Aldrich, Dorset, UK) and assessed for purity and concentration using a NanoDrop™ ND1000 Spectrophotometer (Thermo Fisher Scientific Inc., Hemel Hempstead, Hertfordshire, UK). COMT polymorphisms were genotyped by real-time polymerase chain reaction (RT-PCR) analysis using TaqMan® Predesigned SNP Genotyping Assays for rs4680 polymorphisms (Thermo Fisher Scientific Inc., Hemel Hempstead, Hertfordshire, UK). TaqMan® SNP Genotyping Assays use TaqMan® 5′-nuclease chemistry for amplifying and detecting specific polymorphisms in purified genomic DNA samples. Each assay allows the genotyping of individuals for a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP). Each TaqMan® SNP Genotyping Assay contains: (A) sequence-specific forward and reverse primers to amplify the polymorphic sequence of interest and (B) two TaqMan® minor groove binder (MGB) probes with non-fluorescent quenchers (NFQ): one VICTM-labeled probe to detect Allele 1’s sequence and one FAMTM-labelled probe to detect Allele 2’s sequence. Amplification was carried out an ABI Prism 7000 Sequence Detection System (Thermo Fisher Scientific Inc., Hemel Hempstead, Hertfordshire, UK) in the Genomics unit of the Technological Support Center (CAT) of the Rey Juan Carlos University. All genotypes were determined twice.

2.7. Data Analysis

The normal distribution of the dependent variables was checked using the ratio of kurtosis and asymmetry to the standard error. This index for each variable was between +2 and −2, confirming that the scores did not violate the assumption of normality [81,82].
A chi-square test (χ²) was used to evaluate the statistical distribution of the COMT genotypes in both the fibromyalgia patients and HC participants in order to verify the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE).
To explore the potential effect of the COMT rs4680 polymorphism (participants were grouped in three different genotypes: Met/Met, Val/Val and Met/Val) on the functioning of working memory, two-way ANOVAs were performed. The span of SST forward, SST forward score, span of SST backward, SST backward score, SST total score, span of LNST, LNST score and WMI were considered as dependent variables, while the COMT genotypes (Met/Met, Val/Val and Met/Val) and groups (two levels: fibromyalgia and HC participants) were set as the between-subject factors. Post hoc comparisons to determine the significance of pairwise contrasts were performed using Bonferroni adjustment (α = 0.05) for controlling the Type I error rate. Effect sizes were computed using the partial eta-square (ƞ2p) method only for the significant analyses. We also estimated statistical power using the observed power (1 − β), only for the significant interactions.
Additionally, the association between the clinical questionnaire scores and the performance in the working memory tests was explored in two ways. First, at an exploratory level, Pearson correlations (r) were determined. Secondly, to test whether the effects in the working memory tests were strictly due to the COMT genotypes but not influenced by the differences in the clinical scores between groups, a series of ANCOVAs were performed. Thus, those clinical variables that were significantly associated with neuropsychological outcomes were introduced in these analyses as covariables (i.e., depression, anxiety and pain) in order to neutralise their possible influence on the statistical results. Effect sizes were computed using the partial eta-square (η2p) method. A significance level of 0.05 (two tailed) was used for all statistical analyses where significant.
As has been previously reported, benzodiazepines and antidepressants might have different effects on cognition [83]. To account for this, several control analyses were performed via ANOVAs on working memory scores, only including fibromyalgia patients using and not using particular medications. We introduced the psychotropic drugs (benzodiazepines and antidepressants) as factors, whereas the working memory measures were dependent variables. All statistical analyses were performed with the SPSS package (v.25.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

3. Results

3.1. COMT Polymorphism Frequencies

The chi-square (χ²) test was used to assess the distribution of the genotypes in order to check the HWE. The frequency of the COMT rs4680 polymorphism distribution fulfilled the HWE in both the HC participants (χ2 = 0.351, p = 0.553) and fibromyalgia patients (χ2 = 0.04, p = 0.825). Statistical data related to the genotype and allele frequency distributions of the COMT gene considering each group of participants are displayed in the Table 2.

3.2. Statistical Effects in the Neuropsychological Assessment

ANOVAs revealed a major effect of group, where fibromyalgia patients showed significantly lower scores than HC participants in most of the working memory measures: LNST score (F (1161) = 6.603, p = 0.011, η2p = 0.039), span of SST forward (F (1161) = 6.712, p = 0.010, η2p = 0.040), SST forward score (F (1161) = 6.495, p = 0.012, η2p = 0.039), span of SST backwards (F (1161) = 5.343, p = 0.022, η2p = 0.032), SST backward score (F (1161) = 6.618 p = 0.011, η2p = 0.039), SST total score (F (1161) = 9.080, p = 0.003, η2p = 0.053) and WMI (F (1161) = 9.709, p = 0.002, η2p = 0.057). Only scores related to the span of LNST did not yield significant differences between both groups (F (1161) = 1.126, p = 0.327).

3.3. Effects of Interaction between COMT Genotypes and Group of Participants on Working Memory Performance

The mean scores for both the verbal and spatial working memory tests related to each group of participants are displayed in Table 3. Regarding the interaction between the COMT genotypes and group, the ANOVAs showed statistically significant differences for the SST backward score (F (2161) = 5.543, p = 0.005, η2p = 0.064, 1 − β = 0.849) and SST total score (F (2161) = 4.921, p = 0.008, η2p = 0.058, 1 − β = 0.801). In both cases, post hoc analyses showed a similar difference pattern. Specifically, fibromyalgia patients carrying the Val/Val genotype showed significantly lower scores than the Val/Val genotype carriers belonging to the HC group (p = 0.001 for the comparison of the SST backward score and SST total score), as can be also seen in Figure 2. We also found a significant difference for the fibromyalgia group between those patients carrying the Val/Val and Met/Val genotypes (p = 0.03 for the SST backward score and p = 0.014 for the SST total score). Specifically, the Val/Val genotype carriers performed worse according to both neuropsychological measures than the patients carrying other genotypes. In addition, an interaction effect between the COMT genotype and group was also found for the span of SST backward score (F (2161) = 4.013, p = 0.020, η2p = 0.047, 1 − β = 0.710). Post hoc analyses revealed a significant difference between the fibromyalgia and HC groups that only was true for those participants carrying the Val/Val genotype (p = 0.001). In this case, the fibromyalgia patients showed a lower performance than the HC participants (see Figure 2). Finally, we found a statistically significant interaction between the COMT genotype and group for the WMI (F (2161) = 4.095, p = 0.018, η2p = 0.048, 1 − β = 0.720). Post hoc analyses showed that the fibromyalgia group scored lower than the HC participants. This difference was obtained for both the Val/Val genotype (p = 0.007) and Met/Met genotype (p = 0.014) carriers (Table 3). No other comparison yielded significant results for the span of LNST, LNST score, span of SST forward and SST forward score. In addition, no major effect of COMT was found.

3.4. Relationship between the Clinical Variables and Neuropsychological Test Results

As mentioned in the Data Analysis section, we evaluated the relationship between the scores obtained from the clinical questionnaires and the working memory test results in terms of Pearson correlations (see Table 4). Trait anxiety (Trait-STAI) correlated negatively with the number of correct trials performed in the Spatial Span test (Span of SST forward) (r = −0.219, p = 0.004), the number of correct backward trials performed in the Spatial Span test (Span of SST backward) (r = −0.206, p = 0.008), the score for the Spatial Span test in the forward application (SST forward score) (r = −0.209, p = 0.007) and the total score for the Spatial Span test (SST total score) (r = −0.192, p = 0.013). In other words, the higher the scores related to trait anxiety were, the lower the span of SST forward, the span of SST backward, the SST forward score and the SST total score were. Moreover, depression (BDI) and the Letter and Number Sequence test score (LNST score) (r = −0.157, p = 0.042) showed a significant association. These variables were also inversely correlated, so the higher the depression, the lower the LNST score. Additionally, depression and the Working Memory Index (WMI) showed a significant association (r = −0.64, p = 0.035); the lower the WMI, the higher the scores related to depression. Finally, pain outcomes correlated with the number of correct trials performed in the direct order of the Spatial Span test (span of SST forward) (r = −0.157, p = 0.043). The rest of the analyses did not show statistical differences, as can be seen in Table 4.
Based on the results provided by the correlation analyses, ANCOVAs were carried out including the clinical variables showing a significant association with the working memory test scores as covariables. The analyses revealed that the differences shown in the working memory measures as a function of the interaction between the COMT genotypes and group were independent of the clinical symptomatology. Specifically, the COMT genotype and group interactions, after controlling for scores in trait anxiety, remained significant for the span of SST backward (F (2160) = 3.827, p = 0.024, η2p = 0.046) and SST total score (F (2160) = 4.663, p = 0.011, η2p = 0.055). Furthermore, the interaction effect between the COMT genotype and group for the WMI after controlling for depression scores also remained significant (F (2160) = 4.051, p = 0.019, η2p = 0.048). In the case of the rest of the significant variables described in the correlation analyses, they consistently displayed non-significant differences for the COMT genotypes and group interaction, when trait anxiety (span of SST forward (F (2160) = 0.790, p = 0.456) and SST forward score (F (2160) = 1.606, p = 0.204)), depression (LNST score (F (2160) = 1.593, p = 0.207)) or pain were introduced as covariables (span of SST forward (F (2160) = 0.885, p = 0.415)).

3.5. Working Memory Analyses Controlling for the Intake of Psychotropic Drugs

As we have previously indicated, fibromyalgia patients usually have a high consumption of medications due to their chronic condition. Some of these drugs, such as antidepressants and benzodiazepines, can have a negative impact on cognitive performance. Therefore, we carried out several control analyses in order to explore the potential effects of drug consumption on cognitive functioning within the fibromyalgia group. ANOVAs were performed for patients who were taking and were not taking benzodiazepines or antidepressant drugs. The analyses revealed that working memory outcomes were not significantly influenced by the medications taken for fibromyalgia patients (see Table 5).

4. Discussion

The present investigation aimed to explore the relationship between the different genotypes of the COMT gene and working memory dysfunction in fibromyalgia. The results derived from the statistical analyses showed that patients with fibromyalgia had significantly lower scores on both the Letter and Number Sequence and Spatial Span tests. More interestingly, our findings suggest that the performance according to different working memory measures might be modulated by the different genotypes of the COMT rs4680 polymorphism. Specifically, fibromyalgia patients carrying the Val/Val genotype (i.e., having high COMT activity leading to higher dopamine degradation) exhibited significantly worse performance in terms of the span of SST backward, SST backward score, SST total score and Working Memory Index of the WMS-III. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that establishes a relationship between COMT genotype and working memory dysfunction in fibromyalgia.
As indicated in the Results section, our data have confirmed the presence of clear working memory dysfunction in fibromyalgia as reflected by the low performance according to the different neuropsychological measures (LNST and SST). Although there is some controversy about the nature and extent of the cognitive impairment in fibromyalgia [84,85,86,87], a wide group of studies using varied methodologies—such as neuropsychological tests [55,57,59,63,88,89], brain electrophysiological response assessments [54,58] and neuroimaging techniques [90,91,92]—has evidenced that cognitive functioning differs between fibromyalgia patients and healthy controls. In this sense, the available data suggest that cognitive impairment in fibromyalgia could be more evident when cognitive demands are elevated, as occurs in working memory tasks, where patients have to deal with situations that involve stimulus competition or require the allocation of cognitive resources to process distinct kinds of information at the same time [53,92,93].
What is more interesting regarding the current findings is the potential role of the COMT gene as a factor contributing to the cognitive impairment in fibromyalgia. Remarkably, our data have indicated that those patients carrying the Val/Val genotype of COMT exhibited significantly worse outcomes in terms of the span of SST backward, SST backward score, SST total score and the WMI than the HC participants carrying the same COMT genotype. Furthermore, we observed that homozygous valine fibromyalgia patients performed significantly lower in terms of the SST backward and SST total score than heterozygous patients. Finally, contrary to our expectations, the Met/Met genotype (i.e., that with low enzyme activity) was not associated with an improvement in working memory tasks in fibromyalgia. Although there is a lack of previous studies linking these genetic factors with cognitive dysfunction in fibromyalgia, the relationship between the COMT rs4680 polymorphism and working memory functioning has been consistently reported in healthy volunteers [28,30,31,34,94] and other pathologies characterized by working memory impairments such as schizophrenia or posttraumatic stress disorder [36,37,38,39]. It has been demonstrated that Val/Val carriers scored systematically lower than individuals carrying other genotypes on various working memory tasks. These chronic diseases have been characterized by brain activity abnormalities in prefrontal cortex (PFC) functioning while patients were involved in working memory tasks [95,96,97]. This scientific evidence is in accordance with the results derived from a growing body of investigations indicating that the PFC is a key brain region involved in the working memory impairments in fibromyalgia [90,91,92,98,99]. In this regard, Luerding and colleagues [90] have reported, using voxel-based morphometry, that fibromyalgia patients with lower grey matter volume in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) had poorer performance in the Corsi Block-Tapping Test, confirming the crucial role of prefrontal brain regions for this working memory dysfunction. Furthermore, the variation in the dopamine levels in the PFC seems to be related to the variable neural response of this brain region during the performance of working memory tasks [33,36,100]. It has been suggested that the different COMT enzymatic activity seems to modulate the amount of available dopamine, exerting an effect, in turn, on the interindividual variability in cognition [101]. In this vein, a high enzymatic activity of COMT, as occurs in the Val/Val genotype carriers [26], would lead to diminished levels of dopamine at the synapse and less activity in postsynaptic receptors. Consequently, whereas this extracellular reduction in dopamine availability would be related to a worsening of working memory processes, the lower activity of the COMT enzyme (i.e., the Met/Met genotype) seems to contribute to the augmentation of dopamine levels at the synapse and, in turn, to higher working memory efficiency [6]. Thus, it has been proposed that the increase in extracellular dopamine could improve cognitive symptoms in fibromyalgia [102], due to the involvement of the dopaminergic system in cognitive impairment in this chronic pain syndrome [103]. Nevertheless, it is important to note that the optimal execution in working memory tasks would require intermediate dopamine levels (very high or low levels of dopamine would have negative effects on working memory performance) [7], since the relationship between working memory and dopamine availability seems to follow an inverted-U shape [6,104]. This prior evidence could help us to shed light on some of the unexpected results obtained here, such as those shown by patients carrying the Met/Met COMT genotype who did not reach the highest working memory performance. It leads us to conjecture that patients in this subgroup could potentially have very high levels of dopamine, which might disadvantage them for obtaining better performance in working memory tests. Although these data should be confirmed in future studies, our results suggest that the enzymatic activity of COMT might modulate the cognitive performance of fibromyalgia patients, mainly in spatial working memory tasks.
The relationship between dopamine availability and working memory dysfunction in fibromyalgia deserves further consideration. The effects of enzymatic COMT activity on the cognitive performance in patients with fibromyalgia were observed mainly in those neuropsychological measures involving visuo-spatial working memory capabilities (i.e., the span of SST backward, SST backward score and SST total score) but not in those assessing verbal working memory processes (i.e., the span of LNST and LNST score). The question that now arises is why that effect was not homogeneously reflected across the whole set of applied working memory tests. The answer to this question may admit two complementary but non-exclusive explanations. Firstly, both the difficulty and the cognitive load conveyed by a given task are relevant factors for detecting impairments in working memory functioning [92,105,106,107]. In this sense, the cognitive load generated by the two different working memory tests used here, the LNST and SST, seems to be different. It has been consistently reported that verbal and spatial working memory tasks can be separable for involving different sets of resources [108,109,110]. With respect to this, some studies have indicated that maintenance sub-processes might produce a similar cognitive load in both verbal and spatial working memory tasks, but updating sub-processes (which implies adding or removing representations in working memory (see [111])) could be qualitatively different in both tasks [112]. These authors argued that the irrelevant items included in a verbal working memory updating sequence caused higher proactive interference than those included in spatial working memory, as the semantic content of verbal tasks can create more durable representations in working memory than spatial stimuli. On the other hand, certain spatial tasks, such as the Spatial Span Test, require not only the implementation of working memory sub-processes (i.e., updating, encoding or maintenance) but also the coordination of motor planning processes and integration of spatial information [113]. This integration is not needed in verbal working memory tasks. Secondly, it has been reported that the neural circuit that connects the PFC and the hippocampus is a crucial pathway in spatial working memory tasks and this circuit could be modulated by cortical dopaminergic neurotransmission [113]. In this line, scientific evidence from brain imaging studies has demonstrated that carriers of the Val/Val genotype had lower “connection strength” between the hippocampus and the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC) while subjects performed a visual memory task [23]. On the other hand, primate studies have shown an increase in extracellular dopamine levels in the DLPFC during the performance of tasks that require the implementation of processes related to working memory, mainly in spatial tasks [114,115]. All this experimental evidence seems to confirm the importance of dopamine levels in the fronto-temporal network during cognitive processing [116], which could be proposed as a tentative explanation for the cognitive dysfunction shown by fibromyalgia patients in spatial, but not in verbal, working memory tests.
Finally, as the analysis carried out in the study showed, the influence of the COMT genotypes on the neuropsychological outcomes was independent of clinical symptoms (i.e., pain, depression and anxiety). Although some authors have found an influence of clinical outcomes on cognitive performance in fibromyalgia [84,86,117], others have failed to find this relationship [56,105,118,119,120]. Therefore, there is still some debate on this issue. Some authors have explained that clinical symptoms, especially pain, are not the origin of cognitive problems in fibromyalgia syndrome but are concomitant to these [119]. Although these symptoms (depression or anxiety) may contribute somewhat to cognitive dysfunction in fibromyalgia, they cannot explain all the deterioration that patients show in different cognitive domains [105]. This author has argued that even more research is needed to explore the role of clinical symptoms in cognition, as well as to provide more information about the combined effects of fibromyalgia with clinical symptoms on cognitive function.
Some limitations related to other potential influences should be taken into account for the interpretation of the current findings. Firstly, although the sample size was sufficient to obtain high statistical power, future studies should confirm these conclusions using larger samples. Secondly, the effects of dopamine on working memory are paradoxical [13,121], with opposite effects observed in similar studies. For this reason, the results derived from this study should be taken with caution since it has been observed that the relationship between the COMT genotypes and working memory not only depends on dopamine degradation but also on other genes related to dopamine transporters [29] or dopamine receptors [8,9,11,12,20]. These genes have been considered relevant for the modulation of dopamine transmission within deeper neural structures involved in working memory processing, such as the striatum or the PFC [1,122]. On the other hand, although the single SNP analysis has provided abundant evidence regarding the role of the COMT gene in working memory functioning, the use of a haplotype-based approach could complement this analysis due to its better sensitivity in representing the subjects’ availability of dopamine. Finally, although drug consumption affecting cognition was statistically controlled to neutralise its influence, future research only including fibromyalgia patients who are free of medication is recommended.

5. Conclusions

In summary, our data are the first to establish a role of COMT, a gene involved in the availability of dopamine, on working memory dysfunction in fibromyalgia patients. Specific genotypic variations of the COMT gene (i.e., Val/Val allele carriers) might contribute to diminished cognitive performance in fibromyalgia, mainly in visuo-spatial working memory tasks. These promising data could help to better characterize and detect working memory dysfunction in the more cognitively impaired patients and contribute to the design of more adjusted cognitive treatments for these chronic pain patients. Nevertheless, future studies should be done using a COMT haplotype-based approach to explore its influence on the working memory impairment in fibromyalgia in more depth, not only by means of neuropsychological assessments but also focusing on the neural indices underlying this cognitive dysfunction.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.F., F.G.-E. and F.M.; data curation, D.F., F.G.-E., I.P., P.B., R.F.-M., A.C., M.E.D.L., G.D.-G. and F.M.; formal analysis, D.F., F.G.-E. and F.M.; funding acquisition, F.M.; investigation, D.F., F.G.-E., I.P., P.B., R.F.-M., A.C., M.E.D.L. and F.M.; methodology, D.F., F.G.-E., P.B., R.F.-M. and F.M.; project administration, F.M.; resources, F.G.-E., G.D.-G. and F.M.; software, D.F. and F.G.-E.; supervision, F.G.-E., I.P., P.B., G.D.-G. and F.M.; validation, D.F., F.G.-E., I.P., P.B., R.F.-M, A.C., M.E.D.L., G.D.-G. and F.M.; visualization, D.F.; writing—original draft, D.F., F.G.-E. and F.M.; writing—review and editing, D.F., F.G.-E., I.P., P.B., R.F.-M., A.C., M.E.D.L., G.D.-G. and F.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by grants PSI2017-85241-R from the Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (MINECO) of Spain, EMO-CM S2015/HUM-3327, PEJD-2017-PRE/SOC-3501 and SAPIENTIA-CM H2019/HUM-5705 of the Comunidad de Madrid.

Acknowledgments

We also want to thank the Fibromyalgia and Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Association (AFINSYFACRO) and the Fibromyalgia Association of Pinto (AFAP) for their involvement in this study. We thank Beatriz Santamaría Hernández for technical support.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Zhang, Y.; Bertolino, A.; Fazio, L.; Blasi, G.; Rampino, A.; Romano, R.; Lee, M.-L.T.; Xiao, T.; Papp, A.; Wang, D.; et al. Polymorphisms in human dopamine D2 receptor gene affect gene expression, splicing, and neuronal activity during working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 20552–20557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Sambataro, F.; Reed, J.D.; Murty, V.P.; Das, S.; Tan, H.Y.; Callicott, J.H.; Weinberger, D.R.; Mattay, V.S. Catechol-O-Methyltransferase Valine158Methionine Polymorphism Modulates Brain Networks Underlying Working Memory Across Adulthood. Biol. Psychiatry 2009, 66, 540–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Cassidy, C.M.; Van Snellenberg, J.X.; Benavides, C.; Slifstein, M.; Wang, Z.; Moore, H.; Abi-Dargham, A.; Horga, G. Dynamic Connectivity between Brain Networks Supports Working Memory: Relationships to Dopamine Release and Schizophrenia. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 2016, 36, 4377–4388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Kellendonk, C.; Simpson, E.H.; Polan, H.J.; Malleret, G.; Vronskaya, S.; Winiger, V.; Moore, H.; Kandel, E.R. Transient and Selective Overexpression of Dopamine D2 Receptors in the Striatum Causes Persistent Abnormalities in Prefrontal Cortex Functioning. Neuron 2006, 49, 603–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Cools, R.; D’Esposito, M. Dopaminergic Modulation of Flexible Cognitive Control in Humans. In Dopamine Handbook; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2009; pp. 249–260. [Google Scholar]
  6. Cools, R.; D’Esposito, M. Inverted-U–Shaped Dopamine Actions on Human Working Memory and Cognitive Control. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, e113–e125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. D’Esposito, M.; Postle, B.R. The Cognitive Neuroscience of Working Memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015, 66, 115–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Gelao, B.; Fazio, L.; Selvaggi, P.; Di Giorgio, A.; Taurisano, P.; Quarto, T.; Romano, R.; Porcelli, A.; Mancini, M.; Masellis, R.; et al. DRD2 genotype predicts prefrontal activity during working memory after stimulation of D2 receptors with bromocriptine. Psychopharmacology 2014, 231, 2361–2370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Gosso, M.F.; Geus, E.J.C.D.; Polderman, T.J.C.; Boomsma, D.I.; Heutink, P.; Posthuma, D. Catechol O-methyl transferase and dopamine D2 receptor gene polymorphisms: Evidence of positive heterosis and gene–gene interaction on working memory functioning. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 2008, 16, 1075–1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Brozoski, T.J.; Brown, R.M.; Rosvold, H.E.; Goldman, P.S. Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey. Science 1979, 205, 929–932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Naef, M.; Müller, U.; Linssen, A.; Clark, L.; Robbins, T.W.; Eisenegger, C. Effects of dopamine D2/D3 receptor antagonism on human planning and spatial working memory. Transl. Psychiatry 2017, 7, e1107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Sawaguchi, T.; Goldman-Rakic, P.S. D1 dopamine receptors in prefrontal cortex: Involvement in working memory. Science 1991, 251, 947–950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Kimberg, D.; D’Esposito, M. Cognitive effects of the dopamine receptor agonist pergolide. Neuropsychologia 2003, 41, 1020–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Mehta, M.A.; Swainson, R.; Ogilvie, A.D.; Sahakian, B.; Robbins, T.W. Improved short-term spatial memory but impaired reversal learning following the dopamine D2 agonist bromocriptine in human volunteers. Psychopharmacology 2001, 159, 10–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Kimberg, D.; D’Esposito, M.; Farah, M. Effects of bromocriptine on human subjects depend on working memory capacity. Neuroreport 1997, 8, 3581–3585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Luciana, M.; Collins, P.F. Dopaminergic Modulation of Working Memory for Spatial but Not Object Cues in Normal Humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1997, 9, 330–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Müller, U.; von Cramon, D.Y.; Pollmann, S. D1- versus D2-receptor modulation of visuospatial working memory in humans. J. Neurosci. 1998, 18, 2720–2728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Lee, Y.H.; Kim, J.-H.; Song, G.G. Association between the COMT Val158Met polymorphism and fibromyalgia susceptibility and fibromyalgia impact questionnaire score: A meta-analysis. Rheumatol. Int. 2015, 35, 159–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Park, D.J.; Kim, S.H.; Nah, S.S.; Lee, J.H.; Kim, S.K.; Lee, Y.A.; Hong, S.J.; Kim, H.S.; Lee, H.S.; Kim, H.A.; et al. Association between catechol-O-methyl transferase gene polymorphisms and fibromyalgia in a Korean population: A case-control study. Eur. J. Pain 2016, 20, 1131–1139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Markett, S.A.; Montag, C.; Reuter, M. The association between dopamine DRD2 polymorphisms and working memory capacity is modulated by a functional polymorphism on the nicotinic receptor gene CHRNA4. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 22, 1944–1954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Durstewitz, D.; Seamans, J.K. The Dual-State Theory of Prefrontal Cortex Dopamine Function with Relevance to Catechol- O-Methyltransferase Genotypes and Schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 64, 739–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Bannon, M.; Roth, R. Pharmacology of mesocortical dopamine neurons. Pharmacol. Rev. 1983, 35, 53–68. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  23. Bertolino, A.; Rubino, V.; Sambataro, F.; Blasi, G.; Latorre, V.; Fazio, L.; Caforio, G.; Petruzzella, V.; Kolachana, B.; Hariri, A.; et al. Prefrontal-Hippocampal Coupling During Memory Processing Is Modulated by COMT Val158Met Genotype. Biol. Psychiatry 2006, 60, 1250–1258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Smolka, M.N.; Schumann, G.; Wrase, J.; Grüsser, S.M.; Flor, H.; Mann, K.; Braus, D.F.; Goldman, D.; Büchel, C.; Heinz, A. Catechol-O-Methyltransferase val158met Genotype Affects Processing of Emotional Stimuli in the Amygdala and Prefrontal Cortex. J. Neurosci. 2005, 25, 836–842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. García-Fructuoso, F.J.; Lao-Villadóniga, J.I.; Beyer, K.; Santos, C. Relación entre genotipos del gen catecol O-metiltransferasa y la gravedad de la fibromialgia. Reumatol. Clin. 2006, 2, 168–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Nagel, I.E.; Chicherio, C.; Li, S.-C.; von Oertzen, T.; Sander, T.; Villringer, A.; Heekeren, H.R.; Bäckman, L.; Lindenberger, U. Human aging magnifies genetic effects on executive functioning and working memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2008, 2, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Chen, J.; Lipska, B.K.; Halim, N.; Ma, Q.D.; Matsumoto, M.; Melhem, S.; Kolachana, B.S.; Hyde, T.M.; Herman, M.M.; Apud, J.; et al. Functional Analysis of Genetic Variation in Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT): Effects on mRNA, Protein, and Enzyme Activity in Postmortem Human Brain. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2004, 75, 807–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Bruder, G.E.; Keilp, J.G.; Xu, H.; Shikhman, M.; Schori, E.; Gorman, J.M.; Gilliam, T.C. Catechol-O-Methyltransferase (COMT) Genotypes and Working Memory: Associations with Differing Cognitive Operations. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 58, 901–907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bertolino, A.; Blasi, G.; Latorre, V.; Rubino, V.; Rampino, A.; Sinibaldi, L.; Caforio, G.; Petruzzella, V.; Pizzuti, A.; Weinberger, D.R.; et al. Additive Effects of Genetic Variation in Dopamine Regulating Genes on Working Memory Cortical Activity in Human Brain. J. Neurosci. 2006, 26, 3918–3922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Aguilera, M.; Barrantes-Vidal, N.; Arias, B.; Moya, J.; Villa, H.; Ibáñez, M.I.; Ruipérez, M.A.; Ortet, G.; Fañanás, L. Putative role of the COMT gene polymorphism (Val158Met) on verbal working memory functioning in a healthy population. Am. J. Med. Genet. Part B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 2008, 147B, 898–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. de Frias, C.M.; Annerbrink, K.; Westberg, L.; Eriksson, E.; Adolfsson, R.; Nilsson, L.-G. Catechol O-Methyltransferase Val 158 Met Polymorphism is Associated with Cognitive Performance in Nondemented Adults. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 1018–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Meyer-Lindenberg, A.; Nichols, T.; Callicott, J.H.; Ding, J.; Kolachana, B.; Buckholtz, J.; Mattay, V.S.; Egan, M.; Weinberger, D.R. Impact of complex genetic variation in COMT on human brain function. Mol. Psychiatry 2006, 11, 867–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Mattay, V.S.; Goldberg, T.E.; Fera, F.; Hariri, A.R.; Tessitore, A.; Egan, M.F.; Kolachana, B.; Callicott, J.H.; Weinberger, D.R. Catechol O-methyltransferase val158-met genotype and individual variation in the brain response to amphetamine. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2003, 100, 6186–6191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Goldberg, T.E.; Egan, M.F.; Gscheidle, T.; Coppola, R.; Weickert, T.; Kolachana, B.S.; Goldman, D.; Weinberger, D.R. Executive Subprocesses in Working Memory. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2003, 60, 889–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Malhotra, A.K.; Kestler, L.J.; Mazzanti, C.; Bates, J.A.; Goldberg, T.; Goldman, D. A Functional Polymorphism in the COMT Gene and Performance on a Test of Prefrontal Cognition. Am. J. Psychiatry 2002, 159, 652–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Egan, M.F.; Goldberg, T.E.; Kolachana, B.S.; Callicott, J.H.; Mazzanti, C.M.; Straub, R.E.; Goldman, D.; Weinberger, D.R. Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2001, 98, 6917–6922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Matsuzaka, C.T.; Christofolini, D.; Ota, V.K.; Gadelha, A.; Berberian, A.A.; Noto, C.; Mazzotti, D.R.; Spindola, L.M.; Moretti, P.N.; Smith, M.A.C.; et al. Catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) polymorphisms modulate working memory in individuals with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 2017, 39, 302–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Mestrovic, A.H.; Tudor, L.; Nikolac Perkovic, M.; Nedic Erjavec, G.; Kovacic Petrovic, Z.; Svob Strac, D.; Konjevod, M.; Pivac, N. Significant association between catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) Val158/108Met polymorphism and cognitive function in veterans with PTSD. Neurosci. Lett. 2018, 666, 38–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Diaz-Asper, C.M.; Goldberg, T.E.; Kolachana, B.S.; Straub, R.E.; Egan, M.F.; Weinberger, D.R. Genetic Variation in Catechol-O-Methyltransferase: Effects on Working Memory in Schizophrenic Patients, Their Siblings, and Healthy Controls. Biol. Psychiatry 2008, 63, 72–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Tunbridge, E.M.; Harrison, P.J.; Weinberger, D.R. Catechol-o-Methyltransferase, Cognition, and Psychosis: Val158Met and Beyond. Biol. Psychiatry 2006, 60, 141–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Barbosa, F.R.; Matsuda, J.B.; Mazucato, M.; de Castro França, S.; Zingaretti, S.M.; da Silva, L.M.; Martinez-Rossi, N.M.; Júnior, M.F.; Marins, M.; Fachin, A.L. Influence of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene polymorphisms in pain sensibility of Brazilian fibromialgia patients. Rheumatol. Int. 2012, 32, 427–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Cohen, H.; Neumann, L.; Glazer, Y.; Ebstein, R.P.; Buskila, D. The relationship between a common catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) polymorphism val158met and fibromyalgia. Clin. Exp. Rheumatol. 2009, 27, S51–S56. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  43. Martínez-Jauand, M.; Sitges, C.; Rodríguez, V.; Picornell, A.; Ramon, M.; Buskila, D.; Montoya, P. Pain sensitivity in fibromyalgia is associated with catechol-O- methyltransferase (COMT) gene. Eur. J. Pain 2013, 17, 16–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Fernández-De-Las-Peñas, C.; Ambite-Quesada, S.; Gil-Crujera, A.; Cigarán-Méndez, M.; Peñacoba-Puente, C. Catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met polymorphism influences anxiety, depression, and disability, but not pressure pain sensitivity, in women with fibromyalgia syndrome. J. Pain 2012, 13, 1068–1074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Desmeules, J.; Chabert, J.; Rebsamen, M.; Rapiti, E.; Piguet, V.; Besson, M.; Dayer, P.; Cedraschi, C. Central Pain Sensitization, COMT Val158Met Polymorphism, and Emotional Factors in Fibromyalgia. J. Pain 2014, 15, 129–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Kravitz, H.M.; Katz, R.S. Fibrofog and fibromyalgia: A narrative review and implications for clinical practice. Rheumatol. Int. 2015, 35, 1115–1125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Wolfe, F.; Clauw, D.J.; Fitzcharles, M.A.; Goldenberg, D.L.; Katz, R.S.; Mease, P.; Russell, A.S.; Russell, I.J.; Winfield, J.B.; Yunus, M.B. The American College of Rheumatology Preliminary Diagnostic Criteria for Fibromyalgia and Measurement of Symptom Severity. Arthritis Care Res. 2010, 62, 600–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Wolfe, F.; Clauw, D.; Fitzcharles, M.A.; Goldenberg, D.L.; Häuser, W.; Katz, R.; Mease, P.J.; Russell, A.S.; Russell, I.J.; Walitt, B. 2016 Revisions to the 2010/2011 fibromyalgia diagnostic criteria. Semin. Arthritis Rheum. 2016, 46, 319–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Arnold, L.M.; Crofford, L.J.; Mease, P.J.; Burgess, S.M.; Palmer, S.C.; Abetz, L.; Martin, S.A. Patient perspectives on the impact of fibromyalgia. Patient Educ. Couns. 2008, 73, 114–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Borg, C.; Emond, F.C.; Colson, D.; Laurent, B.; Michael, G.A. Attentional focus on subjective interoceptive experience in patients with fibromyalgia. Brain Cogn. 2015, 101, 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. González, J.L.; Mercado, F.; Barjola, P.; Carretero, I.; López-López, A.; Bullones, M.A.; Fernández-Sánchez, M.; Alonso, M. Generalized hypervigilance in fibromyalgia patients: An experimental analysis with the emotional Stroop paradigm. J. Psychosom. Res. 2010, 69, 279–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Carrillo de la Peña, M.T.; Vallet, M.; Pérez, M.I.I.; Gómez-Perretta, C. Intensity Dependence of Auditory-Evoked Cortical Potentials in Fibromyalgia Patients: A Test of the Generalized Hypervigilance Hypothesis. J. Pain 2006, 7, 480–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Leavitt, F.; Katz, R.S. Distraction as a key determinant of impaired memory in patients with fibromyalgia. J. Rheumatol. 2006, 33, 127–132. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  54. Mercado, F.; González, J.L.; Barjola, P.; Fernández-Sánchez, M.; López-López, A.; Alonso, M.; Gómez-Esquer, F. Brain correlates of cognitive inhibition in fibromyalgia: Emotional intrusion of symptom-related words. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013, 88, 182–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Kim, S.-H.; Kim, S.-H.; Kim, S.-K.; Nam, E.J.; Han, S.W.; Lee, S.J. Spatial versus verbal memory impairments in patients with fibromyalgia. Rheumatol. Int. 2012, 32, 1135–1142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Verdejo-García, A.; López-Torrecillas, F.; Calandre, E.P.; Delgado-Rodríguez, A.; Bechara, A. Executive Function and Decision-Making in Women with Fibromyalgia. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2009, 24, 113–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Walteros, C.; Sánchez-Navarro, J.P.; Muñoz, M.A.; Martínez-Selva, J.M.; Chialvo, D.; Montoya, P. Altered associative learning and emotional decision making in fibromyalgia. J. Psychosom. Res. 2011, 70, 294–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Samartin-Veiga, N.; González-Villar, A.J.; Carrillo-de-la-Peña, M.T. Neural correlates of cognitive dysfunction in fibromyalgia patients: Reduced brain electrical activity during the execution of a cognitive control task. NeuroImage Clin. 2019, 23, 101817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Di Tella, M.; Castelli, L.; Colonna, F.; Fusaro, E.; Torta, R.; Ardito, R.B.; Adenzato, M. Theory of mind and emotional functioning in Fibromyalgia syndrome: An investigation of the relationship between social cognition and executive function. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Akdoǧan, S.; Ayhan, F.F.; Yildirim, Ş.; Borman, P. Impact of fatigue on cognitive functioning among premenopausal women with fibromyalgia syndrome and rheumatoid arthritis: The controlled study. J. Musculoskelet. Pain 2013, 21, 135–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Bertolucci, P.H.F.; Oliveira, F.F. Cognitive Impairment in Fibromyalgia. Curr. Pain Headache Rep. 2013, 17, 344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Gelonch, O.; Garolera, M.; Valls, J.; Rosselló, L.; Pifarré, J. Cognitive complaints in women with fibromyalgia: Are they due to depression or to objective cognitive dysfunction? J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2017, 39, 1013–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Tesio, V.; Torta, D.M.E.; Colonna, F.; Leombruni, P.; Ghiggia, A.; Fusaro, E.; Geminiani, G.C.; Torta, R.; Castelli, L. Are Fibromyalgia Patients Cognitively Impaired? Objective and Subjective Neuropsychological Evidence. Arthritis Care Res. 2015, 67, 143–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Wood, P.B.; Glabus, M.F.; Simpson, R.; Patterson, J.C. Changes in Gray Matter Density in Fibromyalgia: Correlation With Dopamine Metabolism. J. Pain 2009, 10, 609–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Owen, A.M.; McMillan, K.M.; Laird, A.R.; Bullmore, E. N-back working memory paradigm: A meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2005, 25, 46–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Bush, G.; Luu, P.; Posner, M.I. Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 215–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Albrecht, D.S.; MacKie, P.J.; Kareken, D.A.; Hutchins, G.D.; Chumin, E.J.; Christian, B.T.; Yoder, K.K. Differential dopamine function in fibromyalgia. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016, 10, 829–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.-G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Spielberger, C.D.; Gorsuch, R.L.; Lushene, R. Manual del Cuestionario de Ansiedad Estado/Rasgo (STAI); TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 1982. [Google Scholar]
  70. Sanz, J.; Perdigón, A.L.; Vázquez, C. Adaptación española del Inventario para la Depresi ó n de Beck-II (BDI-II): 2 Propiedades psicométricas en población general. Clin. y Salud 2003, 14, 249–280. [Google Scholar]
  71. Rivera, J.; González, T. The Fibromyalgia Impact Questionnaire: A validated Spanish version to assess the health status in women with fibromyalgia. Clin. Exp. Rheumatol. 2004, 22, 554–560. [Google Scholar]
  72. Wechsler, D. WMS-III: Escala de memoria de Wechsler-III; TEA: Madrid, Spain, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  73. Iverson, G.L. Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2001, 16, 183–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Tirapu-Ustárroz, J.; Muñoz-Céspedes, J.M.; Pelegrín-Valero, C.; Albéniz-Ferreras, A. Propuesta de un protocolo para la evaluación de las funciones ejecutivas. Rev. Neurol. 2005, 41, 177–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Corsi, P.M. Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Diss. Abstr. Int. 1973, 34, 891. [Google Scholar]
  76. Manglam, M.K.; Ram, D.; Praharaj, S.K.; Sarkhel, S. Working memory in schizophrenia. Ger. J. Psychiatry 2010, 13, 116–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Barrantes-Vidal, N.; Aguilera, M.; Campanera, S.; Fatjó-Vilas, M.; Guitart, M.; Miret, S.; Valero, S.; Fañanás, L. Working memory in siblings of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr. Res. 2007, 95, 70–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Mammarella, I.C.; Cornoldi, C. Sequence and space: The critical role of a backward spatial span in the working memory deficit of visuospatial learning disabled children. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2005, 22, 1055–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Shannon, C.; Douse, K.; McCusker, C.; Feeney, L.; Barrett, S.; Mulholland, C. The association between childhood trauma and memory functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2011, 37, 531–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Gold, J.M. Auditory Working Memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Performance in Schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1997, 54, 159–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. DeCarlo, L.T. On the meaning and use of kurtosis. Psychol. Methods 1997, 2, 292–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Ryu, E. Effects of skewness and kurtosis on normal-theory based maximum likelihood test statistic in multilevel structural equation modeling. Behav. Res. Methods 2011, 43, 1066–1074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Pariente, A.; de Gage, S.B.; Moore, N.; Bégaud, B. The Benzodiazepine–Dementia Disorders Link: Current State of Knowledge. CNS Drugs 2016, 30, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Pidal-Miranda, M.; González-Villar, A.J.; Carrillo-de-la-Peña, M.T.; Andrade, E.; Rodríguez-Salgado, D. Broad cognitive complaints but subtle objective working memory impairment in fibromyalgia patients. PeerJ 2018, 2018, e5907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Veldhuijzen, D.S.; Sondaal, S.F.V.; Oosterman, J.M. Intact cognitive inhibition in patients with fibromyalgia but evidence of declined processing speed. J. Pain 2012, 13, 507–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Suhr, J.A. Neuropsychological impairment in fibromyalgia: Relation to depression, fatigue, and pain. J. Psychosom. Res. 2003, 55, 321–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Shmygalev, S.; Dagtekin, O.; Gerbershagen, H.J.; Marcus, H.; Jübner, M.; Sabatowski, R.; Petzke, F. Assessing Cognitive and Psychomotor Performance in Patients with Fibromyalgia Syndrome. Pain Ther. 2014, 3, 85–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Landrø, N.I.; Stiles, T.C.; Sletvold, H. Memory functioning in patients with primary fibromyalgia and major depression and healthy controls. J. Psychosom. Res. 1997, 42, 297–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Roldán-Tapia, L.; Cánovas-López, R.; Cimadevilla, J.; Valverde, M. Cognition and Perception Deficits in Fibromyalgia and Rheumatoid Arthritis. Reumatol. Clínica (English Ed.) 2007, 3, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Luerding, R.; Weigand, T.; Bogdahn, U.; Schmidt-Wilcke, T. Working memory performance is correlated with local brain morphology in the medial frontal and anterior cingulate cortex in fibromyalgia patients: Structural correlates of pain-cognition interaction. Brain 2008, 131, 3222–3231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Seo, J.; Kim, S.-H.; Kim, Y.-T.; Song, H.; Lee, J.; Kim, S.-H.; Han, S.W.; Nam, E.J.; Kim, S.-K.; Lee, H.J.; et al. Working memory impairment in fibromyalgia patients associated with altered frontoparietal memory network. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e37808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Glass, J.M.; Williams, D.A.; Fernandez-Sanchez, M.L.; Kairys, A.; Barjola, P.; Heitzeg, M.M.; Clauw, D.J.; Schmidt-Wilcke, T. Executive function in chronic pain patients and healthy controls: Different cortical activation during response inhibition in fibromyalgia. J. Pain 2011, 12, 1219–1229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Leavitt, F.; Katz, R.S. Speed of mental operations in fibromyalgia a selective naming speed deficit. J. Clin. Rheumatol. 2008, 14, 214–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Dumontheil, I.; Roggeman, C.; Ziermans, T.; Peyrard-Janvid, M.; Matsson, H.; Kere, J.; Klingberg, T. Influence of the COMT Genotype on Working Memory and Brain Activity Changes During Development. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 70, 222–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  95. Manoach, D.S.; Gollub, R.L.; Benson, E.S.; Searl, M.M.; Goff, D.C.; Halpern, E.; Saper, C.B.; Rauch, S.L. Schizophrenia subjects show fMRI activation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia during a working memory task. Biol. Psychiatry 2000, 48, 99–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Morey, R.A.; Dolcos, F.; Petty, C.M.; Cooper, D.A.; Hayes, J.P.; LaBar, K.S.; McCarthy, G. The role of trauma-related distractors on neural systems for working memory and emotion processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2009, 43, 809–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  97. Chari, S.; Minzenberg, M.J.; Solomon, M.; Ragland, J.D.; Nguyen, Q.; Carter, C.S.; Yoon, J.H. Impaired prefrontal functional connectivity associated with working memory task performance and disorganization despite intact activations in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2019, 287, 10–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Schmidt-Wilcke, T.; Luerding, R.; Weigand, T.; Jürgens, T.; Schuierer, G.; Leinisch, E.; Bogdahn, U. Striatal grey matter increase in patients suffering from fibromyalgia—A voxel-based morphometry study. Pain 2007, 132, S109–S116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Schmidt-Wilcke, T.; Kairys, A.; Ichesco, E.; Fernandez-Sanchez, M.L.; Barjola, P.; Heitzeg, M.; Harris, R.E.; Clauw, D.J.; Glass, J.; Williams, D.A. Changes in Clinical Pain in Fibromyalgia Patients Correlate with Changes in Brain Activation in the Cingulate Cortex in a Response Inhibition Task. Pain Med. 2014, 15, 1346–1358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. De Frias, C.M.; Marklund, P.; Eriksson, E.; Larsson, A.; Öman, L.; Annerbrink, K.; Bäckman, L.; Nilsson, L.G.; Nyberg, L. Influence of COMT gene polymorphism on fMRI-assessed sustained and transient activity during a working memory task. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 22, 1614–1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Savitz, J.; Solms, M.; Ramesar, R. The molecular genetics of cognition: Dopamine, COMT and BDNF. Genes Brain Behav. 2006, 5, 311–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Stahl, S.M. Fibromyalgia-pathways and neurotransmitters. Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 2009, 24, S11–S17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Ceko, M.; Bushnell, M.C.; Gracely, R.H. Neurobiology Underlying Fibromyalgia Symptoms. Pain Res. Treat. 2012, 2012, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Cools, R.; Robbins, T.W. Chemistry of the adaptive mind. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 2004, 362, 2871–2888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Glass, J.M. Review of Cognitive Dysfunction in Fibromyalgia: A Convergence on Working Memory and Attentional Control Impairments. Rheum. Dis. Clin. N. Am. 2009, 35, 299–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Covey, T.J.; Shucard, J.L.; Shucard, D.W. Event-related brain potential indices of cognitive function and brain resource reallocation during working memory in patients with Multiple Sclerosis. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2017, 128, 604–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. López Zunini, R.A.; Knoefel, F.; Lord, C.; Dzuali, F.; Breau, M.; Sweet, L.; Goubran, R.; Taler, V. Event-related potentials elicited during working memory are altered in mild cognitive impairment. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2016, 109, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Shah, P.; Miyake, A. The separability of working memory ressources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approach. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 1996, 125, 4–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Jonides, J.; Reuter-Lorenz, P.A.; Smith, E.E.; Awh, E.; Barnes, L.L.; Drain, M.; Glass, J.; Lauber, E.J.; Patalano, A.L.; Schumacher, E.H. Verbal and Spatial Working Memory In Humans. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. Adv. Res. Theory 1996, 35, 43–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  110. Handley, S.J.; Capon, A.; Copp, C.; Harper, C. Conditional reasoning and the Tower of Hanoi: The role of spatial and verbal working memory. Br. J. Psychol. 2002, 93, 501–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Bell, T.; Trost, Z.; Buelow, M.T.; Clay, O.; Younger, J.; Moore, D.; Crowe, M. Meta-analysis of cognitive performance in fibromyalgia. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2018, 40, 698–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Watter, S.; Heisz, J.J.; Karle, J.W.; Shedden, J.M.; Kiss, I. Modality-specific control processes in verbal versus spatial working memory. Brain Res. 2010, 1347, 90–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Floresco, S.B.; Seamans, J.K.; Phillips, A.G. Selective roles for hippocampal, prefrontal cortical, and ventral striatal circuits in radial-arm maze tasks with or without a delay. J. Neurosci. 1997, 17, 1880–1890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Kodama, T.; Hikosaka, K.; Watanabe, M. Differential changes in glutamate concentration in the primate prefrontal cortex during spatial delayed alternation and sensory-guided tasks. Exp. Brain Res. 2002, 145, 133–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Watanabe, M.; Kodama, T.; Hikosaka, K. Increase of extracellular dopamine in primate prefrontal cortex during a working memory task. J. Neurophysiol. 1997, 78, 2795–2798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Thierry, A.M.; Gioanni, Y.; Dégénétais, E.; Glowinski, J. Hippocampo-prefrontal cortex pathway: Anatomical and electrophysiological characteristics. Hippocampus 2000, 10, 411–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Galvez-Sánchez, C.M.; Reyes del Paso, G.A.; Duschek, S. Cognitive Impairments in Fibromyalgia Syndrome: Associations With Positive and Negative Affect, Alexithymia, Pain Catastrophizing and Self-Esteem. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  118. Reyes del Paso, G.A.; Pulgar, Á.; Duschek, S.; Garrido, S. Cognitive impairment in fibromyalgia syndrome: The impact of cardiovascular regulation, pain, emotional disorders and medication. Eur. J. Pain 2012, 16, 421–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Park, D.C.; Glass, J.M.; Minear, M.; Crofford, L.J. Cognitive Function in Fibromyalgia Patients. Arthritis Rheum. 2001, 44, 2125–2133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  120. Munguía-Izquierdo, D.; Legaz-Arrese, A.; Moliner-Urdiales, D.; Reverter-Masía, J. Neuropsychological performance in patients with fibromyalgia syndrome: Relation to pain and anxiety. Psicothema 2008, 20, 427–431. [Google Scholar]
  121. Gibbs, S.E.B.; D’Esposito, M. Individual capacity differences predict working memory performance and prefrontal activity following dopamine receptor stimulation. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2005, 5, 212–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  122. Bertolino, A.; Taurisano, P.; Pisciotta, N.M.; Blasi, G.; Fazio, L.; Romano, R.; Gelao, B.; Lo Bianco, L.; Lozupone, M.; Di Giorgio, A.; et al. Genetically determined measures of striatal D2 signaling predict prefrontal activity during working memory performance. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e9348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Spatial Span Test. Example of a trial representing a sequence length of three cubes. (A) Trial of the Spatial Span Test in the forward application; (B) Trial of the Spatial Span Test in the backward application. White arrows mark the correct order in which subjects should have remembered the tapping sequence.
Figure 1. Spatial Span Test. Example of a trial representing a sequence length of three cubes. (A) Trial of the Spatial Span Test in the forward application; (B) Trial of the Spatial Span Test in the backward application. White arrows mark the correct order in which subjects should have remembered the tapping sequence.
Jcm 09 02479 g001
Figure 2. Means and standard errors related to the performance in the working memory tests. Data of fibromyalgia patients (red) and healthy controls (blue) are represented for each COMT genotype: Val/Val, Met/Val and Met/Met. (A) SST backward score, (B) span SST backward, (C) SST total score and (D) WMI. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.001. SST, Spatial Span Test; WMI, Working Memory Index; Val, valine; Met, methionine.
Figure 2. Means and standard errors related to the performance in the working memory tests. Data of fibromyalgia patients (red) and healthy controls (blue) are represented for each COMT genotype: Val/Val, Met/Val and Met/Met. (A) SST backward score, (B) span SST backward, (C) SST total score and (D) WMI. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.001. SST, Spatial Span Test; WMI, Working Memory Index; Val, valine; Met, methionine.
Jcm 09 02479 g002
Table 1. Means and standard deviations of age, levels of trait and state anxiety, depression, pain, drug consumption and educational levels for each group of participants. p-values for each statistical contrast are also included.
Table 1. Means and standard deviations of age, levels of trait and state anxiety, depression, pain, drug consumption and educational levels for each group of participants. p-values for each statistical contrast are also included.
Clinical VariablesFibromyalgia PatientsHealthy Controlp-Value
Age51.38 (7.43)51.42 (7.51)0.975
STAI
STAI-Trait65.23 (28.13)34.60 (26.67)0.001
STAI-State43.27 (26.70)29.53 (25.49)0.001
BDI18.69 (9.28)6.47 (4.86)0.001
VAS Pain6.26 (2.08)0.977 (1.69)0.001
FIQ-S55.89 (18.09)--
Drug consumption
Antidepressants (%)53.50.020.001
Analgesics (%)59.300.020.001
Benzodiazepines (%)33.70.020.001
Others (%)5025.930.001
Educational level
Elementary studies (%)16.2816.050.347
Middle level (%)60.4750.62
Superior university studies (%)23.2533.33
STAI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory; VAS, visual analogue scale; FIQ-S, Spanish version of the Fibromyalgia Impact Questionnaire.
Table 2. Allele and genotype frequencies of COMT in fibromyalgia patients and healthy control group.
Table 2. Allele and genotype frequencies of COMT in fibromyalgia patients and healthy control group.
Genotype Frequencies n (%) Allele Frequencies
GenotypesHC (n = 81)Fibromyalgia (n = 86) HC (n = 81)Fibromyalgia (n = 86)
Val/Val 28 (19.8)22 (25.6)Val0.490.43
Met/Val37 (45.7)44 (51.2)Met0.510.57
Met/Met 16 (34.6)20 (23.3)
HC, healthy control participants; Val, valine; Met, methionine.
Table 3. Mean scores and standard deviations (in parenthesis) for working memory measures. Data are separated by group (fibromyalgia and HC) and genotype (Val/Val, Met/Val and Met/Met). p-values for the interaction between the COMT genotypes and group are also included.
Table 3. Mean scores and standard deviations (in parenthesis) for working memory measures. Data are separated by group (fibromyalgia and HC) and genotype (Val/Val, Met/Val and Met/Met). p-values for the interaction between the COMT genotypes and group are also included.
COMTp-Value
Healthy ControlFibromyalgia
TestVal/ValMet/ValMet/MetVal/ValMet/ValMet/Met
Span of LNST5.04 (0.88)5.08 (0.89)5.38 (0.62)4.77 (1.11)5.09 (1.15)4.80 (0.91)0.327
LNST score10 (2.35)10.05 (2.38)10.75 (1.34)9.05 (2.75)9.86 (2.86)8.75 (2.26)0.191
Span of SST forward5.79 (0.78)5.89 (0.77)5.69 (0.87)5.14 (1.20)5.70 (1.01)5.30 (1.12)0.412
SST forward score8.57 (1.42)8.43 (1.55)8.63 (1.66)7.36 (1.98))8.39 (1.88)7.70 (1.83)0.147
Span of SST backward5.79 (1.97)5.16 (1.11)5.35 (1.06)4.77 (1.06)5.25 (0.94)4.95 (1.27)0.020
SST backward score8.21 (1.72)7.27 (1.82)7.25 (1.73)6.14 (1.91)7.39 (1.82)6.80 (2.01)0.005
SST total score16.79 (2.58)15.62 (3.32)15.87 (2.96)13.50 (3.48)15.77 (3.32)14.50 (2.87)0.008
WMI108.63 (10.06)103.11 (12.69)108.54 (12.21)98.92 (14.95)104.23 (14.75)97.45 (13.29)0.018
LNST, Letter and Number Sequence Test; SST, Spatial Span Test; WMI, Working Memory Index.
Table 4. Pearson correlations between the clinical questionnaire scores and neuropsychological measures.
Table 4. Pearson correlations between the clinical questionnaire scores and neuropsychological measures.
State AnxietyTrait AnxietyDepressionPain
Span of LNST−0.057−0.051−0.093−0.068
LNST score−0.087−0.095−0.157 *−0.137
Span of SST forward−0.083−0.219 **−0.129−0.157 *
SST forward score−0.116−0.209 **−0.134−0.121
Span of SST backward−0.143−0.206 **−0.150−0.119
SST backward score−0.085−0.133−0.130−0.146
SST total score−0.1140.192*−0.148−0.149
WMI−0.012−0.131−0.166 *−0.117
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 5. Means and standard deviations (in parenthesis) of working memory outcomes for fibromyalgia patients who were taking or not taking medications (benzodiazepines and antidepressants). p-values of the one way-ANOVAs are also included.
Table 5. Means and standard deviations (in parenthesis) of working memory outcomes for fibromyalgia patients who were taking or not taking medications (benzodiazepines and antidepressants). p-values of the one way-ANOVAs are also included.
Antidepressant ANOVAsBenzodiazepine ANOVAs
Medication No Medicationp-ValueMedication No Medicationp-Value
Span of LNST4.98 (0.97)4.90 (1.236)0.7444.83 (0.92)5.00 (1.18)0.495
LNST score9.43 (2.50)9.35 (2.99)0.8878.97 (2.42)9.61 (2.85)0.299
Span of SST forward 5.61 (0.97)5.30 (1.22)0.1975.59 (1.01)5.40 (1.14)0.471
SST forward score8.24 (1.66)7.65 (2.17)0.1598.14 (1.66)7.88 (2.06)0.557
Span of SST backward5.15 (1.13)4.95 (0.98)0.3845.14 (0.95)5.02 (1.12)0.624
SST backward score7.17 (1.99)6.65 (1.86)0.2137.10 (1.67)6.84 (2.06)0.558
SST total score15.41 (2.98)14.30 (3.71)0.12715.24 (2.98)14.72 (3.56)0.500
WMI101.63 (13.31)100.43 (16.25)0.706100.31 (13.29)101.46 (15.43)0.734

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ferrera, D.; Gómez-Esquer, F.; Peláez, I.; Barjola, P.; Fernandes-Magalhaes, R.; Carpio, A.; De Lahoz, M.E.; Díaz-Gil, G.; Mercado, F. Effects of COMT Genotypes on Working Memory Performance in Fibromyalgia Patients. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 2479. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9082479

AMA Style

Ferrera D, Gómez-Esquer F, Peláez I, Barjola P, Fernandes-Magalhaes R, Carpio A, De Lahoz ME, Díaz-Gil G, Mercado F. Effects of COMT Genotypes on Working Memory Performance in Fibromyalgia Patients. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2020; 9(8):2479. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9082479

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ferrera, David, Francisco Gómez-Esquer, Irene Peláez, Paloma Barjola, Roberto Fernandes-Magalhaes, Alberto Carpio, María E. De Lahoz, Gema Díaz-Gil, and Francisco Mercado. 2020. "Effects of COMT Genotypes on Working Memory Performance in Fibromyalgia Patients" Journal of Clinical Medicine 9, no. 8: 2479. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9082479

APA Style

Ferrera, D., Gómez-Esquer, F., Peláez, I., Barjola, P., Fernandes-Magalhaes, R., Carpio, A., De Lahoz, M. E., Díaz-Gil, G., & Mercado, F. (2020). Effects of COMT Genotypes on Working Memory Performance in Fibromyalgia Patients. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 9(8), 2479. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9082479

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop