The Genesis of Spanish /θ/: A Revised Model
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Previous Analyses
3. The Linguistic Background
4. Materials and Methods
5. Results
6. Discussion
6.1. Dissibilation of Denti-Alveolar / / (< /dz/) to /ð/ by the Early 1500s
6.2. Parallel Emergence of /θ/
Los de Castilla la Vieja dizen hacer y en Toledo hazer […]‘The inhabitants of Old Castile say hacer but in Toledo they say hazer’(Del arte en lengua zapoteca, fol. 68)
6.3. Spanish /θ/ Originally Dental Rather than Interdental
Suena a manera de c poniendo el pico de la lengua entre los dientes altos y baxos de manera que suena como pronuncian la ce los ceceosos.‘It sounds like c but putting the tip of the tongue between the upper and lower teeth, so that it sounds like the way people with sigmatism pronounce ce.’(Vocabulista arauigo en letra castellana, fol. a. iii v)
6.4. Causation
7. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Appendix B
1 | In the general case, the [tj] sequence arose in words like puteum ‘well’ and vitium ‘vice’ once the unstressed front vowel had ceased to be syllabic. It also evolved, in many dialects of spoken Latin, from the palatals [cj] (brachium ‘arm’) and [cj] (vicinum ‘neighbor’). Where the latter development occurred, the relevant modern language usually has the same sound (or type of sound) in the reflexes of words like brachium and vicinum as in the reflexes of words like puteum and vitium (cf. Castilian Spanish brazo, vecino, pozo and vicio/vezo, all with /θ/). |
2 | ‘El desplazamiento de ambos fonemas reside en el intento de ampliar al margen de seguridad respecto a la ápico-alveolar /s/’. |
3 | ‘La fricatización y el ensordecimiento produjeron que tres fonemas sibilantes, los tres fricativos y de articulación muy próxima, corriesen el peligro de confundirse’. |
4 | ‘A comienzos del siglo XVI ya se generalizaba en muchas regiones de la Península la pronunciación interdental, simplemente fricativa θ y ẓ: plaça, hazer. Ambos sonidos se confundieron a partir del siglo XVII en un solo sordo, perdiéndose el sonoro.’ |
5 | Little can be known with certainty about these reflexes, but the prevailing assumption in the literature (Lapesa 1981; Penny 2002; Dworkin 2018, etc.) is that they were laminal denti-alveolar sibilants, transcribable as / / and / /, respectively. |
6 | The Latin digraph th was presumably articulated as [tʰ] by at least some, no doubt highly educated, speakers. However, the aspirated articulation does not seem to have been normal among the general populace, at least not in northern central Iberia, where Spanish emerged. This can be inferred from the fact that, in the history of Spanish, the phonological correlate of Latin th evolves in exactly the same way as does the phonological correlate of Latin t. Compare, for example, cathedram > cadera ‘hip’ with catēnam > cadena ‘chain’. |
7 | This process of orthographical substitution did not apply to cultismos like admirar and advertir, where the preconsonantal d was directly modelled on the Latin spelling. |
8 | The phonemic status of this /ð/ stems from the fact that the letter z represented a discrete phoneme. Moreover, despite the encroachment of z into an orthographic space previously occupied by d, the sounds represented by these letters must have continued to contrast phonemically. For example, under the assumptions advanced here, minimal pairs such as lazo ‘loop/noose/trap’ and lado ‘side’ would have been distinguished on the basis of a /ð/–/d/ contrast, which, following the devoicing of the coronal fricatives, is now a /θ/–/d/ contrast. |
9 | That the two sounds differed minimally at the time is independently confirmed in Juan de Valdés’s Diálogo de la lengua (c. 1535), where it is noted that applying the cedilla to the letter c ‘la haze sonar cassy como .z.’ (‘makes it sound almost like z’) (MSS/8629, fol. 60r). |
10 | Where a manuscript’s known or estimated copy date is a range rather than a specific date, it has been assigned on the basis of the mid-point in the range. For example, the PhiloBiblon database (Faulhaber 1997–) gives the range 1436–1450 as the copy date for the manuscript of the Libro de los ejemplos por A.B.C. used in this study. The mid-point in this range is 1443, so the text is assigned to the 1430–1449 period. |
11 | This excludes, therefore, the z in words like diezmo ‘tithe’, where it corresponds to a (palatalized) Latin velar, viz., the /k/ (= [cj]) in decimum. Conversely, the d of cultismos like admirar and advertir is also excluded, as this d was never replaced by z. |
12 | If the data are arranged as a three-column matrix in a text file named ‘dataset.txt’, with headers ‘date’, ‘yes’ (= count of z) and ‘no’ (= count of d), the relevant command lines are the ones shown below:
|
13 | The coefficients in Table 5 are based on 1220 (the mid-point in the first twenty-year period) being treated as year zero, with all subsequent time values recalibrated accordingly (i.e., 1260 becomes year 40, 1280 become year 60, etc.). The choice of year zero is mathematically arbitrary and does not affect the regression analysis per se (Kroch 1989, p. 225), because the values of the logistic function go from minus infinity to plus infinity. However, for the purpose of plotting the fitted logistic curve on a line chart, as in Figure 1, the values assigned to x in the logistic equation must match the time value recalibration that stems from the particular choice of year zero. |
14 | When represented as a logistic curve, the initial and final years of a linguistic change are, by definition, statistical outliers and hence should not be thought of as being integral components of the change event. The real-world analogue of this is that speakers experiencing the change would be unlikely to perceive a highly infrequent variant, be it an innovative one at the start of the change or a conservative one at the end, as evidence of genuine variation. |
15 | Conceivably, this early modern /ð/ is also the source of the /ð/, notated as d, which occurs in Chinato, the nearly extinct dialect of Extremaduran spoken in Malpartida de Plasencia (see Ariza 1995–1996). It should be noted, however, that in that dialect, /ð/ occurs not just in words which in Old Spanish had /dz/ but also in those which had /z/ (< Latin /s/); for example, didil ‘to say’ (Old Spanish: dezir [deˈdziɾ]) and cada ‘house’ (Old Spanish: casa [ˈkaza]). |
16 | ‘Neque sunt ridendi minus fere omnes galli qui huius litterae sonum cum s. littera confundunt.’ |
17 | ‘[…] ad supernorum dentium radices lingua illisa sonum reddit.’ |
18 | This can be seen, for example, in the differential manner in which ceceo and ceceoso are defined in the Real Academia Española’s Diccionario de Autoridades (1726). The first term is defined there purely in linguistic terms, viz., as ‘la pronunciacion de la persona que trueca la S en C’ (‘the pronunciation of someone who merges S with C’) and the entry includes a reference to a verse from Quevedo that mocks the ‘cecéos’ of Andalusians. In contrast, a ceceoso is defined as someone who suffers from a natural disorder, albeit one which has a linguistic consequence: ‘el que naturalmente y sin poderlo remediar muda en las palabras la pronunciacion de la S en C’ (‘that person who naturally and without being able to remedy it changes, within words, the pronunciation of S into C’). |
19 | Sed nos illos hac una in re superamus: quod utramque vocem possumus efferre: illi vero inemendabili oris pravitate non possunt.’ |
20 | ‘A medida que el lugar de articulación va avanzando y se sitúa en la proximidad dental, la estridencia va disminuyendo, dejando paso a la cualidad de mate, que se hace patente en el espectro de la [s] predorsodentoalveolar […] La característica mate lleva consigo una distribución más regular de las regiones de frecuencias, distribución que origina unos espectros semejantes a los de [θ]’. |
References
- Alarcos Llorach, Emilio. 1988. De nuevo sobre los cambios fonéticos del siglo XVI. In Actas del I Congreso Internacional de Historia de la Lengua Española. Edited by Manuel Ariza, Antonio Salvador and Antonio Vindas. Madrid: Arco Libros, pp. 47–59. [Google Scholar]
- Alarcos Llorach, Emilio. 1991. Fonología española, 4th ed. Madrid: Gredos. [Google Scholar]
- Altmann, Gabriel, Haro Buttlar, Walter Rott, and Udo Strauss. 1983. A law of change in language. In Historical Linguistics. Edited by Barron Brainerd. Bochum: Studienverlag Dr. N. Brockmeyer, pp. 104–15. [Google Scholar]
- Ariza, Manuel. 1995–1996. De nuevo sobre Serradilla y el chinato. Cauce. Revista de Filología y su Didáctica 18–19: 689–701. [Google Scholar]
- Ariza, Manuel. 2012. Fonología y fonética históricas del español. Madrid: Arco Libros. [Google Scholar]
- Blevins, Juliette. 2004. Evolutionary Phonology. The Emergence of Sound Patterns. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Canellada, María Josefa, and John Kuhlmann Madsen. 1987. Pronunciación del Español. Lengua Hablada y Literaria. Madrid: Castalia. [Google Scholar]
- Dalbor, John B. 1980. Observations on present-day seseo and ceceo in Southern Spain. Hispania 63: 5–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dworkin, Steven N. 2018. A Guide to Old Spanish. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Faulhaber, Charles B. 1997–. PhiloBiblon. Available online: https://bancroft.berkeley.edu/philobiblon/index.html (accessed on 11 November 2021).
- Fruehwald, Josef, Jonathan Gress-Wright, and Joel C. Wallenberg. 2009. Phonological change: The constant rate effect. In North East Linguistic Society (NELS) 40. Amherst: GLSA. [Google Scholar]
- Gago Jover, Francisco. 2011. Digital Library of Old Spanish Texts. Hispanic Seminary of Medieval Studies. Available online: http://www.hispanicseminary.org/textconc-en.htm (accessed on 11 November 2021).
- Hinzelin, Marc-Olivier. 2018. Contact-induced change in Franco-provençal phonological systems caused by standard French. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 249: 49–70. [Google Scholar]
- Guitarte, Guillermo. 1992. Cecear y palabras afines. In Actas del II Congreso Internacional de Historia de la Lengua Española Tomo I. Edited by Manuel Ariza, Rafael Cano, Josefa Mendoza and Antonio Narbona. Madrid: Pabellón de España, pp. 127–64. [Google Scholar]
- Hualde, José Ignacio. 1991. Basque Phonology. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Hualde, José Ignacio. 2005. The Sounds of Spanish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kauhanen, Henri, and George Walkden. 2018. Deriving the constant rate effect. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 36: 483–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kroch, Anthony. 1989. Reflexes of grammar in patterns of language change. Language Variation and Change 1: 199–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ladefoged, Peter, and Ian Maddieson. 1996. The Sounds of the World’s Languages. Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Ladefoged, Peter, and Keith Johnson. 2014. A Course in Phonetics, 7th ed. Boston: Cengage. [Google Scholar]
- Lapesa, Rafael. 1981. Historia de la lengua española, 9th ed. Madrid: Gredos. [Google Scholar]
- Martinet, André. 1951. The Unvoicing of Old Spanish Sibilants. Philology 5: 133–56. [Google Scholar]
- Menéndez Pidal, Ramón. 1987. Manual de gramática histórica española, 19th ed. Madrid: Espasa Calpe. [Google Scholar]
- Navarro Tomás, Tomás, Aurelio M Espinosa, and L. Rodríguez Castellano. 1933. La frontera del andaluz. Revista de Filología Española 20: 225–77. [Google Scholar]
- Noll, Volker. 2021. The Emergence of Latin American Spanish. In English and Spanish: World Languages in Interaction. Edited by Danae Perez, Marianne Hundt, Johannes Kabatek and Daniel Schreier. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 76–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Núñez Méndez, Eva. 2016. A diachronic approach to the Old Spanish sibilant merger and its impact on trans-Atlantic Spanish (part I). Revista de Estudios Hispánicos 3: 59–98. [Google Scholar]
- Ohala, John. 1981. The listener as a source of sound change. In Papers from the Parasession on Language and Behavior. Edited by Carrie S. Masek, Roberta A. Hendrick and Mary Frances Miller. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society, pp. 178–203. [Google Scholar]
- Ohala, John. 2012. The listener as a source of sound change. An update. In The Initiation of Sound Change: Perception, Production, and Social Factors. Edited by Maria-Josep Solé and Daniel Recasens. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 21–36. [Google Scholar]
- O’Neill, John. 1999. Electronic Texts and Concordances of the Madison Corpus of Early Spanish Manuscripts and Printings. CD-ROM. Madison: Hispanic Seminary of Medieval Studies. [Google Scholar]
- Penny, Ralph. 2002. A History of the Spanish Language, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Postma, Gertjan. 2010. The impact of failed changes. In Continuity and Change in Grammar. Anne Breitbarth, Christopher Lucas, Sheila Watts and David Willis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 269–302. [Google Scholar]
- Quilis, Antonio. 1981. Fonética Acústica de la Lengua Española. Madrid: Gredos. [Google Scholar]
- Ranson, Diana L., and Margaret Lubbers Quesada. 2018. The History of Spanish: A Student’s Introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Rost Bagudanch, Assumpció. 2022. More on Sibilant Devoicing in Spanish Diachrony: An Initial Phonetic Approach. Languages 7: 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Villena Ponsoda, Juan Andrés, José María Sánchez Sáez, and Antonio Manuel Ávila Muñoz. 1994–1995. Modelos probabilísticos multinomiales para el estudio del ceceo, seseo y distinción de /s/ y /θ/. Datos de la ciudad de Málaga. Estudios de Lingüística Universidad de Alicante 10: 391–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Widdison, Kirk A. 1987. 16th Century Spanish sibilant reordering: Reasons for divergence. Deseret Language and Linguistic Society Symposium 13: 63–70. [Google Scholar]
- Yang, Charles D. 2000. Internal and external forces in language change. Language Variation and Change 12: 231–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zamboni, Alberto. 1974. Veneto (Profilo dei Dialetti Italiani, 5). Pisa: Pacini. [Google Scholar]
Denti-Alveolar Sibilant (/ /) | Apical-Alveolar Sibilant (/ /) |
---|---|
caçar ‘to hunt’ | esso ‘this’ |
pozo ‘well’ | casa ‘house’ |
Prevocalic (ç, c or z) | Preconsonantal (z) | Final (z) |
---|---|---|
cabeça ‘head’ | diezmo ‘tithe’ | foz ‘sickle’ |
[kaˈbetsa] | [ˈdjedzmo] | [fots] |
tercero ‘third’ | lobezno ‘wolf cub’ | assaz ‘enough’ |
[teɾˈtseɾo] | [loˈbedzno] | [aˈsats] |
pereza ‘sloth’ | bizconde ‘viscount’ | ueiez ‘old age’ |
[peˈɾedza] | [bitsˈkonde] | [βeˈʒets] |
Time Period | Unique Time Value Assigned |
---|---|
1210–1229 | 1220 |
1250–1269 | 1260 |
1270–1289 | 1280 |
1310–1329 | 1320 |
1350–1369 | 1360 |
1370–1389 | 1380 |
1410–1429 | 1420 |
1430–1449 | 1440 |
1450–1469 | 1460 |
1470–1489 | 1480 |
1490–1509 | 1500 |
1510–1529 | 1520 |
1530–1549 | 1540 |
Time Period | z | z + d | Probability of z (%) |
---|---|---|---|
1210–1229 | 0 | 24 | 0.0 |
1250–1269 | 2 | 1145 | 0.2 |
1270–1289 | 0 | 236 | 0.0 |
1310–1329 | 0 | 49 | 0.0 |
1350–1369 | 1 | 111 | 0.9 |
1370–1389 | 0 | 2 | 0.0 |
1410–1429 | 8 | 73 | 11.0 |
1430–1449 | 19 | 79 | 24.1 |
1450–1469 | 27 | 105 | 25.7 |
1470–1489 | 258 | 431 | 59.9 |
1490–1509 | 195 | 236 | 82.6 |
1510–1529 | 35 | 36 | 97.2 |
1530–1549 | 105 | 107 | 98.1 |
Coefficient | Estimate | Standard Error | z-Value | Probability |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | −11.431566 | 0.805484 | −14.19 | <2 × 10−16 *** |
Slope | 0.045686 | 0.003108 | 14.70 | <2 × 10−16 *** |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Mackenzie, I. The Genesis of Spanish /θ/: A Revised Model. Languages 2022, 7, 191. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030191
Mackenzie I. The Genesis of Spanish /θ/: A Revised Model. Languages. 2022; 7(3):191. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030191
Chicago/Turabian StyleMackenzie, Ian. 2022. "The Genesis of Spanish /θ/: A Revised Model" Languages 7, no. 3: 191. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030191
APA StyleMackenzie, I. (2022). The Genesis of Spanish /θ/: A Revised Model. Languages, 7(3), 191. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030191