Grounding Intuitive Physics in Perceptual Experience
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. A Brief Historical Overview of the Study of Intuitive Physics
3. A Review of Studies on How Realism Augments Performance in Intuitive Physics Tasks
4. Exploring Studies Challenging the Notion That Realism Eliminates Fundamental Misconceptions
5. The Role of Heuristics in Laypeople’s Understanding of the Physical World
6. Current Controversies and Future Directions
7. Summary
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | The relationship between object speed and the resistance offered by an object to an applied force is thought to be inversely proportional. This notion helps shed light on why children often hold the belief that when a fixed force is applied to two objects initially at rest on a horizontal plane, the lighter object will move faster than the heavier one, and the smallest object will move faster than the larger one (Hast and Howe 2012). |
References
- Anderson, Norman H. 1983. Intuitive physics: Understanding and learning of physical relations. In Perception, Cognition, and Development. Edited by Thomas J. Tighe and Bryan E. Shepp. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 231–65. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson, Norman H., and Friedrich Wilkening. 2014. Adaptive thinking in intuitive physics. In Contributions to Information Integration Theory. Edited by Norman H. Anderson. New York: Psychology Press, vol. 3, pp. 1–42. [Google Scholar]
- Baillargeon, Renée. 2002. The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy: A summary in eight lessons. In Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Edited by Usha C. Goswami. Malden: Blackwell, pp. 349–70. [Google Scholar]
- Bass, Ilona, Kevin A. Smith, Elizabeth Bonawitz, and Tomer D. Ullman. 2021. Partial mental simulation explains fallacies in physical reasoning. Cognitive Neuropsychology 38: 413–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bates, Christopher J., Ilker Yildirim, Joshua B. Tenenbaum, and Peter W. Battaglia. 2015. Humans predict liquid dynamics using probabilistic simulation. In Proceedings of the 37th Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Edited by David C. Noelle, Rick Dale, Anne Warlamount, Jeffrey Yoshimi, Teenie Matlock, Carolyn Jennings and Paul P. Maglio. Austin: Cognitive Science Society, pp. 172–77. [Google Scholar]
- Battaglia, Peter W., Jessica B. Hamrick, and Joshua B. Tenenbaum. 2013. Simulation as an engine of physical scene understanding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110: 18327–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baurès, Robin, Nicholas Benguigui, Michel-Ange Amorim, and Isabelle A. Siegler. 2007. Intercepting free falling objects: Better use Occam’s razor than internalize Newton’s law. Vision Research 47: 2982–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bertamini, Marco, Alice Spooner, and Heiko Hecht. 2003a. Naïve optics: Predicting and perceiving reflections in mirrors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 29: 982–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bertamini, Marco, and Roberto Casati. 2009. False beliefs and naive beliefs: They can be good for you. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32: 512–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bertamini, Marco, Rebecca Lawson, Luke Jones, and Madeline Winters. 2010. The Venus effect in real life and in photographs. Attention Perception & Psychophysics 72: 1948–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bertamini, Marco, Richard Latto, and Alice Spooner. 2003b. The Venus effect: People’s understanding of mirror reflections in paintings. Perception 32: 593–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, and Ugo Savardi. 2012. What fits in into a mirror: Naïve beliefs on the field of view of mirrors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 38: 1144–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, and Ugo Savardi. 2014. Grounding naive physics and optics in perception. The Baltic International Yearbook for Cognition Logic and Communication. Perception and Concepts 9: 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianchi, Ivana, Marco Bertamini, and Ugo Savardi. 2015. Differences between predictions of how a reflection behaves based on the behaviour of an object, and how an object behaves based on the behaviour of its reflection. 2015. Acta Psychologica 161: 54–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blanco, Fernando, Helena Matute, and Miguel A. Vadillo. 2013. Interactive effects of the probability of the cue and the probability of the outcome on the overestimation of null contingency. Learning & Behavior 41: 333–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bozzi, Paolo. 1958. Analisi fenomenologica del moto pendolare armónico (Phenomenological analysis of pendular harmonic motion). Rivista di Psicologia 52: 281–30. [Google Scholar]
- Bozzi, Paolo. 1959. Le condizioni del movimento “naturale” lungo i piani inclinati (The conditions for “natural” motion along incline). Rivista di Psicologia 53: 337–52. [Google Scholar]
- Bozzi, Paolo. 1989. Sulla preistoria della fisica ingenua (On the prehistory of naïve physics). Sistemi Intelligenti 1: 61–74. [Google Scholar]
- Bressan, Paola, and Paolo Gaudiano. 2019. Phenomenological analysis of pendular harmonic motion and the conditions for “natural” motion along incline. In Paolo Bozzi’s Experimental Phenomenology. Edited by Ivana Bianchi and Richard Davies. London: Routledge, pp. 213–42. [Google Scholar]
- Caramazza, Alfonso, Michael McCloskey, and Bert Green. 1981. Naive beliefs in “sophisticated” subjects: Misconceptions about trajectories of objects. Cognition 9: 117–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carey, Susan. 1989. Cognitive science and science education. American Psychologist 41: 1123–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Champagne, Audrey B., Leopold E. Klopfer, and John H. Anderson. 1980. Factors influencing the learning of classical mechanics. American Journal of Physics 48: 1074–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clement, John. 1982. Students’ preconceptions in introductory mechanics. American Journal of Physics 50: 66–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corneli, Emanuela, and Michele Vicovaro. 2007. Intuitive cognitive algebra of sliding friction. Teorie & Modelli 12: 133–42. [Google Scholar]
- Croucher, Camilla J., Marco Bertamini, and Heiko Hecht. 2002. Naïve optics: Understanding the geometry of mirror reflections. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28: 546–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Sà Teixeira, Nuno A., Armando M. Oliveira, and Ana Duarte Silva. 2014. An information integration study on the intuitive physics of the Newton’s cradle. Psicológica 35: 479–502. [Google Scholar]
- DiSessa, Andrea A. 1993. Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction 10: 105–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ehrhardt, Sébastien, Aron Monszpart, and Andrea Vedaldi. 2018. Unsupervised intuitive physics from visual observation. arXiv arXiv:1805.05086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fischer, Jason. 2021. The building blocks of intuitive physics in the mind and brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology 38: 409–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fischer, Jason, and Bradford Z. Mahon. 2021. What tool representation, intuitive physics, and action have in common: The brain’s first-person physics engine. Cognitive Neuropsychology 38: 455–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gerstenberg, Tobias, Noah Goodman, David A. Lagnado, and Joshua B. Tenenbaum. 2012. Noisy Newtons: Unifying process and dependency accounts of causal attribution. In Proceedings of the 34th Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Edited by Naomi Miyake, David Peebles and Richard P. Cooper. Austin: Cognitive Science Society, pp. 378–83. [Google Scholar]
- Gilden, David L., and Dennis R. Proffitt. 1994. Heuristic judgment of mass ratio in two-body collisions. Perception & Psychophysics 56: 708–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hafri, Alon, and Chaz Firestone. 2021. The perception of relations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 25: 775–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halloun, Ibrahim A., and David Hestenes. 1985. Common sense concepts about motion. American Journal of Physics 53: 1056–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hamrick, Jessica, Peter Battaglia, and Joshua B. Tenenbaum. 2011. Internal physics models guide probabilistic judgments about object dynamics. In Proceedings of the 33rd Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Edited by Laura Carlson, Christoph Holscher and Thomas Shipley. Austin: Cognitive Science Society, pp. 1545–50. [Google Scholar]
- Hast, Michael. 2014. Exploring the shift in children’s motion predictions: Fragmentation and integration of knowledge as possible contributors. Journal of Educational and Developmental Psychology 4: 74–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hast, Michael. 2016. Children’s reasoning about rolling down curves: Arguing the case for a two-component commonsense theory of motion. Science Education 100: 837–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hast, Michael, and Christine Howe. 2012. Understanding the beliefs informing children’s commonsense theories of motion: The role of everyday object variables in dynamic event predictions. Research in Science & Technological Education 30: 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hast, Michael, and Christine Howe. 2015. Children’s predictions and recognition of fall: The role of object mass. Cognitive Development 36: 103–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hast, Michael, and Christine Howe. 2017. Changing predictions, stable recognition: Children’s representations of downward incline motion. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 35: 516–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hecht, Heiko. 2001. Regularities of the physical world and the absence of their internalization. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 608–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hecht, Heiko. 2015. Beyond illusions—On the limitations of perceiving relational properties. In Open MIND: 18(T). Edited by Thomas Metzinger and Jennifer M. Windt. Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group, pp. 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hecht, Heiko, and Dennis R. Proffitt. 1995. The price of expertise: Effects of experience on the water-level task. Psychological Science 6: 90–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hecht, Heiko, and Marco Bertamini. 2000. Understanding projectile acceleration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26: 730–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hecht, Heiko, Marco Bertamini, and Matthias Gamer. 2005. Naive optics: Acting on mirror reflections. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 31: 1023–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hill, Julia L., Anne Schlottmann, Michelle R. Ellefson, Keith S. Taber, Venus W. S. Tse, and Tiffany S. W. Yung. 2014. Early understanding of intensive properties of matter: Developmental and cultural differences. In Proceedings of the 36th Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Edited by Paul Bello, Marcello Guarini, Marjorie McShane and Brian Scassellati. Austin: Cognitive Science Society, pp. 2357–62. [Google Scholar]
- Hubbard, Timothy L. 2022. The possibility of an impetus transmission heuristic. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 29: 2015–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hubbard, Timothy L., and Susan E. Ruppel. 2013. Ratings of causality and force in launching and shattering. Visual Cognition 21: 987–1009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huber, Susanne, and Horst Krist. 2004. When is the ball going to hit the ground? Duration estimates, eye movements, and mental imagery of object motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 30: 431–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huber, Susanne, Horst Krist, and Friedrich Wilkening. 2003. Judgment of action knowledge in speed adjustment tasks: Experiments in a virtual environment. Developmental Science 6: 197–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jäger, Stephan, and Friedrich Wilkening. 2001. Development of cognitive averaging: When light and light make dark. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 79: 323–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jörges, Björn, and Joan López-Moliner. 2019. Earth-gravity congruent motion facilitates ocular control for pursuit of parabolic trajectories. Scientific Reports 9: 14094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kaiser, Mary K., Dennis R. Proffitt, and Kenneth Anderson. 1985a. Judgments of natural and anomalous trajectories in the presence and absence of motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 11: 795–803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kaiser, Mary K., Dennis R. Proffitt, and Michael McCloskey. 1985b. The development of beliefs about falling objects. Perception & Psychophysics 38: 533–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaiser, Mary K., Dennis R. Proffitt, and Michael McCloskey. 1986a. Development of intuitive theories of motion: Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces. Developmental Psychology 22: 67–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaiser, Mary K., Dennis R. Proffitt, Susan M. Whelan, and Heiko Hecht. 1992. Influence of animation on dynamical judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18: 669–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaiser, Mary K., John Jonides, and Joanne Alexander. 1986b. Intuitive reasoning about abstract and familiar physics problems. Memory & Cognition 14: 308–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karpp, Edward R., and Norman H. Anderson. 1997. Cognitive assessment of function knowledge. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 34: 359–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klaassen, Keess. 2005. The concept of force as a constitutive element of understanding the world. In Research and the Quality of Science Education. Edited by Kerst Boersma, Martin Goedhart, Onno de Jong and Harrie Eijkelhof. Dordecht: Springer, pp. 447–57. [Google Scholar]
- Krist, Horst. 2000. Development of naive beliefs about moving objects: The straight-down belief in action. Cognitive Development 15: 281–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krist, Horst. 2003. Knowing how to project objects: Probing the generality of children’s action knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Development 4: 383–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krist, Horst, Edgar L. Fieberg, and Friedrich Wilkening. 1993. Intuitive physics in action and judgment: The development of knowledge about projectile motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 19: 952–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kubricht, James R., Keith J. Holyoak, and Hongjing Lu. 2017. Intuitive physics: Current research and controversies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21: 749–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lacquaniti, Francesco, and Claudio Maioli. 1989. The role of preparation in tuning anticipatory and reflex responses during catching. Journal of Neuroscience 9: 134–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lau, Jonas S.-H., and Timothy F. Brady. 2020. Noisy perceptual expectations: Multiple object tracking benefits when objects obey features of realistic physics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 46: 1280–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lawson, Rebecca, and Marco Bertamini. 2006. Errors in judging information about reflections on mirrors. Perception 35: 1265–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lawson, Rebecca, Marco Bertamini, and Dan Liu. 2007. Overestimation of the projected size of objects on the surface of mirrors and windows. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 33: 1027–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Léoni, Véronique, and Etienne Mullet. 1993. Evolution of the intuitive mastery of the relation-ships between mass, volume, and density from nursery school to college. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs 119: 389–412. [Google Scholar]
- Levelt, W. M. J. 1962. Motion braking and the perception of causality. In Causalité, Permanence et Réalité Phénoménales. Edited by Albert Michotte. Louvain: Publications Universitaires de Louvain, Studia Psychologica, pp. 244–58. [Google Scholar]
- Little, Patrick C., and Chaz Firestone. 2021. Physically implied surfaces. Psychological Science 32: 799–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Liu, Yaxin, Vladislav Ayzenberg, and Stella F. Lourenco. 2023. Object geometry serves humans’ intuitive physics of stability. PsyArXiv. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ludwin-Peery, Ethan, Neil R. Bramley, Ernest Davis, and Todd M. Guerckis. 2021a. Limits on simulation approaches in intuitive physics. Cognitive Psychology 127: 101396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ludwin-Peery, Ethan, Neil R. Bramley, Ernest Davis, and Todd M. Guerckis. 2021b. Broken physics: A conjunction-fallacy effect in intuitive physical reasoning. Psychological Science 31: 1602–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Masin, Sergio Cesare, Francesco Crivellaro, and Diego Varotto. 2014. The intuitive physics of the equilibrium of the lever and of the hydraulic pressures: Implications for the teaching of elementary physics. Psicológica 35: 441–61. [Google Scholar]
- McCloskey, Michael. 1983. Intuitive physics. Scientific American 248: 122–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCloskey, Michael, Alfonso Caramazza, and Bert Green. 1980. Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces: Naïve beliefs about the motion of objects. Science 210: 1139–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCloskey, Michael, Allyson Washburn, and Linda Felch. 1983. Intuitive physics: The straight-down belief and its origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 9: 636–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCloskey, Michael, and Deborah Kohl. 1983. Naive physics: The curvilinear impetus principle and its role in interactions with moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 9: 146–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDermott, Lillian C. 1991. What we teach and what is learned: Closing the gap. American Journal of Physics 59: 301–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McIntyre, Joseph, Myrka Zago, Alain Berthoz, and Francesco Lacquaniti. 2001. Does the brain model Newton’s laws? Nature Neuroscience 4: 693–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Michotte, Albert. 1963. The Perception of Causality. London: Methuen. First published 1946. [Google Scholar]
- Neupärtl, Nils, Fabian Tatai, and Constantin A. Rothkopf. 2021. Naturalistic embodied interactions elicit intuitive physical behaviour in accordance with Newtonian physics. Cognitive Neuropsychology 38: 440–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oberle, Crystal D., Michael K. McBeath, Sean C. Madigan, and Thomas G. Sugar. 2005. The Galileo bias: A naive conceptual belief that influences people’s perceptions and performance in a ball-dropping task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31: 643–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parovel, Giulia. 2023. Perceiving animacy from kinematics: Visual specification of life-likeness in simple geometric patterns. Frontiers in Psychology 14: 1167809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paulun, Vivian C., and Roland W. Fleming. 2020. Visually inferring elasticity from the motion trajectory of bouncing cubes. Journal of Vision 20: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piloto, Luis S., Ari Weinstein, Peter Battaglia, and Matthew Botvinick. 2022. Intuitive physics learning in a deep-learning model inspired by developmental psychology. Nature Human Behavior 6: 1257–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pittenger, John B. 1990. Detection of violations of the law of pendulum motion: Observers’ sensitivity to the relation between period and length. Ecological Psychology 2: 55–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proffitt, Dennis R., Mary K. Kaiser, and Susan M. Whelan. 1990. Understanding wheel dynamics. Cognitive Psychology 22: 342–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rohrer, Doug. 2002. Misconceptions about incline speed for nonlinear slopes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28: 963–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rohrer, Doug. 2003. The natural appearance of unnatural incline speed. Memory & Cognition 31: 816–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Runeson, Sverker. 1983. On visual perception of dynamic events. In Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis: Studia Psychologica Upsaliensia, Serial No. 9. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell. [Google Scholar]
- Runeson, Sverker, and Dankert Vedeler. 1993. The indispensability of precollision kinematics in the visual perception of relative mass. Perception & Psychophysics 53: 617–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Runeson, Sverker, Peter Juslin, and Henrik Olsson. 2000. Visual perception of dynamic properties: Cue heuristics versus direct-perceptual competence. Psychological Review 107: 525–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanborn, Adam N., Vikash K. Mansinghka, and Thomas L. Griffiths. 2013. Reconciling intuitive physics and Newtonian mechanics for colliding objects. Psychological Review 120: 411–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Savardi, Ugo, Ivana Bianchi, and Marco Bertamini. 2010. Naive prediction of orientation and motion in mirrors. From what we see to what we expect reflections to do. Acta Psychologica 134: 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schwartz, Daniel L., and Tamara Black. 1999. Inferences through imagined actions: Knowing by simulated doing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 25: 116–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sequeira, Manuel, and Laurinda Leite. 1991. Alternative conceptions and history of science in physics teacher education. Science Education 75: 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shaffer, Dennis M., and Michael K. McBeath. 2005. Naive beliefs in baseball: Systematic distortion in perceived time of apex for fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 31: 1492–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shanon, Benny. 1976. Aristotelianism, Newtonianism, and the physics of the layman. Perception 5: 241–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, Kevin A., and Edward Vul. 2013. Sources of uncertainty in intuitive physics. Topics in Cognitive Sciences 5: 185–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, Kevin A., Peter Battaglia, and Edward Vul. 2013. Consistent physics underlying ballistic motion prediction. In Proceedings of the 35th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Edited by Markus Knauff, Michael Pauen, Natalie Sebanz and Ipke Wachsmuth. Austin: Cognitive Science Society, pp. 3426–31. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, Kevin A., Peter Battaglia, and Edward Vul. 2018. Different physical intuitions exist between tasks, not domains. Computational Brain & Behavior 1: 101–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. 1974. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases: Biases in judgments reveal some heuristics of thinking under uncertainty. Science 185: 1124–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Twardy, Charles R., and Geoffrey P. Bingham. 2002. Causation, causal perception, and conservation laws. Perception & Psychophysics 64: 956–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ullman, Tomer D., Elizabeth Spelke, Peter Battaglia, and Joshua B. Tenenbaum. 2017. Mind games: Game engines as an architecture for intuitive physics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21: 649–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vicovaro, Michele. 2012. Intuitive physics of collision effects on simulated spheres differing in size, velocity, and material. Psicológica 33: 451–71. [Google Scholar]
- Vicovaro, Michele. 2014. Intuitive physics of free fall: An information-integration approach to the mass-speed belief. Psicológica 35: 463–77. [Google Scholar]
- Vicovaro, Michele. 2018. Causal reports: Context-dependent contribution of intuitive physics and visual impressions of launching. Acta Psychologica 186: 133–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicovaro, Michele. 2021. Intuitive physics and cognitive algebra: A review. European Review of Applied Psychology 71: 100610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicovaro, Michele, and Luigi Burigana. 2014. Intuitive understanding of the relation between velocities and masses in simulated collisions. Visual Cognition 22: 896–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicovaro, Michele, and Luigi Burigana. 2016. Intuitive understanding of the relationship between the elasticity of objects and kinematic patterns of collisions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 78: 618–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicovaro, Michele, Loris Brunello, and Giulia Parovel. 2023. The psychophysics of bouncing: Perceptual constraints, physical constraints, animacy, and phenomenal causality. PLoS ONE. in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicovaro, Michele, Ludovic Hoyet, Luigi Burigana, and Carol O’Sullivan. 2012. Evaluating the plausibility of edited throwing animations. Paper presented at ACM SIG-GRAPH/Eurographics Symposium on Computer Animation (SCA’12), Lausanne, Switzerland, July 29–31; pp. 175–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicovaro, Michele, Ludovic Hoyet, Luigi Burigana, and Carol O’Sullivan. 2014. Perceptual evaluation of motion editing for realistic throwing animations. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception 11: 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicovaro, Michele, Stefano Noventa, and Luca Battaglini. 2019. Intuitive physics of gravitational motion as shown by perceptual judgment and prediction-motion tasks. Acta Psychologica 194: 51–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicovaro, Michele, Stefano Noventa, Andrea Ghiani, Federica Mena, and Luca Battaglini. 2021. Evidence of weight-based representations of gravitational motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 47: 1445–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Warren, William H., Elizabeth E. Kim, and Robin Husney. 1987. The way the ball bounces: Visual and auditory perception of elasticity and control of the bounce pass. Perception 16: 309–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whitaker, Robert J. 1983. Aristotle is not dead: Student understanding of trajectory motion. American Journal of Physics 51: 352–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, Peter A. 2007. Impressions of force in visual perception of collision events: A test of the causal asymmetry hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 14: 647–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, Peter A. 2009. Perception of forces exerted by objects in collision events. Psychological Review 116: 580–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, Peter A. 2015. Visual impressions of generative transmission. Visual Cognition 23: 1168–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, Peter A., and Alan Milne. 1997. Phenomenal causality: Impressions of pulling in the visual perception of objects in motion. American Journal of Psychology 110: 573–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, Peter A., and Alan Milne. 1999. Impressions of enforced disintegration and bursting in the visual perception of collision events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128: 499–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, Peter A., and Alan Milne. 2003. Visual impressions of penetration in the perception of objects in motion. Visual Cognition 10: 605–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilkening, Friedrich. 1981. Integrating velocity, time, and distance information: A developmental study. Cognitive Psychology 13: 231–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilkening, Friedrich, and Claudia Martin. 2004. How to speed up to be in time: Action-judgment dissociations in children and adults. Swiss Journal of Psychology 63: 17–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wong, Kimberly W., Wenyan Bi, Amir A. Soltani, Ilker Yildrim, and Brian J. Scholl. 2023. Seeing soft materials draped over objects: A case study of intuitive physics in perception, attention, and memory. Psychological Science 34: 111–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yates, Jack, Margaret Bessman, Martin Dunne, Deeann Jertson, Kaye Sly, and Bradley Wendelboe. 1988. Are conceptions of motion based on a naive theory or on prototypes? Cognition 29: 251–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zago, Myrka, and Francesco Lacquaniti. 2005. Cognitive, perceptual and action-oriented representations of falling objects. Neuropsychologia 43: 178–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zago, Myrka, Joseph McIntyre, Patrice Senot, and Francesco Lacquaniti. 2008. Internal models and prediction of visual gravitational motion. Vision Research 48: 1532–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhao, Huaiyong, and William H. Warren. 2015. On-line and model-based approaches to the visual control of action. Vision Research 110 Pt B: 190–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Vicovaro, M. Grounding Intuitive Physics in Perceptual Experience. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 187. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11100187
Vicovaro M. Grounding Intuitive Physics in Perceptual Experience. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(10):187. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11100187
Chicago/Turabian StyleVicovaro, Michele. 2023. "Grounding Intuitive Physics in Perceptual Experience" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 10: 187. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11100187
APA StyleVicovaro, M. (2023). Grounding Intuitive Physics in Perceptual Experience. Journal of Intelligence, 11(10), 187. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11100187