Moderating Role of Creative Mindset in the Effect of Metacognitive Experience on Insight Problem Solving
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Metacognitive Experience and Creativity
1.2. Metacognitive Experience, Creative Mindset, and Creativity
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Methods
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Design
2.1.3. Chinese Logogriphs Materials
2.1.4. Font Style Manipulation
2.1.5. Procedure
2.1.6. Manipulation Check
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Manipulation Check
2.2.2. Task Performance
2.2.3. Insight Value
2.3. Discussion
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Methods
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Design
3.1.3. Chinese Logogriphs Materials
3.1.4. Font Style Manipulation
3.1.5. Creative Mindset Manipulation
3.1.6. Procedure
3.1.7. Manipulation Checks
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Manipulation Checks
3.2.2. Task Performance
3.2.3. Reaction Time
3.3. Discussion
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Aiello, Daniel A., Andrew F. Jarosz, Patrick J. Cushen, and Jennifer Wiley. 2012. Firing the executive: When an analytic approach to problem solving helps and hurts. The Journal of Problem Solving 4: 116–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alter, Alter L., and Daniel M. Oppenheimer. 2009. Uniting the Tribes of Fluency to Form a Metacognitive Nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review 13: 219–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Alter, Adam L., Daniel M. Oppenheimer, Nicholas Epley, and Rebecca N. Eyre. 2007. Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136: 569–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Andiliou, Andria, and P. Karen Murphy. 2010. Examining variations among researchers’ and teachers’ conceptualizations of creativity: A review and synthesis of contemporary research. Educational Research Review 5: 201–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ansburg, Pamela I., and Katherine Hill. 2003. Creative and analytic thinkers differ in their use of attentional resources. Personality and Individual Differences 34: 1141–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Ben Bolker, and Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benedek, Mathias, Sabine Bergner, Tanja Könen, Andreas Fink, and Aljoscha C. Neubauer. 2011. EEG alpha synchronization is related to top-down processing in convergent and divergent thinking. Neuropsychologia 49: 3505–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berkowitz, Aaron L., and Daniel Ansari. 2008. Generation of novel motor sequences: The neural correlates of musical improvisation. Neuroimage 41: 535–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blackwell, Lisa S., Kali H. Trzesniewski, and Carol S. Dweck. 2007. Implicit Theories of Intelligence Predict Achievement Across an Adolescent Transition: A Longitudinal Study and an Intervention. Child Development 78: 246–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boksman, Kristine, Jean Théberge, Peter C. Williamson, Dick J. Drost, Ashok Malla, Maria Densmore, Jatinder Takhar, William Pavlosky, Ravi S. Menon, and Richard W. J. Neufeld. 2005. A 4.0-T fMRI study of brain connectivity during word fluency in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 75: 247–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Botvinick, Matthew M. Braver, Todd S. Barch, Deanna M. Carter, Cameron S. Cohen, and D. Jonathan. 2001. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review 108: 624–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brand, Serge, Torsten Reimer, and Klaus Opwis. 2007. How do we learn in a negative mood? Effects of a negative mood on transfer and learning. Learning and Instruction 17: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Qun, Jun Luo, Jun Jiang, Dong Wei, and Qing Zhang. 2012. Effects of Unconscious Processing on Creativity Problems Solving. Psychological Devclopment and Education 28: 569–75. [Google Scholar]
- Chiu, Chi, Yue Ying, Yi Hong, and Carol S. Dweck. 1997. Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 73: 19–30. [Google Scholar]
- De Dreu, Carsten K. W., Matthijs Baas, and Bernard A. Nijstad. 2008. Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood-creativity link: Toward a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology 94: 739–56. [Google Scholar]
- Dweck, Carol S. 1990. Self-Theories: Their Role in Motivation, Personality, and Development. Essays in Social Psychology. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 38: 199–235. [Google Scholar]
- Dweck, Carol S., and Ellen L. Leggett. 1988. A Social-Cognitive Approach to Motivation and Personality. Psychological Review 95: 256–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Erbas, Ayhan Kursat, and Selda Bas. 2015. The Contribution of Personality Traits, Motivation, Academic Risk-Taking and Metacognition to the Creative Ability in Mathematics. Creativity Research Journal 27: 299–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eysenck, Hans. J. 1993. Creativity and Personality: Suggestions for a Theory. Psychological Inquiry 4: 147–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faul, Franz, Edgar Erdfelder, Albert-Georg Lang, and Axel Buchner. 2007. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods 39: 175–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fink, Andreas, Karl Koschutnig, Mathias Benedek, Gernot Reishofer, Anja Ischebeck, Elisabeth M. Weiss, and Franz Ebner. 2012. Stimulating creativity via the exposure to other people’s ideas. Human Brain Mapping 33: 2603–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Flavell, John H. 1976. Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In The Nature of Intelligence. Edited by Lauren B. Resnick. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 231–36. [Google Scholar]
- Förster, Jens, Ronald S. Friedman, and Nira Liberman. 2004. Temporal Construal Effects on Abstract and Concrete Thinking: Consequences for Insight and Creative Cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 87: 177–89. [Google Scholar]
- Gabora, Liane. 2005. Creative Thought as a non-Darwinian Evolutionary Process. Journal of Creative Behavior 39: 262–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilhooly, Kenneth J., Evridiki Fioratou, Susan H. Anthony, and V. Wynn. 2007. Divergent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology 98: 611–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guilford, Joy Paul. 1967. Creativity: Yesterday, Today and Tomorrow. The Journal of Creative Behavior 1: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hong, Ying, Chi Chiu, Carol S. Dweck, Derrick M. S. Lin, and Wendy Wan. 1999. Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77: 588–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howardjones, Paul A. 2002. A Dual-state Model of Creative Cognition for Supporting Strategies that Foster Creativity in the Classroom. International Journal of Technology and Design Education 12: 215–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaeger, T. Florian. 2008. Categorical Data Analysis: Away from ANOVAs (Transformation or Not) and towards Logit Mixed Models. Journal of Memory and Language 59: 434–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- James, William. 2007. The Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt. [Google Scholar]
- Jausovec, Norbert. 1994. Metacognition in Creative Problem-Solving. In Problem Finding, Problem Solving, and Creativity. Edited by Mark A. Runco. Norwood: Ablex Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Jia, Xiao, Ping Li, Xin Li, Yu Zhang, Wei Cao, Li Cao, and Wei Li. 2016. The Effect of Word Frequency on Judgments of Learning: Contributions of Beliefs and Processing Fluency. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jia, Xiao, Tian Xu, and Yu Zhang. 2022. The Role of Metacognitive Strategy Monitoring and Control in the Relationship between Creative Mindsets and Divergent Thinking Performance. Journal of Intelligence 10: 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jia, Xiao, Wei Li, and Li Cao. 2019. The Role of Metacognitive Components in Creative Thinking. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jungbeeman, Mark, Edward M. Bowden, Jason Haberman, Jennifer L. Frymiare, Stella Arambel-Liu, Richard Greenblatt, Paul J. Reber, and John Kounios. 2004. Neural Activity When People Solve Verbal Problems with Insight. PLoS Biology 2: e97. [Google Scholar]
- Karwowski, Maciej. 2014. Creative mindsets: Measurement, correlates, consequences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 8: 62–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koriat, Asher. 2008. Easy comes, easy goes? The link between learning and remembering and its exploitation in metacognition. Memory & Cognition 36: 416–28. [Google Scholar]
- Koriat, Asher, and Hilit Ma’ayan. 2005. The Effects of Encoding Fluency and Retrieval Fluency on Judgments of Learning. Journal of Memory and Language 52: 478–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koriat, Asher, Robert A. Bjork, Limor Sheffer, and Sarah K. Bar. 2004. Predicting One’s Own Forgetting: The Role of Experience-Based and Theory-Based Processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133: 643–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kounios, John, Jessica I. Fleck, Deborah L. Green, Lisa Payne, Jennifer L. Stevenson, Edward M. Bowden, and Mark Jung-Beeman. 2008. The origins of insight in resting-state brain activity. Neuropsychologia 46: 281–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kühl, Tim Alexander Eitel, Katharina Scheiter, and Peter Gerjets. 2014. A Call for an Unbiased Search for Moderators in Disfluency Research: Reply to Oppenheimer and Alter. Applied Cognitive Psychology 28: 805–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Labroo, Aparna A., and Sara Kim. 2009. The “instrumentality” heuristic: Why metacognitive difficulty is desirable during goal pursuit. Psychological Science 20: 127–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, Ping, Xiao Jia, Xin Li, and Wei Li. 2016. The effect of animacy on metamemory. Memory & Cognition 44: 696–705. [Google Scholar]
- Lieberman, Matthew D., Ruth Gaunt, and Daniel T. Gilbert. 2002. Reflexion and reflection: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 34: 199–249. [Google Scholar]
- Lizarraga, María Luisa Sanz de Acedo, and María Teresa Sanz de Acedo Baquedano. 2013. How creative potential is related to metacognition. European Journal of Education and Psychology 6: 69–81. [Google Scholar]
- Lucas, Brian J., and Loran F. Nordgren. 2015. People underestimate the value of persistence for creative performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 109: 232–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luo, Jun, and Guenther Knoblich. 2007. Studying insight problem solving with neuroscientific methods. Methods 42: 77–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luo, Jun, Wen Li, Andreas Fink, Lei Jia, Xiao Xiao, Jiang Qiu, and Qing Zhang. 2011. The time course of breaking mental sets and forming novel associations in insight-like problem solving: An ERP investigation. Experimental Brain Research 212: 583–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mehta, Ravi P., Rui Zhu, and Amar Cheema. 2012. Is Noise Always Bad? Exploring the Effects of Ambient Noise on Creative Cognition. Journal of Consumer Research 39: 784–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Metcalfe, Janet, and David Wiebe. 1987. Intuition in insight and noninsight problem solving. Memory & Cognition 15: 238–46. [Google Scholar]
- Miele, David B., and Daniel C. Molden. 2010. Naive theories of intelligence and the role of processing fluency in perceived comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 139: 535–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miele, David B., Bridgid Finn, and Daniel C. Molden. 2011. Does Easily Learned Mean Easily Remembered? It Depends on Your Beliefs About Intelligence. Psychological Science 22: 320–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miele, David B., Lisa K. Son, and Janet Metcalfe. 2013. Children’s Naive Theories of Intelligence Influence Their Metacognitive Judgments. Child Development 84: 1879–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Molden, Daniel C., and Carol S. Dweck. 2006. Finding “meaning” in psychology: A lay theories approach to self-regulation, social perception, and social development. American Psychologist 61: 192–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nijstad, Bernard A., Carsten K. W. De Dreu, Eric F. Rietzschel, and Matthijs Baas. 2010. The dual pathway to creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European Review of Social Psychology 21: 34–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Novemsky, Nathan, Ravi Dhar, Norbert Schwarz, and Itamar Simonson. 2007. Preference fluency in choice. Journal of Marketing Research 44: 347–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oconnor, Alexander J., Charlan J. Nemeth, and Satoshi Akutsu. 2013. Consequences of Beliefs about the Malleability of Creativity. Creativity Research Journal 25: 155–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Preiss, David D., Diego Cosmelli, Valeska Grau, and Dominga Ortiz. 2016. Examining the influence of mind wandering and metacognition on creativity in university and vocational students. Learning and Individual Differences 51: 417–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Puente-Díaz, Rogelio. 2023. Metacognitive Feelings as a Source of Information for the Creative Process: A Conceptual Exploration. Journal of Intelligence 11: 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Qiu, Jiang, Hong Li, Dong Yang, Yue Luo, Ying Li, Zhen Wu, and Qing Zhang. 2008. The neural basis of insight problem solving: An event-related potential study. Brain and Cognition 68: 100–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qiu, Jiang, Hong Li, Jerwen Jou, Jia Liu, Yue Luo, Ting Feng, Zhen Wu, and Qing Zhang. 2010. Neural correlates of the “Aha” experiences: Evidence from an fMRI study of insight problem solving. Cortex 46: 397–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reber, Rolf, Pascal Wurtz, and Thomas D. Zimmermann. 2004. Exploring “fringe” consciousness: The subjective experience of perceptual fluency and its objective bases. Consciousness and Cognition 13: 47–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roskes, Marieke, Carsten K. W. De Dreu, and Bernard A. Nijstad. 2012. Necessity Is the Mother of Invention: Avoidance Motivation Stimulates Creativity Through Cognitive Effort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 103: 242–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubensteina, Lisa DaVia, Gregory L. Callanb, Lisa M. Ridgleya, and Amanda Henderson. 2019. Students’ strategic planning and strategy use during creative problem solving: The importance of perspective-taking. Thinking Skills and Creativity 34: 100556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skaar, Øystein O., and Rolf Reber. 2020. The phenomenology of Aha-experiences. Motivation Science 6: 49–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sowden, Paul T., Andrew Pringle, and Liane Gabora. 2015. The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory. Thinking & Reasoning 21: 40–60. [Google Scholar]
- Sprugnoli, Giulia, Simone Rossi, Alexandra Emmendorfer, Alessandro Rossi, Sook-Lei Liew, Elisa Tatti, Giorgio di Lorenzo, Alvaro Pascual-Leone, and Emiliano Santarnecchi. 2017. Neural correlates of Eureka moment. Intelligence 62: 99–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., and Todd I. Lubart. 1991. An Investment Theory of Creativity and Its Development. Human Development 34: 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Storbeck, Justin, and Gerald L. Clore. 2007. On the interdependence of cognition and emotion. Cognition & Emotion 21: 1212–37. [Google Scholar]
- Thomas, Manoj, and Vicki G. Morwitz. 2008. The Ease-of-Computation Effect: The Interplay of Metacognitive Experiences and Naive Theories in Judgments of Price Differences. Journal of Marketing Research 46: 81–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Threadgold, Emma, John E. Marsh, Neil McLatchie, and Linden J. Ball. 2019. Background music stints creativity: Evidence from compound remote associate tasks. Applied Cognitive Psychology 33: 873–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Topolinski, Sascha, and Rolf Reber. 2010. Gaining Insight Into the “Aha” Experience. Current Directions in Psychological Science 19: 402–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- West, Keon, and Susanne Bruckmüller. 2013. Nice and easy does it: How perceptual fluency moderates the effectiveness of imagined contact. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 49: 254–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, Zhen, Qiu Jiang, and Qing Zhang. 2009. Exploring a New Experimental Paradigm in the Brain Research of Insight. Psychological Science 32: 122–25. [Google Scholar]
- Yeager, David S., Paul Hanselman, Gregory M. Walton, Jared S. Murray, Robert Crosnoe, Chandra Muller, Elizabeth Tipton, Barbara Schneider, Chris S. Hulleman, Cintia P. Hinojosa, and et al. 2019. A national experiment reveals where a growth mindset improves achievement. Nature 573: 364–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhou, Yi, Wa Yang, and Xin Bai. 2020. Creative mindsets: Scale validation in the Chinese setting and generalization to the real workplace. Frontiers in Psychology 11: 463–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Task Difficulty | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Font Style | Easy | Difficult | t | p | dz |
Easy | 0.63 (0.05) | 0.94 (0.07) | −11.52 | <0.001 | 3.62 |
Difficult | 0.62 (0.05) | 0.95 (0.07) | −12.44 | <0.001 | 3.97 |
Performance Accuracy | Insight Value | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effects | β | SE | t Value | p | β | SE | t Value | p |
Intercept | 0.64 | 0.02 | 31.87 | <0.001 | 3.14 | 0.2 | 16.13 | <0.001 |
Task difficulty | −0.50 | 0.02 | −20.99 | <0.001 | −0.50 | 0.2 | −2.22 | <0.05 |
Font style | −0.31 | 0.02 | −13.03 | <0.001 | −0.24 | 0.2 | −1.18 | 0.24 |
Task difficulty×Font style | 0.27 | 0.03 | 8.17 | <0.001 | −0.07 | 0.29 | −0.26 | 0.80 |
Performance Accuracy | Reaction Time | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Effects | β | SE | t Value | p | β | SE | t Value | p |
Intercept | 0.45 | 0.02 | 19.1 | <0.001 | 7.15 | 0.41 | 17.53 | <0.001 |
Creative mindset | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.47 | 0.64 | −1.17 | 0.58 | −2.03 | <0.05 |
Font style | −0.08 | 0.03 | −3.22 | <0.005 | 1.00 | 0.58 | 1.73 | 0.09 |
Creative mindset × Font style | 0.08 | 0.04 | 2.27 | <0.05 | 2.57 | 0.82 | 3.15 | <0.005 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Jia, X.; Li, P.; Chen, Q.; Li, W. Moderating Role of Creative Mindset in the Effect of Metacognitive Experience on Insight Problem Solving. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 99. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060099
Jia X, Li P, Chen Q, Li W. Moderating Role of Creative Mindset in the Effect of Metacognitive Experience on Insight Problem Solving. Journal of Intelligence. 2023; 11(6):99. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060099
Chicago/Turabian StyleJia, Xiaoyu, Ping Li, Qunlin Chen, and Weijian Li. 2023. "Moderating Role of Creative Mindset in the Effect of Metacognitive Experience on Insight Problem Solving" Journal of Intelligence 11, no. 6: 99. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060099
APA StyleJia, X., Li, P., Chen, Q., & Li, W. (2023). Moderating Role of Creative Mindset in the Effect of Metacognitive Experience on Insight Problem Solving. Journal of Intelligence, 11(6), 99. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence11060099