Resilient Subject Agreement Morpho-Syntax in the Germanic Romance Contact Area
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Subject Pronouns
2.1. The Subject Pronoun System of the Northern Italian Varieties
(1) | Implicational scale: |
Second singular2 → third singular→ third plural → first singular/plural → second plural |
(2) | *(ti) vien |
you come |
(3) | TI *(ti) vien | (Venetian) |
YOU (you) come |
(4) | a. | Cossa ga-lo? | (Venetian) |
What has-he | |||
‘What does he have’ | |||
b. | Cossa ti ga?4 | ||
What you have? | |||
‘What do you have?’ |
(5) | a. | Ti magni patate e *(ti)bevi café ‘You eat potatoes and (you) drink coffee’ | |
‘You are eating potatoes and drinking coffee’ | |||
b. | Ti canti e *(ti) bali | ||
‘You sing and (you) dance’ | |||
‘You sing and dance’ | |||
c. | Ti lesi e ti rilesi sempre el stesso libro | ||
‘You read and you reread always the same book’ | |||
‘You are always reading and rereading the same book’ |
(6) | a. | Un tu vieni | (Florentine) |
‘Not you come’ | |||
‘You are not coming’ | |||
b. | La un viene | ||
‘She not comes’ | |||
‘She is not coming’ |
2.2. The Bavarian Subject Pronoun System and Complementizer Agreement
(7) | …, ob-st (du) des ned spuin kon-st |
if-2sg (you) this not play can-2sg | |
If you cannot play this | |
(8) | …, ob-ts (es) des ned spuin ken-ts |
if-2pl (you) this not play can-2pl | |
If you cannot play this | |
(9) | …, dass-ma (mia) noch Minga fahr-n |
…, that-1pl (we) to Munich go-1pl | |
…, that we will go to Munich |
(10) | 2sg > 2pl > 1pl. |
(11) | a. | schreib mir einmal den Brief, kriegst pro auch einen Groschen |
write.imp.2sg me once the letter, get.2sg too a penny’ | ||
‘Write the letter for me and you will get a coin’ | ||
b. | schreibt mir einmal den Brief, *kriegt pro auch einen Groschen | |
write-imp-2pl me once the letter, get-2pl too a penny |
(12) | [SpeakerP….[AdresseeP….[ForceP….[FinP [TP…[vP [VP]]]]]]] |
2.3. The Cimbrian Subject Pronoun System
(13) | adapted from Panieri et al. (2006) | ||||
1 person | 2 person | 3 person | 1 person pl. | 2 person pl. | 3 person pl. |
I | du | er/si/iz | biar | iear | se |
-e | -do/to10 | -ar/-se/-z | -bar | -dar | -sa |
dar | da |
(14) | Haüt khintar atz Lusern |
today comes = he to Luserna | |
(He will come to Luserna today) | |
(15) | azar khemm atz Lusern |
that = he comes.sbjv to Luserna | |
‘that he is coming to Lusern’ |
(16) | Benn khisto atz Lusérn? |
today comes = he to Luserna | |
When come = you to Luserna? | |
When are you coming to Lusern? | |
(17) | Haüt arbatesto dahuam |
Today work = you at home | |
Today you are working at home | |
(18) | I sperar azto arbatest dahuam |
I hope that = you work at home | |
I hope that you are working at home | |
(19) | Dar khütt ke du arbatest dahuam haüt |
He says that you work at home today | |
He says that you are working at home today | |
(20) | Dar khütt ke haüt arbatesto dahuam |
He says that today work = you at home today | |
He says that today you are working at home |
3. Nominal Subjects
(21) | A: Wer hat gestern dem Hans das Buch gegeben? |
Who has yesterday the.dat.m Hans the.acc.n book given | |
‘To whom has Hans given the book yesterday?’ | |
B: Gestern hat dem Hans das Buch erfreulicherweise der StuDENT gegeben. | |
Yesterday, has the.dat.m Hans the.acc.n book fortunately the.nom.n stu DENT given | |
‘Fortunately yesterday the student gave / has given the book to Hans.’ | |
B: Gestern hat dem Hans das Buch versehentlich der StuDENT gegeben. | |
Yesterday has the.dat.m Hans the.acc.n book accidentally the.nom.n stuDENT given | |
‘Yesterday, the student accidentally have / has given the book to Hans.’ | |
(22) | Gestern sind pro endlich nach Verona viele Studenten gekommen11 |
Yesterday are finally to Verona many students come | |
‘Yesterday, finally many students came to Verona’ |
(23) | Gestern wurde pro getanzt |
Yesterday was danced | |
‘Yesterday somebody danced’ |
(24) | Haüt iz = ta khent dar nono atz Lusern |
today is = DA come the grandpa to Luserna | |
‘Yesterday somebody danced’ | |
(Today the grandpa has arrived to Luserna) | |
(25) | Gestarn in balt hatt = ta gisek di diarn in has |
yesterday in = the wood has = DA seen the girl the hare | |
(26) | Gestarn iz = ta khent/ getånzt |
yesterday is = DA come/ danced | |
(Yesterday, it was danced) |
(27) | Allz in an stroach in balt der pua hatt gesek in vuks | |
All of a sudden in.the wood the boy has seen the fox | ||
‘All of a sudden, the boy saw the fox in the wood’ | ||
(28) | *Allz in an stroach in balt hatt dar pua gesek in vuks | |
yesterday is = DA come/danced | ||
‘Yesterday, it was danced´’ |
(29) | [ForceP [TopP…[SubjPdef [TopP [FocusP [SubjQP..[WhP [FinP [TP…[FocP…[vP [VP ]]]]]]]]]]] |
(30) | [FinP [SpecFin]…[TP [Spec T17] T° … [FocusP [SpecFocus] Foc° [vP ]]]]]]]]] |
4. General Conclusions: Resilient (Morpho)-Syntax
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | The data used in this article can be found in the literature quoted or come from the ASIt database for the NIDs, and from our own field work for Cimbrian. For the purposes of the Italian academy, Cecilia Poletto is responsible for Section 1, Section 2.1 and Section 2.2 and Alessandra Tomaselli for Section 2.3, Section 3 and Section 4. |
2 | This first step of Renzi and Vanelli´s generalization is violated precisely in some Trentino dialects, such as Tuenno (see AIS, Atlas Italiens und der Südwschweiz), which is, however, not the closest Trentino variety to Cimbrian, which is geographically closer to Valsuganotto (see Poletto 1993, 2000; Poletto and Tortora 2016 for a discussion of the violation of the first step of the implicational scale). This is also pointed out by Manzini and Savoia (2005) and Manzini (2019) for other dialects. |
3 | This is so, even in V2 Ladin varieties such as Badiot that have otherwise no clitic doubling at all. |
4 | It is interesting to note that the Trentino dialects in contact with Cimbrian still have a more widespread and conservative pattern of subject clitic inversion in main interrogatives, which also includes the second person singular pronoun. |
5 | Some authors (see Gruber 2008) propose that complementizer agreement is actually a type of pronoun. We remain agnostic with respect to this point, simply noting that the type of pronoun found in Bavarian is identical to verbal inflection, not to a reduction of a tonic pronoun. This difference shows that complementizer agreement is not a regular case of enclitic subject pronouns but something else. |
6 | |
7 | On the special status of the second person, there is a long tradition of studies from different perspectives (see, for instance, Lazzeroni 1994). We do not see fit to enter here a long summary of all the existing approaches, since this is not the major focus of the article. In any case, an anonymous reviewer suggests that an alternative view to capture the special status of the second person could be found in the perspective discussed in distributed morphology in work by Bobaljik (2008); Calabrese (1995); Halle (1997); Noyer (1992). |
8 | |
9 | We leave this point for further research. We thank an anonymous reviewer for pointing this out to us. |
10 | The distinction between do and to is a purely phonological one and depends on the preceding phonological context: when there is a voiceless consonant that precedes the pronoun, the /d/ assimilates for sonority and becomes voiceless. |
11 | For low subjects with unaccusative verbs and in passive constructions cf. also Den Besten (1989) and Tomaselli (1986). |
12 | There is evidence that the past participle position in Cimbrian is rather low, and most probably lower than all aspectual adverbs. For our analysis to be correct, we only need to say that it moves immediately higher than the SpecFocus position where the subject is sitting. As shown by Belletti (2004, p. 13), the vP peripheral SpecFocus position is indeed located lower than the lowest (manner) adverbs. Alternatively, the postverbal position in Cimbrian might be the argumental SpecvP one, since Cimbrian probably resolves these cases of Focus through a prosodic strategy relying either on remnant VP movement or Subject right-extraposition. |
13 | Whether we assume a null category pro licensed in SpecT by either verbal morphology or subject clitics sitting in a higher projection or not, i.e., we postulate that subject clitics/agreement morphology satisfy the pronominal feature of T per se, is a problem of theoretical viewpoint which should be left open for further discussion. In any case, it does not have any direct import on the analysis presented here. In this perspective, an inactive SpecT could well be interpreted as non-projected. |
14 | For a different view on the complementation system of Salentino, which treats ku as a modal particle similar to mi/mu of Calabrian and Sicilian, see Manzini and Savoia (2005). |
15 | It is probably the case that subjects in German can also be located higher, since they can occur higher than Topics, but we do not pursue these facts further here, since they would require a whole detailed investigation on this property. |
16 | For a detailed discussion on the properties of TP in Cimbrian, see Bidese et al. (2020). |
17 | The SpecT position is in italics because it is not projected at all. |
References
- Bayer, Josef. 1984. COMP in Bavarian syntax. The Linguistic Review 3: 209–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bayer, Josef. 2013. Klitisierung, Reanalyse und die Lizensierung von Nullformen: Zwei Beispiele aus dem Bairischen. In Dialektologie in Neuem Gewand. Zu Mikro-/Varietätenlinguistik, Sprachenvergleich und Universalgrammatik. Edited by Werner Abraham and Elisabeth Leiss. Hamburg: Helmut Buske, pp. 29–45. [Google Scholar]
- Belletti, Adriana. 1988. The Case of Unaccusatives. Linguistic Inquiry 19: 1–34. [Google Scholar]
- Belletti, Adriana. 2004. Aspects of the Low IP Area. In The Structure of CP and IP. Edited by Luigi Rizzi. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 16–51. [Google Scholar]
- Benincà, Paola, and Guglielmo Cinque. 1985. Lexical Subjects in Italian and the pro-drop Parameter. Paper presented at the Comparative Generative Grammar Fiesta, Salzburg, Austria, August 30. [Google Scholar]
- Den Besten, Hans. 1989. Studies in Westgermanic Syntax. Doctoral dissertation, Katholieke Universiteit Brabant, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. [Google Scholar]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo, and Alessandra Tomaselli. 2018. Developing pro-drop: The case of Cimbrian. In Null Subjects in Generative Grammar. A synchronic and Diachronic Perspective. Edited by Federica Cognola and Jan Casalicchio. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 52–69. [Google Scholar]
- Bidese, Ermenegildo, Andrea Padovan, and Alessandra Tomaselli. 2020. Rethinking V2 and Nominative case assignment: New insights from a Germanic variety in Northern Italy. In Rethinking Verb Second. Edited by Rebecca Woods and Sam Wolfe. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 575–93. [Google Scholar]
- Bobaljik, Jonathan. 2008. Missing Persons: A Case Study in Morphological Universals. The Linguistic Review 25: 203–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brandi, Luciana, and Patrizia Cordin. 1989. Two Italian Dialects and the Null Subject Parameter. I. Edited by O. Jaeggli and K. Safir. The Null Subject Parameter. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 111–42. [Google Scholar]
- Calabrese, Andrea. 1993. The Sentential Complementation of Salentino:A Study of a Language without Infinitival Clauses. In Syntactic Theory and the Dialects of Italy. Edited by Adriana Belletti. Turin: Rosenberg and Sellier, pp. 28–98. [Google Scholar]
- Calabrese, Andrea. 1995. A Constraint-Based Theory of Phonological Markedness and Simplification Procedures. Linguistic Inquiry 26: 373–463. [Google Scholar]
- Carstens, Vicky. 2003. Rethinking complementizer agreement: Agree with a case-checked goal. Linguistic Inquiry 34: 393–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Damonte, Federico. 2010. Mood Concord between CP and IP in Salentino and Southern Calabrian Subjunctive Complements. In Mapping the Left Pheriphery. Edited by Paola Benincà and Nicola Munaro. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 228–56. [Google Scholar]
- Giorgi, Alessandra. 2010. About the Speaker: Towards a Syntax of Indexicality. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Grewendorf, Günther, and Cecilia Poletto. 2011. Hidden Verb Second: The case of Cimbrian. In German-Language Speech Islands: Generative and Structural Approaches. Edited by Mike Putnam. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 301–46. [Google Scholar]
- Grewendorf, Günther. 2005. The discourse configurationality of scrambling. In The Free Word Order Phenomenon: Its Syntactic Sources and Diversity. Edited by Joachim Sabel and Mamoru Saito. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter, pp. 75–135. [Google Scholar]
- Gruber, Bettina. 2008. Complementiser Agreement New Evidence from the Upper Austrian Variant of Gmunden. Master’s thesis, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria. [Google Scholar]
- Haegemann, Liliane. 1992. Theory and Description in Generative Syntax: A Case Study in West Flemish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Halle, Morris. 1997. Distributed Morphology: Impoverishment and Fission. In PF: Papers at the Interface. Edited by Benjamin Bruening, Yoonjung Kang and Martha McGinnis. Cambridge: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, pp. 425–49. [Google Scholar]
- Lazzeroni, Romano. 1994. Rileggendo Benveniste: Le relazioni di persona nel verbo indoeuropeo. Rivista di Linguistica 6: 267–74. [Google Scholar]
- Ledgeway, Adam. 2003. Il sistema completivo dei dialetti meridionali: La doppia serie di complementatori. Rivista Italiana di Dialettologia 25: 89–145. [Google Scholar]
- Manzini, Maria Rita, and Leonardo Savoia. 2005. I Dialetti Italiani e Romanci: Morfosintassi Generativa. Alessandria: Edizioni dell’Orso. [Google Scholar]
- Manzini, Maria Rita. 2019. Parameters and the design of the language faculty: Northern Italian partial null subjects. Evolutionary Linguistic Theory 1: 24–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Munaro, Nicola, and Cecilia Poletto. 2003. Ways of Clausal Typing. In Proceedings of the XXIX IGG. Edited by Gloria Cocchi and C.aterina Donati. Padova: Unipress, pp. 87–106. [Google Scholar]
- Munaro, Nicola, and Cecilia Poletto. 2006. On the diachronic origin of particles in North-Eastern Italian dialects. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 28: 247–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Noyer, Rolf. 1992. Features, Positions, and Affixes in Autonomous Morphological Structure. Ph.D. dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Padovan, Andrea, Ermenegildo Bidese, and Alessandra Tomaselli. 2021. Circumventing the ‘That-Trace’ Effect: Different Strategies between Germanic and Romance. Languages 6: 84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panieri, Luca, Monica Pedrazza, Adelia Nicolussi Baiz, Sabine Hipp, and Cristina Pruner. 2006. Bar lirnen z’schraiba un zo reda az be biar. Grammatica del cimbro di Luserna/Grammatik der zimbrischen Sprache von Lúsern. Trento: Regione Autonoma Trentino-Alto Adige/Autonome Region Trentino-Südtirol & Istituto Cimbro/Kulturinstitut Lúsern. [Google Scholar]
- Paoli, Sandra. 2003. Comp and the Left Periphery: Comparative Evidence from Romance. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Poletto, Cecilia. 1995. Null Subjects Without Pro. Oslo: GLOW 1995 University of Oslo. [Google Scholar]
- Poletto, Cecilia, and Christina Tortora. 2016. Subject clitics: Syntax. In The Oxford Guide to the Romance Languages. Edited by Adam Ledgeway and Martin Maiden. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 772–85. [Google Scholar]
- Poletto, Cecilia. 1993. La Sintassi del Soggetto nei Dialetti Dell’italia Settentrionale. Padova: Unipress. [Google Scholar]
- Poletto, Cecilia. 2000. The Higher Functional Field. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Portner, Paul, Miok Pak, and Raffaella Zanuttini. 2019. The Speaker-Addressee Relation in Imperatives. In Proceedings of the 14th Workshop on Altaic Formal Linguistics (WAFL14). Edited by T. Bondarenko, C. Davis, J. Colley and D. Privoznov. Cambridge: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, pp. 219–38. [Google Scholar]
- Renzi, Lorenzo, and Laura Vanelli. 1983. I pronomi soggetto in alcune varietà romanze. In Scritti linguistici in onore di Giovan Battista Pellegrini. Pisa: Pacini, pp. 121–45. [Google Scholar]
- Rizzi, Luigi. 1991. Residual V2 and the WH-Criterion, Geneva Generative Papers 2: 102–14. In Parameters and Functional Heads. 1996. Edited by Adriana Belletti and Luigi Rizzi. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 63–90. [Google Scholar]
- Rizzi, Luigi. 1982. Issues in Italian Syntax. Dordrecht: Foris. [Google Scholar]
- Shlonsky, Ur. 1994. Agreement in COMP. The Linguistic Review 11: 351–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sigurdðsson, Hálldor. 2011. Conditions on argument drop. Linguistic Inquiry 42: 267–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tomaselli, Alessandra, and Ermenegildo Bidese. 2019. Subject clitic languages in comparison: Subject clitics, finite verb movement, and nominative case assignment in Germanic and Romance varieties. In La linguistica Vista Dalle Alpi/Linguistic Views from the Alps. Edited by Ermenegildo Bidese, Jan Casalicchio and Manuela Caterina Moroni. Berlin: Peter Lang, pp. 43–61. [Google Scholar]
- Tomaselli, Alessandra. 1986. Das unpersönliches es—Eine Analyse im Rahmen der Generativen Grammatik. Linguistische Berichte 102: 171–90. [Google Scholar]
- van Gelderen, Elly. 2011. The Linguistic Cycle. Language Change and the Language Faculty. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Walkden, George. 2013. Null subjects in Old English. Language Variation and Change 25: 155–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Weise, Oskar. 1900. Syntax der Altenburger Mundart. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel. [Google Scholar]
- Weiβ, Helmut. 1998. Syntax des Bairischen. Studien zur Grammatik einer natürlichen Sprache. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- Weiβ, Helmut. 2005. Inflected Complementizers in Continental West Germanic Dialects. Zeitschrift für Dialektologie und Linguistik 72: 148–66. [Google Scholar]
- Weiβ, Helmut. 2016. Pronominalsyntax deutscher Dialekte. In Syntaktische Variation—Areallinguistische Perspektiven. Edited by Alexandra Lenz and Franz Patocka. Wien: Wiener Linguistische Arbeiten, pp. 89–116. [Google Scholar]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Poletto, C.; Tomaselli, A. Resilient Subject Agreement Morpho-Syntax in the Germanic Romance Contact Area. Languages 2021, 6, 119. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6030119
Poletto C, Tomaselli A. Resilient Subject Agreement Morpho-Syntax in the Germanic Romance Contact Area. Languages. 2021; 6(3):119. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6030119
Chicago/Turabian StylePoletto, Cecilia, and Alessandra Tomaselli. 2021. "Resilient Subject Agreement Morpho-Syntax in the Germanic Romance Contact Area" Languages 6, no. 3: 119. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6030119
APA StylePoletto, C., & Tomaselli, A. (2021). Resilient Subject Agreement Morpho-Syntax in the Germanic Romance Contact Area. Languages, 6(3), 119. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages6030119