Spatial Visualization Supports Students’ Math: Mechanisms for Spatial Transfer
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Intervention Studies
1.2. The Nature of Transfer
1.3. Spatial Visualization
1.3.1. Reflection and Symmetry
1.3.2. 2D to 3D Transformations
1.4. The Present Study
- To what extent do the spatial training interventions (i.e., the isolated and embedded training programs) facilitate near transfer (to spatial skills) and far transfer (mathematics understandings)?
- If transfer occurs, can any transfer mechanisms be determined from the different training approaches?
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Study Design
2.2.1. The Business-as-Usual Group (Group A)
2.2.2. The Isolated Intervention Group (Group B)
2.2.3. The Embedded Intervention Group (Group C)
2.3. Data Gathering Instruments and Procedures
2.3.1. Spatial Reasoning
2.3.2. Mathematics
2.3.3. Test Administration
2.3.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Trained Spatial Skills as a Mediator
3.2. Spatial Skill Level as a Moderator
4. Discussion
5. Implications and Directions for Future Research
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
ELPSA Framework | Student Work Samples | Student Voice |
---|---|---|
Experience: What is symmetry? Students begin with more familiar reflections of letters and symbols along the y and x axes. They are then asked to consider reflections of similar letters and symbols along the diagonal axis. | “Symmetry is something like butterfly wings, they look the same on both sides and it might be a shape that is a mirror shape that is rotated” | |
Language: What are the language conventions associated with symmetry? Line of symmetry; reflections; mirror image; visualize; fold; y and x axes; diagonal axis; vertical; horizontal; imagine→predict→experiment→check; | “How to flip it on the y or x axis so I was trying to visualize the mirror” “Today we revised on our spatial awareness. I understand now what the y and x axes are. The y axis is when it goes these ways and the x axis when it goes these ways ” | |
Pictorial: Students represented their ideas about lines of symmetry in a drawing. They are required to draw a square with letters in the bottom left corner and a vertical (y axis) line of symmetry. Teacher example below. Move onto horizontal (x axis) line of symmetry and then diagonal line of symmetry. | “I was imagining a mirror reflecting the picture” “I was imagining in my mind the paper folding and where it would print” “I worked it out by picturing it in my mind and trying to think of the page was folded diagonally and thinking what the “T” was doing during that time” “I was visualizing if I flipped it, what shape would it make and where would it be?” “When I was doing this activity I was flipping the images in my mind to get the reflections correct. I was also imagining a mirror on the line so I could draw the reflections correctly.” | |
Symbolic: Symbolic stage requires analytic thinking. Here, students need to recognize conventions associated with lines of reflection on vertical, horizontal, and diagonal axes. Students begin to reason that for reflections on the x and y axes, horizontal stays horizontal and vertical stays vertical. However, with diagonal reflections, horizontal moves to vertical and vertical moves to horizontal. See images below for the concept of perpendicularity. | “I did have to think a bit harder to remember that if the curve in the umbrella on the left is facing left, then the one on the right will be opposite.” “I was imagining a mirror on the fold of the page. Using visual measurements to make it as accurate as possible” “When I was doing the activity I remembered that we talked about the arrow and how it went from ↑ to ←.” “I used my hand a first then I realised I could just reflect then rotate which was much easier.” (diagonal reflection) |
References
- Atit, Kinnari, and Kelsey Rocha. 2020. Examining the relations between spatial skills, spatial anxiety, and K-12 teacher practice. Mind Brain and Education 15: 139–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Australian Curriculum, Assessment, and Reporting Authority (ACARA). 2022. Australian Curriculum: Mathematics Version 8.4. Available online: https://www.australiancurriculum.edu.au/f-10-curriculum/ (accessed on 16 June 2023).
- Battista, Michael T., Leah M. Frazee, and Michael L. Winer. 2018. Analyzing the relation between spatial and geometric reasoning for elementary and middle school students. In Visualizing Mathematics: The Role of Spatial Reasoning in Mathematical Thought. Edited by Kelly S. Mix and Michael T. Battista. Switzerland: Springer, pp. 195–228. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett, George K., Harold G. Seashore, and Alexander G. Wesman. 1947. Differential Aptitude Tests. New York: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Bower, Corinne A., and Lynn S. Liben. 2021. Can a domain-general spatial intervention facilitate children’s science learning? A Lesson from Astronomy. Child Development 92: 76–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bower, Corinne A., Lindsey Foster, Laura Zimmermann, Brian N. Verdine, Maya Marzouk, Siffat Islam, Roberta M. Golinkoff, and Kathy Hirsh-Pasek. 2020. Three-year-olds’ spatial language comprehension and links with mathematics and spatial performance. Developmental Psychology 56: 1894–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Burte, Heather, Aaron L. Gardony, Allyson Hutton, and Holly A. Taylor. 2019. Make-A-dice test: Assessing the intersection of mathematical and spatial thinking. Behavior Research Methods 51: 602–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Chen, Yun-Zu, and Kai-Lin Yang. 2023. Variables for designing cube folding tasks influencing sixth-graders’ performance. Applied Cognitive Psychology 37: 137–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, Yi-Ling, and Kelly S. Mix. 2014. Spatial training improves children’s mathematics ability. Journal of Cognition and Development 15: 2–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clements, Doug H., and Michael T. Battista. 1992. Geometry and spatial reasoning. In Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning. Edited by D. A. Grouws. New York: Macmillan, pp. 420–64. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cronbach, Lee J. 1951. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychomerika 16: 297–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ekstrom, Ruth B., John. W. French, and Harry. H. Harman. 1976. Manual for Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Princeton: Educational Testing Service. [Google Scholar]
- Field, Andy. 2009. Discovering Statistics Using SPSS, 3rd ed. Thousand Oaks: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Gagnier, Kristin M., and Kelly R. Fisher. 2020. Unpacking the Black Box of Translation: A framework for infusing spatial thinking into curricula. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications 5: 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilligan, Katie A., Alex Hodgkiss, Michael Thomas, and Emily K. Farran. 2019. The developmental relations between spatial cognition and mathematics in primary school children. Developmental Science 22: e12786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gilligan-Lee, Katie A., Michael S. C. Thomas, and Emily K. Farran. 2020. First demonstration of effective spatial training for near transfer to spatial performance and far transfer to a range of mathematics skills at 8 years. Developmental Science 23: e12909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Green, C. Shawn, and Nora S. Newcombe. 2020. Cognitive training: How evidence, controversies, and challenges inform education policy. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7: 80–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunderson, Elizabeth A., Gerardo Ramirez, Sian L. Beilock, S. L, and Susan C. Levine. 2012. The relation between spatial skill and early number knowledge: The role of the linear number line. Developmental Psychology 48: 1229–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Harris, Justin, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, and Nora S. Newcombe. 2013. Understanding spatial transformations: Similarities and differences between mental rotation and mental folding. Cognitive Processing 14: 105–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hawes, Zachary C. K., Joan Moss, Beverly Caswell, Sarah Naqvi, and Sharla MacKinnon. 2017. Enhancing children’s spatial and numerical skills through a dynamic spatial approach to early geometry instruction: Effects of a 32-week intervention. Cognition and Instruction 35: 236–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hawes, Zachary C. K., Jo-Anne LeFevre, Chang Xu, and Cathy D. Bruce. 2015. Mental rotation with tangible three-dimensional objects: A new measure sensitive to developmental differences in 4-to 8-year-old children. Mind, Brain, and Education 9: 10–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hawes, Zachary C. K., Katie A. Gilligan-Lee, and Kelly S. Mix. 2022. Effects of spatial training on mathematics performance: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology 58: 112–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hawes, Zachary C. K., Katie A. Gilligan-Lee, and Kelly S. Mix. 2023. Infusing spatial thinking into middle school mathematics: What, why, and how? In Mathematical Learning and Cognition in Middle Childhood and Early Adolescence: Integrating Interdisciplinary Research into Practice. Edited by K. M. Robinson, D. Kostopoulos and A. Dubé. Berlin: Springer Nature Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, Andrew F. 2015. An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral Research 50: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hegarty, Mary, and Maria Kozhevnikov. 1999. Types of visual–spatial representations and mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology 91: 684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- IBM Corp. 2022. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 29.0. Armonk: IBM Corp. [Google Scholar]
- Kieran, Carolyn. 1981. Concepts associated with the equality symbol. Educational Studies in Mathematics 12: 317–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leikin, Roza, Abraham Berman, and Orit Zaslavsky. 2000. Learning through teaching: The case of symmetry. Mathematics Education Research Journal 12: 18–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Linn, Maria C., and A. C. Petersen. 1985. Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development 56: 1479–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lowrie, Tom, Danielle Harris, Tracy Logan, and Mary Hegarty. 2021. The impact of a spatial intervention program on students’ spatial reasoning and mathematics performance. Journal of Experimental Education 89: 259–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lowrie, Tom, Ilyse Resnick, Danielle Harris, and Tracy Logan. 2020. In search of the mechanisms that enable transfer from spatial reasoning to mathematics understanding. Mathematics Education Research Journal 32: 175–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lowrie, Tom, Tracy Logan, and Ajay Ramful. 2017. Visuospatial training improves elementary student’s mathematics performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology 87: 170–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lowrie, Tom, Tracy Logan, and Mary Hegarty. 2019. The influence of spatial visualization training on students’ spatial reasoning and mathematics performance. Journal of Cognition and Development 20: 729–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lowrie, Tom, Tracy Logan, and Sitti Patahuddin. 2018a. A learning design for developing mathematics understanding: The ELPSA framework. Australian Mathematics Teacher 74: 26–31. [Google Scholar]
- Lowrie, Tom, Tracy Logan, Danielle Harris, and Mary Hegarty. 2018b. The impact of an intervention program on students’ spatial reasoning: Student engagement through mathematics-enhanced learning activities. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications 3: 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mix, Kelly S., and Michael T. Battista, eds. 2018. Visualizing Mathematics: The Role of Spatial Reasoning in Mathematical Thought. Dordrecht: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Mix, Kelly S., Susan C. Levine, Yi-Ling Cheng, Jerri D. Stockton, and Corrine Bower. 2021. Effects of spatial training on mathematics in first and sixth grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology 113: 304–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mulligan, Joanne J., Geoff Woolcott, Michael Mitchelmore, Susan Busatto, Jennifer Lai, and Brent Davis. 2020. Evaluating the impact of a Spatial Reasoning Mathematics Program (SRMP) intervention in the primary school. Mathematics Education Research Journal 32: 285–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ng, Oi-Lam, and Nathalie Sinclair. 2015. Young children reasoning about symmetry in a dynamic geometry environment. ZDM Mathematics Education 47: 421–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patahuddin, S. M., T. Logan, and A. Ramful. 2018. Characteristics of spatial visualisation: Perspectives from area of composite shapes. In Making Waves, Opening Spaces, Proceedings of the 41st annual conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia). Edited by J. Hunter, P. Perger and L. Darragh. Auckland: MERGA, pp. 623–30. [Google Scholar]
- Ramful, Ajay, Siew Yin Ho, and Tom Lowrie. 2015. Visual and analytical strategies in spatial visualisation: Perspectives from bilateral symmetry and reflection. Mathematics Education Research Journal 27: 443–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramful, Ajay, Tom Lowrie, and Tracy Logan. 2017. Measurement of spatial ability: Construction and validation of the spatial reasoning instrument for middle school students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 35: 709–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Resnick, Ilyse, and Mike Stieff. Forthcoming. The role of theory in research and practice: Characterizing cognition and context within mathematics education. In Fourth International Handbook of Mathematics Education: Controversial Issues in Mathematics Education. Edited by M. A. (Ken) Clements, Berinderjeet Kaur, Tom Lowrie, Vilma Mesa and Johan Prytz. Dordrecht: Springer.
- Resnick, Ilyse, and Tom Lowrie. Forthcoming. Spatial games improve spatial reasoning and numeracy in preschool: Findings from a large scale nationally representative randomized control trial. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education.
- Resnick, Ilyse, Tracy Logan, Danielle Harris, and Tom Lowrie, eds. 2020. The relation between mathematics achievement and spatial reasoning. Mathematics Education Research Journal 32: 171–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sinclair, Nathalie. 2004. The Roles of the aesthetic in mathematical inquiry. Mathematical Thinking and Learning 6: 261–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sinclair, Nathalie N., and Cathy D. Bruce, eds. 2015. New opportunities in geometry education at the primary school. ZDM–Mathematics Education [Special Issue] 47: 319–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sorby, Sheryl, Beth Casey, N. Veurink, and A. Dulaney. 2013. The role of spatial training in improving spatial and calculus performance in engineering students. Learning and Individual Differences 26: 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stieff, Mike, and David Uttal. 2015. How much can spatial training improve STEM achievement? Educational Psychology Review 27: 607–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wai, Jonathan, David Lubinski, and Camilla P. Benbow. 2009. Spatial ability for STEM domains: Aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. Journal of Educational Psychology 101: 817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, Vince, and Ken Smith. 2017. Children’s schemes for anticipating the validity of nets for solids. Mathematics Educational Research Journal 29: 369–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xie, Fang, Li Zhang, Xu Chen, and Ziqiang Xin. 2020. Is spatial ability related to mathematical ability: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychology Review 32: 113–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Group | Group Title | Intervention | N |
---|---|---|---|
A | Business-as-usual | Control: Followed the curriculum of the state. Completed approximately 4 h of mathematics per week for 14 weeks. | 97 |
B | Isolated Intervention | Digital Spatial Skills: Followed the curriculum of the state. Completed approximately 3.5 h of mathematics and 40 min (+/−10 min) of digital spatial skills per week for 14 weeks. | 98 |
C | Embedded Intervention | Spatial Visualization Program and Digital Spatial Skills: Followed the curriculum of the state. Completed approximately 3 h of mathematics and 60 min (+/−10 min) of spatial visualization and digital spatial skills per week for 14 weeks. | 92 |
Reflections | 3D Mental Folding |
---|---|
Week 1. Students choose the image which has been reflected over horizontal and vertical lines. The images are colour; the line is displayed. There are 3 answer options. | Week 8. Students choose the configuration of shapes that will fold to make a net. Three options are provided. There are 11 questions for the 11 nets. |
Week 2. Students choose the image which has been reflected over a horizontal and vertical line. The images are colour; the line is not displayed. There are 4 answer options. | Week 9. As above, but four options are provided, and nets are presented in random orientation. |
Week 3. Students choose the image which has been reflected over an inclined line. The images are colour; the line is displayed. There are 3 answer options. | Week 10. Students choose the opposite face to the star on the given net. These nets are all 1:4:1 nets. |
Week 4. Students choose the image which has been reflected over a horizontal, vertical, and inclined line. The images are colour; the line is displayed. There are 4 answer options. | Week 11. As above, but these nets are combinations of 1:3:2, 2:3:1, 2:2:2, and 3:3 nets. |
Week 5. Students choose the image which has been reflected over a horizontal, vertical, and inclined line. The images are colour; the line is not displayed. There are 4 answer options. | Week 12. As above, but these are combinations of all net configurations. |
Week 6. Students choose the image which has been reflected over a horizontal, vertical, and inclined line. The images are black and white; the line is displayed. There are 3 answer options. | Week 13. Students are shown a net configuration with a symbol on one of its faces. They must rotate a given 3D cube to see it has two symbols on it. One of the symbols corresponds to the symbol on the net. They must position the other symbol on the correct face of the net. These are all 1:4:1 nets. It begins with opposite faces and then moves to adjacent faces. |
Week 7. Students choose the image which has been reflected over a horizontal, vertical, and inclined line. The images are black and white; the line is not displayed. There are 4 answer options. | Week 14. As above, but these are a combination of 1:3:2, 2:3:1, and 2:2:2 nets. The last two questions have no fixed symbol on the net, so the students must position both symbols to correspond with the cube. |
Outcome Variable | Pre-Test | Post-Test | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Business-as-Usual (BAU) Control | Isolated Spatial Intervention | Embedded Spatial Intervention | Business-as-Usual (BAU) Control | Isolated Spatial Intervention | Embedded Spatial Intervention | ||
97 | 98 | 92 | 97 | 98 | 92 | ||
Spatial Visualization: | |||||||
Reflection/3D Folding | 3.24 (1.59) | 4.04 (1.79) | 3.23 (1.66) | 3.96 (1.87) | 4.48 (1.97) | 4.53 (1.92) | |
2D Folding/Tessellation | 2.43 (1.22) | 2.88 (1.33) | 2.45 (1.51) | 2.58 (1.44) | 3.23 (1.53) | 2.95 (1.45) | |
Transfer skill: Math | 9.81 (2.55) | 10.11 (2.65) | 8.94 (2.71) | 10.19 (2.49) | 11.07 (2.83) | 10.25 (2.70) |
Outcome Measure | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pre-test | 1. Reflection/3D Folding | -- | ||||
2. 2D Folding/Tessellation | .338 ** | -- | ||||
3. Math | .428 ** | .461 ** | -- | |||
Post-test | 4. Reflection/3D Folding | .563 ** | .334 ** | .490 ** | -- | |
5. 2D Folding/Tessellation | .406 ** | .438 ** | .513 ** | .452 ** | -- | |
6. Math | .430 ** | .422 ** | .700 ** | .579 ** | .514 ** |
(I) Group | (J) Group | Mean Difference (I-J) | Std. Error | p Value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Trained spatial skills | ||||
Group A BAU | Group B Isolated | .005 | .233 | .984 |
Group C Embedded | −.574 * | .233 | .014 | |
Group B Isolated | Group C Embedded | −.578 * | .234 | .014 |
Non-trained spatial skills | ||||
Group A BAU | Group B Isolated | −.449 * | .196 | .023 |
Group C Embedded | −.391 * | .197 | .048 | |
Group B Isolated | Group C Embedded | .058 | .198 | .769 |
Math transfer | ||||
Group A BAU | Group B Isolated | −.664 * | .274 | .016 |
Group C Embedded | −.672 * | .281 | .017 | |
Group B Isolated | Group C Embedded | −.008 | .282 | .976 |
Level of Spatial Skill | N | % | Score Range/8 |
---|---|---|---|
Low | 23 | 23.5 | <2 |
Mid | 36 | 36.7 | 2–4 |
High | 39 | 39.8 | >4 |
Total | 98 | 100.0 |
Spatial Level | Effect | SE | t | p | LLCI | ULCI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Low | .402 | .222 | 1.81 | .071 | −.038 | .843 |
Medium | .809 | .137 | 5.89 | .001 | .536 | 1.08 |
High | 1.21 | .198 | 6.13 | .001 | .823 | 1.61 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lowrie, T.; Logan, T. Spatial Visualization Supports Students’ Math: Mechanisms for Spatial Transfer. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 127. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060127
Lowrie T, Logan T. Spatial Visualization Supports Students’ Math: Mechanisms for Spatial Transfer. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(6):127. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060127
Chicago/Turabian StyleLowrie, Tom, and Tracy Logan. 2023. "Spatial Visualization Supports Students’ Math: Mechanisms for Spatial Transfer" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 6: 127. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060127
APA StyleLowrie, T., & Logan, T. (2023). Spatial Visualization Supports Students’ Math: Mechanisms for Spatial Transfer. Journal of Intelligence, 11(6), 127. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060127