A Comparative Analysis of Language Skills and Parent–Child Interactions in Monolingual and Bilingual Children Born Preterm
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Language Skills in Preterm Children
1.2. Parent–Child Interactions in Preterm Children
1.3. Bilingualism Effects on Language and Parent–Child Interactions in Preterm Children
1.4. Current Study and Hypotheses
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.2.1. Language Development Survey
2.2.2. Parent–Child Interaction Activity
2.2.3. Transcription and Coding
2.3. Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Wilcoxon Rank Sum Tests Comparing Monolinguals and Bilinguals
3.1.1. Vocabulary
3.1.2. Mother–Child Interactions
3.2. Spearman Correlations Testing Relationships Between Parent Input, Conversational Turns, and Language Outcomes
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Achenbach, Thomas Max, and Leslie Altman Rescorla. 2000. Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms & Profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont. [Google Scholar]
- Adams, Katherine A., Virgina A. Marchman, Elizabeth C. Loi, Melanie D. Ashland, Anne Fernald, and Heidi M. Feldman. 2018. Caregiver talk and medical risk as predictors of language outcomes in full term and preterm toddlers. Child Development 89: 1674–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anderson, Nina J., Susan A. Graham, Heather Prime, Jennifer M. Jenkins, and Sheri Madigan. 2021. Linking Quality and Quantity of Parental Linguistic Input to Child Language Skills: A Meta-Analysis. Child Development 92: 484–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anthopolos, Rebecca, Jay S. Kaufman, Lynne C. Messer, and Marie Lynn Miranda. 2014. Racial residential segregation and preterm birth: Built environment as a mediator. Epidemiology 25: 397–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arias, Graciela, and Jennifer Friberg. 2017. Bilingual Language Assessment: Contemporary Versus Recommended Practice in American Schools. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 48: 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barak, Lara, Tamar Degani, and Rama Novogrodsky. 2022. Influences of bilingualism and developmental language disorder on how children learn and process words. Developmental Psychology 58: 821–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baralt, Melissa, and Ashley Darcy Mahoney. 2020. Bilingualism and the executive function advantage in preterm-born children. Cognitive Development 55: 100931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barre, Natalie, Angela Morgan, Lex W. Doyle, and Peter J. Anderson. 2011. Language abilities in children who were very preterm and/or very low birth weight: A meta-analysis. The Journal of Pediatrics 158: 766–74.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bayley, Nancy. 1993. Bayley Scale of Infants Development. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Bayley, Nancy. 2006. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3rd ed. New York: Harcourt Assessment. [Google Scholar]
- Bedore, Lisa M., and Elizabeth D. Peña. 2008. Assessment of bilingual children for identification of language impairment: Current findings and implications for practice. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 11: 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bhutta, Adnan T., Mario A. Cleves, Patrick H. Casey, Mary M. Cradock, and Karthika J. Anand. 2002. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: A meta-analysis. JAMA 288: 728–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bishop, Dorothy V., Margaret J. Snowling, Paul A. Thompson, Trisha Greenhalgh, and Catalise Consortium. 2016. CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study. Identifying language impairments in children. PLoS ONE 11: e0158753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bishop, Dorothy V., Margaret J. Snowling, Paul A. Thompson, Trisha Greenhalgh, and The CATALISE-2 Consortium. 2017. Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 58: 1068–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blom, Elma. 2010. Effects of input on the early grammatical development of bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism 14: 422–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blom, Elma, Paula Fikkert, Annette Scheper, Merel van Witteloostuijn, and Petra van Alphen. 2023. The Language Environment at Home of Children With (a Suspicion of) a Developmental Language Disorder and Relations with Standardized Language Measures. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 66: 2821–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bornstein, Marc H., Chun-Shin Hahn, and Joan T. D. Suwalsky. 2014. Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Social Status and the Socioeconomic Index of Occupations. In Socioeconomic Status, Parenting, and Child Development. Abingdon: Taylor and Francis. [Google Scholar]
- Bos, Arend F., Koenraad N. J. A. Van Braeckel, Marrit M. Hitzert, Jozien C. Tanis, and Elise Roze. 2013. Development of fine motor skills in preterm infants. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 55: 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brito, Natalie H., Sonya V. Troller-Renfree, Ana Leon-Santos, Joseph R. Isler, William P. Fifer, and Kimberly G. Noble. 2020. Associations among the home language environment and neural activity during infancy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 43: 100780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Britsch, Emily Roemer, and Jana M. Iverson. 2024. Adding to the Conversation: Language Delays and Parent-Child Interactions in the Younger Siblings of Children with Autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brojde, Chandra L., Sabeen Ahmed, and Eliana Colunga. 2012. Bilingual and monolingual children attend to different cues when learning new words. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brósch-Fohraheim, Nina, Renate Fuiko, Peter B. Marschik, and Bernhard Resch. 2019. The influence of preterm birth on expressive vocabulary at the age of 36 to 41 months. Medicine 98: e14404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castagna, Annalisa, Niccolò Butti, Laura Cordolcini, Mark S. Innocenti, and Rosario Montirosso. 2024. Severity of developmental delay and parenting behavior in toddlers with neurodevelopmental disabilities. Frontiers in Psychology 14: 1306227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, Roseanne. 1999. The parent–child early relational assessment: A factorial validity study. Educational and Psychological Measurement 59: 821–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Committee on Fetus and Newborn. 2004. Age Terminology During the Perinatal Period. Pediatrics 114: 1362–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cote, Linda R., and Marc H. Bornstein. 2009. Child and mother play in three U.S. cultural groups: Comparisons and associations. Journal of Family Psychology 23: 355–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Coughlan, Sarah, Jean Quigley, and Elizabeth Nixon. 2024. Parent-infant conversations are differentially associated with the development of preterm- and term-born infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 239: 105809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Lamo White, Caroline, and Lixian Jin. 2011. Evaluation of speech and language assessment approaches with bilingual children. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 46: 613–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Leeuw, Stéphanie, Gilda Delens, Laura Vanden Brande, Elisabeth Henrion, and Ludovic Legros. 2024. Socio-familial environment influence on cognitive and language development in very preterm children. Child: Care, Health and Development 50: e13239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Desmarais, Chantal, Audette Sylvestre, François Meyer, Isabelle Bairati, and Nancie Rouleau. 2008. Systematic review of the literature on characteristics of late-talking toddlers. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 43: 361–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeVeney, Shari L., Jessica L. Hagaman, and Abby L. Bjornsen. 2017. Parent-Implemented Versus Clinician-Directed Interventions for Late-Talking Toddlers: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Communication Disorders Quarterly 39: 293–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Donnelly, Seamus, and Evan Kidd. 2021. The longitudinal relationship between conversational turn-taking and vocabulary growth in early language development. Child Development 92: 609–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ebert, Kerry D. 2020. Language Sample Analysis with bilingual children: Translating research to practice. Topics in Language Disorders 40: 182–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Escobar, Gabriel J., John D. Greene, Peter Hulac, Elizabeth Kincannon, K. Bischoff, Marla N. Gardner, Mary Anne Armstrong, and E. France. 2005. Rehospitalisation after birth hospitalisation: Patterns among infants of all gestations. Archives of Disease in Childhood 90: 125–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gampe, Anja, Leonie Hartmann, and Moritz M. Daum. 2020. Dynamic interaction patterns of monolingual and bilingual infants with their parents. Journal of Child Language 47: 45–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gampe, Anja, Stephanie Wermelinger, and Moritz M. Daum. 2019. Bilingual Children Adapt to the Needs of Their Communication Partners, Monolinguals Do Not. Child Development 90: 98–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gangopadhyay, Ishanti, and Margarita Kaushanskaya. 2020. The role of speaker eye gaze and mutual exclusivity in novel word learning by monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 197: 104878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gillenson, Caroline J., Daniel M. Bagner, Ashley Darcy Mahoney, and Melissa Baralt. 2023. A Preliminary Study of Executive Functioning in Preterm-Born Children: A Bilingual Advantage. Advances in Neonatal Care: Official Journal of the National Association of Neonatal Nurses 23: E121–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Girolametto, Luigi, Patsy Steig Pearce, and Elaine Weitzman. 1995. The effects of focused stimulation for promoting vocabulary in young children with delays: A pilot study. Journal of Children’s Communication Development 17: 39–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Girolamo, Teresa, and Mabel L. Rice. 2022. Language Impairment in Autistic Adolescents and Young Adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 65: 3518–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greenfield, Patricia M., Elise Trumbull, Heidi Keller, Carrie Rothstein-Fisch, Lalita Suzuki, and Blanca Quiroz. 2006. Cultural conceptions of learning and development. In Handbook of Educational Psychology. Edited by Patricia. A. Alexander and Philip H. Winne. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 675–92. [Google Scholar]
- Grosjean, François. 1998. Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 1: 131–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosjean, François. 2001. Life with Two Languages: An Introduction to Bilingualism. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gross, Megan, Milijana Buac, and Margarita Kaushanskaya. 2014. Conceptual scoring of receptive and expressive vocabulary measures in simultaneous and sequential bilingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 23: 574–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gutiérrez-Clellen, Vera F., Maria Adelaida Restrepo, Lisa Bedore, Elizabeth Peña, and Raquel Anderson. 2000. Language sample analysis in Spanish-speaking children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 31: 88–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hammer, Carol Scheffner, Megan Dunn Davison, Frank R. Lawrence, and Adele W. Miccio. 2009. The Effect of Maternal Language on Bilingual Children’s Vocabulary and Emergent Literacy Development During Head Start and Kindergarten. Scientific Studies of Reading 13: 99–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harrell, Frank E., Jr. 2024. Hmisc: Harrell Miscellaneous. [R Package]. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=Hmisc (accessed on 12 November 2024).
- Hibbard, Judith U., Isabelle Wilkins, Liping Sun, Kimberly Gregory, Shoshana Haberman, Matthew Hoffman, Michelle A. Kominiarek, Uma Reddy, Jennifer Bailit, D. Ware Branch, and et al. 2010. Respiratory morbidity in late preterm births. JAMA 304: 419–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy, Lauren B. Adamson, Roger Bakeman, Margaret Tresch Owen, Roberta Michnick Golinkoff, Amy Pace, Paula K. S. Yust, and Katharine Suma. 2015. The Contribution of Early Communication Quality to Low-Income Children’s Language Success. Psychological Science 26: 1071–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hoff, Erika. 2006. How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review 26: 55–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika. 2013. Language development and language impairment in children with early language delay: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 56: 1231–44. [Google Scholar]
- Hoff, Erika, and Katherine F. Shanks. 2024. Mother–Child Conversations of Latina Immigrant and U.S.-Born Mothers in the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 55: 74–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika, and Krystal M. Ribot. 2017. Language growth in English monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual children from 2.5 to 5 years. The Journal of Pediatrics 190: 241–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika, and Letitia Naigles. 2002. How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development 73: 418–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika, Cynthia Core, and Katherine F. Shanks. 2020. The quality of child-directed speech depends on the speaker’s language proficiency. Journal of Child Language 47: 132–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika, Cynthia Core, Silvia Place, Rosario Rumiche, Melissa Señor, and Marisol Parra. 2012. Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language 39: 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoffman, Lois Wladis. 2014. Methodological Issues in Studies of SES, parenting, and Child Development. In Socioeconomic Status, Parenting, and Child Development. Abingdon: Taylor and Francis. [Google Scholar]
- Höhle, Barbara, Ranka Bijeljac-Babic, and Thierry Nazzi. 2020. Variability and stability in early language acquisition: Comparing monolingual and bilingual infants’ speech perception and word recognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 23: 56–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huttenlocher, Janellen, Marina Vasilyeva, Elina Cymerman, and Susan Levine. 2002. Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology 45: 337–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kassambara, Alboukadel. 2021. _rstatix: Pipe-Friendly Framework for Basic Statistical Tests_. R Package Version 0.7.0. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rstatix (accessed on 1 January 2020).
- Kremin, Lena V., Julia Alves, Adriel John Orena, Linda Polka, and Krista Byers-Heinlein. 2022. Code-switching in parents’ everyday speech to bilingual infants. Journal of Child Language 49: 714–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Levickis, Penny, Sheena Reilly, Luigi Girolametto, Obioha C. Ukoumunne, and Melissa Wake. 2018. Maternal behaviors promoting language acquisition in slow-to-talk toddlers: Prospective community-based study. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics 39: 544–53. [Google Scholar]
- Loe, Irene M., and Heidi M. Feldman. 2016. The effect of bilingual exposure on executive function skills in preterm and full-term preschoolers. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 37: 548–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lorang, Emily K., and Audra Sterling. 2021. Parent Command Use and Child Compliance During Parent-Child Interactions Including Children With Down Syndrome. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 30: 1203–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luo, Rufan, Kelly Escobar, and Catherine S. Tamis-LeMonda. 2020. Heterogeneity in the trajectories of US Latine mothers’ dual-language input from infancy to preschool. First Language 40: 275–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luu, Thuy Mai, Betty R. Vohr, Karen C. Schneider, Karol H. Katz, Richard Tucker, Walter C. Allan, and Laura R. Ment. 2009. Trajectories of receptive language development from 3 to 12 years of age for very preterm children. Pediatrics 124: 333–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luu, Thuy Mai, Betty R. Vohr, Walter Allan, Karen C. Schneider, and Laura R. Ment. 2011. Evidence for catch-up in cognition and receptive vocabulary among adolescents born very preterm. Pediatrics 128: 313–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maalouf, Elia F., Philip J. Duggan, Serena J. Counsell, Mary A. Rutherford, Frances Cowan, Denis Azzopardi, and A. David Edwards. 2001. Comparison of findings on cranial ultrasound and magnetic resonance imaging in preterm infants. Pediatrics 107: 719–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maguire, Mandy J., Julie M. Schneider, Anna E. Middleton, Yvonne Ralph, Michael Lopez, Robert A. Ackerman, and Alyson D. Abel. 2018. Vocabulary knowledge mediates the link between socioeconomic status and word learning in grade school. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 166: 679–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Manning, Brittany L., Alexandra Harpole, Emily M. Harriott, Kamila Postolowicz, and Elizabeth S. Norton. 2020. Taking Language Samples Home: Feasibility, Reliability, and Validity of Child Language Samples Conducted Remotely with Video Chat Versus In-Person. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 63: 3982–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Masho, Saba Woldemichael, Susan Cha, Derek A. Chapman, and David Chelmow. 2017. Understanding the role of violence as a social determinant of preterm birth. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 216: 183.e1–183.e7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mason-Apps, Emily, Vesna Stojanovik, Carmel Houston-Price, and Sue Buckley. 2018. Longitudinal predictors of early language in infants with Down syndrome: A preliminary study. Research in Developmental Disabilities 81: 37–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McBryde, Melinda, Grace C. Fitzallen, Helen G. Liley, Hudson Gerry Taylor, and Samudragupta Bora. 2020. Academic Outcomes of School-Aged Children Born Preterm: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA 3: e202027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miller, John, and Aquiles Iglesias. 2017. Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT). [Software]. Madison: SALT Software, LLC. [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell, Shelley, Jessica Brian, Lonnie Zwaigenbaum, Wendy Roberts, Peter Szatmari, Isabel Smith, and Susan Bryson. 2006. Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 27: S69–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Montagna, Anita, and Chiara Nosarti. 2016. Socio-Emotional Development Following Very Preterm Birth: Pathways to Psychopathology. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mossabeb, Roschanak, Kelly C. Wade, Kathryn Finnegan, Emidio Sivieri, and Soraya Abbasi. 2012. Language development survey provides a useful screening tool for language delay in preterm infants. Clinical Pediatrics 51: 638–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- NCH Software. 2019. Express Scribe Pro (Version 8.14). [Computer SoftWare]. NCH Software. Available online: https://www.nch.com.au/scribe/index.html (accessed on 1 January 2020).
- Ortiz-Mantilla, Silvia, Naseem Choudhury, Hilary Leevers, and April A. Benasich. 2008. Understanding language and cognitive deficits in very low birth weight children. Developmental Psychobiology 50: 107–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peterson, Bradley S., Betty Vohr, Michael J. Kane, Douglas H. Whalen, Karen C. Schneider, Karol H. Katz, Heping Zhang, Charles C. Duncan, Robert Makuch, John C. Gore, and et al. 2002. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of language processing and its cognitive correlates in prematurely born children. Pediatrics 110: 1153–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pham, Giang, and Kathryn Kohnert. 2014. A longitudinal study of lexical development in children learning Vietnamese and English. Child Development 85: 767–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poehlmann, Julie, A. J. Schwichtenberg, Daniel M. Bolt, Amanda Hane, Cynthia Burnson, and Jill Winters. 2011. Infant physiological regulation and maternal risks as predictors of dyadic interaction trajectories in families with a preterm infant. Developmental Psychology 47: 91–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Poehlmann-Tynan, Julie, Emily D. Gerstein, Cynthia Burnson, Lindsay Weymouth, Daniel M. Bolt, Sarah Maleck, and A. J. Schwichtenberg. 2015. Risk and resilience in preterm children at age 6. Development and Psychopathology 27: 843–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Putnick, Diane L., Marc H. Bornstein, Suna Eryigit-Madzwamuse, and Dieter Wolke. 2017. Long-Term Stability of Language Performance in Very Preterm, Moderate-Late Preterm, and Term Children. The Journal of Pediatrics 181: 74–79.e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- R Core Team. 2022. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 12 November 2024).
- R Studio Team. 2022. RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. Boston: RStudio, PBC. Available online: http://www.rstudio.com/ (accessed on 12 November 2024).
- Rescorla, Leslie. 1989. The Language Development Survey: A screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. The Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54: 587–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roberts, Megan Y., and Ann P. Kaiser. 2015. Early intervention for toddlers with language delays: A randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics 135: 686–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rojas, Raúl, and Aquiles Iglesias. 2013. The language growth of Spanish-speaking English Language Learners. Child Development 84: 630–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Romeo, Rachel R., Julia A. Leonard, Sydney T. Robinson, Martin R. West, Allyson P. Mackey, Meredith L. Rowe, and John D. E. Gabrieli. 2018. Beyond the 30-Million-Word Gap: Children’s Conversational Exposure Is Associated with Language-Related Brain Function. Psychological Science 29: 700–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rose, Karen, Sharon Armon-Lotem, and Carmit Altman. 2022. Profiling bilingual children: Using monolingual assessment to inform diagnosis. Journal of Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 53: 494–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowe, Meredith L. 2012. A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development 83: 1762–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowe, Meredith L., and Adriana Weisleder. 2020. Language development in the first three years: Learning from input. Annual Review of Linguistics 6: 215–34. [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez, Katherine, Alicia J. Spittle, Jeanie L. Cheong, Deanne K. Thompson, Lex W. Doyle, Peter J. Anderson, and Angela T. Morgan. 2019. Language in 2-year-old children born preterm and term: A cohort study. Archives of Disease in Childhood 104: 647–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shapiro-Mendoza, Carrie K., Kay M. Tomashek, Milton Kotelchuck, Wanda Barfield, Judith Weiss, and Stephen Evans. 2006. Risk factors for neonatal morbidity and mortality among “healthy,” late preterm newborns. Seminars in Perinatology 30: 54–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Silvey, Catriona, Özlem E. Demir-Lira, Susan Goldin-Meadow, and Stephen W. Raudenbush. 2021. Effects of time-varying parent input on children’s language outcomes differ for vocabulary and syntax. Psychological Science 32: 536–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, Jamie Mahurin, Laura Segebart DeThorne, Jessica A. R. Logan, Ron W. Channell, and Stephen A. Petrill. 2014. Impact of prematurity on language skills at school age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 57: 901–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, Jodie, Catherine A. Bent, Cherie C. Green, Alexandra Woollacott, and Kristelle Hudry. 2020. Non-native language proficiency may influence the responsiveness of bilingual parents towards young children with autism: A short report. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments 5: 2396941519899684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, Jodie, Tricia Eadie, Penny Levickis, Lesley Bretherton, and Sharon Goldfeld. 2018. Predictive validity of verbal and non-verbal communication and mother–child turn-taking at 12 months on language outcomes at 24 and 36 months in a cohort of infants experiencing adversity: A preliminary study. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 53: 969–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snowling, Margaret J., Fiona J. Duff, Hannah M. Nash, and Charles Hulme. 2016. Language profiles and literacy outcomes of children with resolving, emerging, or persisting language impairments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 57: 1360–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soraya, Maryam, Behrooz Mahmoodi-Bakhtiari, Zohreh Badiee, Yalda Kazemi, and B. Soleimani. 2012. The size of expressive lexicon in prematurely born children low-birth-weight with full-term 18 to 36 months children: A comparative study. Journal of Neuroscience and Behavioral Health 4: 33–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- St. Clair, Michelle C., Claire L. Forrest, Shaun Goh Kok Yew, and Jenny L. Gibson. 2019. Early Risk Factors and Emotional Difficulties in Children at Risk of Developmental Language Disorder: A Population Cohort Study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 62: 2750–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stoll, Barbara J., Nellie I. Hansen, Edward F. Bell, Michele C. Walsh, Waldemar A. Carlo, Seetha Shankaran, Abbot R. Laptook, Pablo J. Sánchez, Krisa P. Van Meurs, Myra Wyckoff, and et al. 2015. Trends in Care Practices, Morbidity, and Mortality of Extremely Preterm Neonates, 1993–2012. JAMA 314: 1039–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tamis-LeMonda, Catherine S., Lisa Baumwell, and Tonia Cristofaro. 2012. Parent–child conversations during play. First Language 32: 413–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tamis-LeMonda, Catherine S., Yana Kuchirko, and Lulu Song. 2014. Why is infant language learning facilitated by parental responsiveness? Current Directions in Psychological Science 23: 121–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, Hudson Gerry. 2020. Neurodevelopmental origins of social competence in very preterm children. Seminars in Fetal & Neonatal Medicine 25: 101108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- The HandBrake Team. 2022. HandBrake (Version 1.6.1) [Computer Software]. The HandBrake Team. Available online: https://handbrake.fr/ (accessed on 12 November 2024).
- Uljarević, Mirko, Napoleon Katsos, Kristelle Hudry, and Jenny L. Gibson. 2016. Practitioner Review: Multilingualism and neurodevelopmental disorders—An overview of recent research and discussion of clinical implications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines 57: 1205–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- van Noort-van der Spek, Inge L., Marie-Christine J. P. Franken, and Nynke Weisglas-Kuperus. 2012. Language functions in preterm-born children: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatrics 129: 745–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- van Veen, Sisco, Sebastiaan Remmers, Cornelieke Sandrine Hanan Aarnoudse-Moens, Jaap Oosterlaan, Anton H. van Kaam, and Aleid G. van Wassenaer-Leemhuis. 2019. Multilingualism was associated with lower cognitive outcomes in children who were born very and extremely preterm. Acta Paediatrica 108: 479–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandormael, Charlotte, Lucie Schoenhals, Petra S. Hüppi, Manuela Filippa, and Cristina Borradori Tolsa. 2019. Language in Preterm Born Children: Atypical Development and Effects of Early Interventions on Neuroplasticity. Neural Plasticity 2019: 6873270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Venker, Courtney E., and Jennifer R. Johnson. 2022. Electronic Toys Decrease the Quantity and Lexical Diversity of Spoken Language Produced by Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Age-Matched Children with Typical Development. Frontiers in Psychology 13: 929589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verhagen, Josje, Susanne Grassmann, and Aylin C. Küntay. 2017. Monolingual and bilingual children’s resolution of referential conflicts: Effects of bilingualism and relative language proficiency. Cognitive Development 41: 10–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vohr, Betty. 2014. Speech and language outcomes of very preterm infants. Seminars in Fetal & Neonatal Medicine 19: 78–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walch, Elisabeth, Tanja Chaudhary, Brigit Herold, and Michael Obladen. 2009. Parental bilingualism is associated with slower cognitive development in very low birth weight infants. Early Human Development 85: 449–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Weisleder, Adriana, and Anna Fernald. 2013. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science 24: 2143–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Welsh, Stephanie N., and Erika Hoff. 2021. Language exposure outside the home becomes more English-dominant from 30 to 60 months for children from Spanish-speaking homes in the United States. The International Journal of Bilingualism 25: 483–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wermelinger, Stephanie, Anja Gampe, and Moritz M. Daum. 2017. Bilingual toddlers have advanced abilities to repair communication failure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 155: 84–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wheeler, Sarahn, Pamela Maxson, Tracy Truong, and Geeta Swamy. 2018. Psychosocial stress and preterm birth: The impact of parity and race. Maternal and Child Health Journal 22: 1430–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yow, Wei Quin, and Ellen M. Markman. 2011. Bilingualism and children’s use of paralinguistic cues to interpret emotion in speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14: 562–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yow, Wei Quin, and Ellen M. Markman. 2015. A bilingual advantage in how children integrate multiple cues to understand a speaker’s referential intent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18: 391–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yow, Wei Quin, and Ellen M. Markman. 2016. Children increase their sensitivity to a speaker’s nonlinguistic cues following a communicative breakdown. Child Development 87: 385–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zuccarini, Mariagrazia, Chiara Suttora, Arianna Bello, Arianna Aceti, Luigi Corvaglia, Maria Christina Caselli, Annalisa Guarini, and Alessandra Sansavini. 2020. A parent-implemented language intervention for late talkers: An exploratory study on low-risk preterm and full-term children. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17: 9123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Monolingual (N = 9) | Bilingual (N = 9) | |
---|---|---|
Sex | ||
Female | 4 (44.4%) | 4 (44.4%) |
Male | 5 (55.6%) | 5 (55.6%) |
Gestational age at birth | ||
Mean (SD) | 30.48 (3.40) | 30.91 (2.53) |
Median [Min, Max] | 31.71 [25.43, 34.14] | 31.14 [27.22, 34.57] |
APGAR score at 1 min | ||
Mean (SD) | 6.44 (1.51) | 5.00 (2.35) |
Median [Min, Max] | 7.00 [4.00, 8.00] | 5.00 [1.00, 8.00] |
APGAR score at 5 min | ||
Mean (SD) | 7.78 (1.20) | 7.44 (1.51) |
Median [Min, Max] | 8.00 [6.00, 9.00] | 7.00 [6.00, 10.00] |
Mother’s years of education | ||
Mean (SD) | 13.67 (2.45) | 13.11 (3.02) |
Median [Min, Max] | 12.00 [11.00, 18.00] | 12.00 [8.00, 18.00] |
Mother’s Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test Standard Score | ||
Mean (SD) | 95.22 (11.52) | 94.00 (13.41) |
Median [Min, Max] | 99.00 [76.00, 106.00] | 96.00 [76.00, 111.00] |
Bayley Mental Development Index at 16 m | ||
Mean (SD) | 89.11 (13.86) | 84.44 (10.85) |
Median [Min, Max] | 90.00 [64.00, 104.00] | 84.00 [64.00, 98.00] |
Stanford-Binet Abbreviated IQ at 36 m | ||
Mean (SD) | 93.67 (12.59) | 95.71 (17.65) |
Median [Min, Max] | 94.00 [76.00, 118.00] | 100.00 [61.00, 109.00] |
Monolingual (N = 9) | Bilingual (N = 9) | |
---|---|---|
Vocabulary at 16 m | ||
Mean (SD) | 32.33 (33.09) | 20.00 (20.37) |
Median [Min, Max] | 24.50 [2.00, 97.00] | 7.50 [3.00, 48.00] |
Vocabulary at 24 m | ||
Mean (SD) | 126.89 (112.40) | 88.22 (77.39) |
Median [Min, Max] | 56.00 [13.00, 298.00] | 51.00 [5.00, 242.00] |
Vocabulary at 36 m | ||
Mean (SD) | 285.56 (36.56) | 264.00 (36.11) |
Median [Min, Max] | 302.00 [206.00, 310.00] | 263.00 [219.00, 316.00] |
Child’s MLU at 16 m visit a | ||
Mean (SD) | 0.15 (0.20) | 0.09 (0.13) |
Median [Min, Max] | 0.08 [0.00, 0.57] | 0.03 [0.00, 0.39] |
Child’s MLU at 36 m visit a | ||
Mean (SD) | 2.22 (0.81) | 1.56 (0.61) |
Median [Min, Max] | 2.31 [1.01, 3.40] | 1.40 [0.99, 2.75] |
Mother’s NTW at 16 m visit b | ||
Mean (SD) | 1093.89 (198.18) | 733.56 (276.04) |
Median [Min, Max] | 1168.00 [760.00, 1308.00] | 692.00 [201.00, 1149.00] |
Mother’s NTW at 36 m visit b | ||
Mean (SD) | 1154.57 (327.90) | 826.67 (321.82) |
Median [Min, Max] | 986.00 [830.00, 1591.00] | 669.50 [527.00, 1294.00] |
Number of conversational turns at 16 m visit | ||
Mean (SD) | 45.33 (23.31) | 51.56 (28.43) |
Median [Min, Max] | 43.00 [14.00, 80.00] | 56.00 [19.00, 86.00] |
Child MLUw 16 m | Mother NTW 16 m | Conv. Turns 16 m | Child MLUw 36 m | Mother NTW 36 m | Vocab. 16 m | Vocab. 24 m | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mother NTW 16 m | −0.08 | ||||||
Conversational Turns 16 m | 0.39 | −0.12 | |||||
Child MLUw 36 m | 0.51 | 0.32 | 0.25 | ||||
Mother NTW 36 m | 0.12 | 0.79 ** | 0.03 | −0.01 | |||
Vocabulary 16 m | 0.64 ** | 0.14 | 0.09 | 0.49 | 0.61 | ||
Vocabulary 24 m | 0.62 * | 0.11 | 0.66 * | 0.71 ** | 0.15 | 0.42 | |
Vocabulary 36 m | 0.24 | 0.23 | 0.03 | 0.73 * | −0.13 | 0.34 | 0.39 |
Child MLUw 16 m | Mother NTW 16 m | Conv. Turns 16 m | Child MLUw 36 m | Mother NTW 36 m | Vocab. 16 m | Vocab. 24 m | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mother NTW 16 m | −0.20 | ||||||
Conversational Turns 16 m | 0.42 | −0.33 | |||||
Child MLUw 36 m | 0.25 | 0.07 | −0.21 | ||||
Mother NTW 36 m | 0.05 | 0.59 | −0.02 | −0.34 | |||
Vocabulary 16 m | 0.66 * | 0.66 | 0.83 | 0 | 0.6 | ||
Vocabulary 24 m | 0.63 | −0.17 | 0.57 | 0.61 | −0.14 | 0.54 | |
Vocabulary 36 m | 0.26 | 0.17 | −0.18 | 0.45 | −0.38 | 0.03 | 0.23 |
Child MLUw 16 m | Mother NTW 16 m | Conv. Turns 16 m | Child MLUw 36 m | Mother NTW 36 m | Vocab. 16 m | Vocab. 24 m | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mother NTW 16 m | −0.33 | ||||||
Conversational Turns 16 m | 0.29 | 0.38 | |||||
Child MLUw 36 m | 0.30 | 0.20 | 0.66 | ||||
Mother NTW 36 m | −0.54 | 0.83 | −0.14 | −0.26 | |||
Vocabulary 16 m | 0.47 | −0.37 | −0.29 | 0.15 | −0.05 | ||
Vocabulary 24 m | 0.46 | 0.29 | 0.79 ** | 0.75 ** | −0.2 | 0.13 | |
Vocabulary 36 m | −0.07 | −0.18 | 0.18 | 0.3 | −0.6 | 0.49 | 0.29 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Crespo, K.; Libersky, E.; Poehlmann, J.; Kaushanskaya, M. A Comparative Analysis of Language Skills and Parent–Child Interactions in Monolingual and Bilingual Children Born Preterm. Languages 2024, 9, 361. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9120361
Crespo K, Libersky E, Poehlmann J, Kaushanskaya M. A Comparative Analysis of Language Skills and Parent–Child Interactions in Monolingual and Bilingual Children Born Preterm. Languages. 2024; 9(12):361. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9120361
Chicago/Turabian StyleCrespo, Kimberly, Emma Libersky, Julie Poehlmann, and Margarita Kaushanskaya. 2024. "A Comparative Analysis of Language Skills and Parent–Child Interactions in Monolingual and Bilingual Children Born Preterm" Languages 9, no. 12: 361. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9120361
APA StyleCrespo, K., Libersky, E., Poehlmann, J., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2024). A Comparative Analysis of Language Skills and Parent–Child Interactions in Monolingual and Bilingual Children Born Preterm. Languages, 9(12), 361. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9120361