Analysis of Data-Based Scientific Reasoning from a Product-Based and a Process-Based Perspective
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Background
2.1. Data-Based Scientific Reasoning
2.2. Changes of Conceptual Development with Data in the Context of Population Dynamics
2.3. Aim and Research Questions
- How does anomalous data affect the change of initial predictions regarding the scientific phenomenon of population dynamics?
- How are changes of initial predictions about population dynamics related to a change in confidence towards the initial predictions?
- How are reactions regarding initial predictions about population dynamics related to presented proportions of anomalous to supportive data?
- How are reactions regarding initial predictions about population dynamics related to individual processes of data-based scientific reasoning?
3. Materials and Methods
3.1. Participants
3.2. Instrument
3.3. Analyses
4. Results
4.1. Prediction Group Changes
4.2. Reactions to Anomalous Data
4.3. Relation to the Proportion between Anomalous Data and Supportive Data
4.4. Role of Data-Based Reasoning Process
5. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Category | Subcategory | Code | Description |
---|---|---|---|
Type of graphed prediction | BoN Graph | The graph shows a trend that represents a stable population development. Stable is defined as linear horizontal or around a mean value fluctuating lines. The fluctuation is mostly uniform, and the amplitudes are low. | |
FoN Graph | The graph shows an unstable, chaotic trend. FoN graphs include increasing, decreasing, and chaotic or with high amplitudes fluctuating graphs. | ||
Conceptual knowledge | BoN conceptions | Stability | The general assumption of a stable development or that disturbances are not expected is stated. |
Human disturbances | Human caused disturbances are named as reasons for instability. | ||
Harmonic prey–predator relationship (PPR) | A harmonic regulation by prey–predator relationship is stated as a reason for stability. | ||
FoN conceptions | Instability | An unpredictable/instable development is described. | |
Natural causes | Natural causes (e.g., disturbances like epidemics, fires, and invasive species; climate changes; change of environmental resource; imi- and emigration) are described as reasons for an instable development. | ||
Inharmonic PPR | Predator caused changes that may also cause extinction are stated. | ||
Content knowledge | Population models | Biological models like capacity limit, logarithmic population development, or prey–predator models (Lotka–Volterra) are named. | |
Patch dynamics | Aspects of a heterogeneous ecosystem like naturally changing resources or imi- and emigration of populations are named. | ||
Disturbances | The chance and importance of disturbances for development in ecosystems are named. | ||
Biodiversity | Aspects of biodiversity (also genetics) are named. | ||
Environmental factors | Change of biotic and/or abiotic factors are named. | ||
Procedural knowledge | Statistics | The data are statistically treated (e.g., comparison of means/data points, calculating/estimating mean values). | |
CVS | Aspects of the importance to control variables are stated. | ||
Patterns | The identification of patterns in the data is stated. | ||
Diagram competence | Represent | The data sets represented as line graphs are described superficially without explaining the shown relation. | |
Syntactic | The data sets represented as line graphs are described by stating aspects of the shown relation, trend or single data points, no connection to the phenomenon/conceptual knowledge is given. Data sets are compared superficially. | ||
Semantic | The data sets represented as line graphs are described by stating aspects of the shown relation, trend, or single data points and a connection to the phenomenon/conceptual knowledge is given. Data sets are compared with relation to the phenomenon. | ||
Epistemic knowledge | Limits of models | Aspects of the limits or hypothetical nature of models are named. | |
Probability | Aspects of probability and significance are named. | ||
Credibility | Aspects of credibility or believability of the data are stated. | ||
Quality | Aspects of quality of the data are stated (e.g., reliability of measurement, replication, experimentation bias). | ||
Others | Uncertainty | Aspects of uncertainty (e.g., need for more information) are stated. | |
Test wiseness | Experiences from previous tasks are stated as reasons for any task performance. | ||
General prior knowledge/Intuition | General prior knowledge (e.g., memorizing from schoolbooks) or intuition are stated as reasons for any task performance. |
References
- Fischer, F.; Kollar, I.; Ufer, S.; Sodian, B.; Hussmann, H.; Pekrun, R.; Eberle, J. Scientific reasoning and argumentation: Advancing an interdisciplinary research agenda in education. Frontline Learn. Res. 2014, 2, 28–45. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman, C.; Croker, S. Learning science through inquiry. In Handbook of the Psychology of Science; Feist, G.J., Gorman, M.E., Eds.; Springer Publishing Company: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 49–70. [Google Scholar]
- Kind, P.; Osborne, J. Styles of scientific reasoning: A cultural rationale for science education? Sci. Educ. 2017, 101, 8–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lehrer, R.; Schauble, L. The development of scientific thinking. In Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science: Cognitive Processes; Liben, L.S., Müller, U., Lerner, R.M., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2015; pp. 671–714. [Google Scholar]
- Zimmerman, C. The development of scientific thinking skills in elementary and middle school. Dev. Rev. 2007, 27, 172–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- NGSS Lead States. Next Generation Science Standards: For States, by States; The National Academy Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Samarapungavan, A. Construing scientific evidence: The role of disciplinary knowledge in reasoning with and about evidence in scientific practice. In Scientific Reasoning and Argumentation: The Roles of Domain-Specific and Domain-General Knowledge; Fischer, F., Chinn, C.A., Engelmann, K., Osborne, J., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2018; pp. 66–86. [Google Scholar]
- Chinn, C.A.; Brewer, W.F. An empirical test of a taxonomy of responses to anomalous data in science. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1998, 35, 623–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ludwig, T.; Priemer, B.; Lewalter, D. Assessing secondary school students’ justifications for supporting or rejecting a scientific hypothesis in the physics lab. Res. Sci. Educ. 2019, 51, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Meister, S.; Krell, M.; Göhner, M.; Upmeier zu Belzen, A. Pre-service biology teachers’ responses to first-hand anomalous data during modelling processes. Res. Sci. Educ. 2020, 52, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Göhner, M.; Krell, M. Preservice science teachers’ strategies in scientific reasoning: The case of modeling. Res. Sci. Educ. 2020, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crujeiras-Pérez, B.; Jiménez-Aleixandre, M.P. Students’ progression in monitoring anomalous results obtained in inquiry-based laboratory tasks. Res. Sci. Educ. 2019, 49, 243–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hug, B.; McNeill, K.L. Use of first-hand and second-hand data in science: Does data type influence classroom conversations? Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2008, 30, 1725–1751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnstone, A.H.; Sleet, R.J.; Vianna, J.F. An information processing model of learning: Its application to an undergraduate laboratory course in chemistry. Stud. Higher Educ. 1994, 19, 77–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chinn, C.A.; Brewer, W.F. The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: A theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Rev. Educ. Res. 1993, 63, 1–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Limón, M. On the cognitive conflict as an instructional strategy for conceptual change: A critical appraisal. Learn. Instr. 2001, 11, 357–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mason, L. Responses to anomalous data on controversial topics and theory change. Learn. Instr. 2001, 11, 453–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lin, J.-Y. Responses to anomalous data obtained from repeatable experiments in the laboratory. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2007, 44, 506–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chinn, C.A.; Malhotra, B.A. Children’s responses to anomalous scientific data: How is conceptual change impeded? J. Educ. Psychol. 2002, 94, 327–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hemmerich, J.A.; Van Voorhis, K.; Wiley, J. Anomalous evidence, confidence change, and theory change. Cognit. Sci. 2016, 40, 1534–1560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jeong, H.; Songer, N.B.; Lee, S.-Y. Evidentiary Competence: Sixth Graders’ Understanding for Gathering and Interpreting Evidence in Scientific Investigations. Res. Sci. Educ. 2007, 37, 75–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kang, S.; Scharmann, L.C.; Noh, T. Reexamining the role of cognitive conflict in science concept learning. Res. Sci. Educ. 2004, 34, 71–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brewer, W.F. Perception is theory laden: The naturalized evidence and philosophical implications. J. Gen. Philos. Sci. 2015, 46, 121–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chinn, C.A.; Brewer, W.F. Models of data: A theory of how people evaluate data. Cognit. Instr. 2001, 19, 323–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roberts, R.; Johnson, P. Understanding the quality of data: A concept map for ‘the thinking behind the doing’ in scientific practice. Curriculum J. 2015, 26, 345–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Duncan, R.G.; Chinn, C.A.; Barzilai, S. Grasp of evidence: Problematizing and expanding the next generation science standards’ conceptualization of evidence. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2018, 55, 907–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chan, C.; Burtis, J.; Bereiter, C. Knowledge building as a mediator of conflict in conceptual change. Cognit. Instr. 1997, 15, 1–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- A Osborne, J.F.; Patterson, A. Scientific argument and explanation: A necessary distinction? Sci. Educ. 2011, 95, 627–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kane, J.E.; Webster, G.D. Heuristics and Biases That Help and Hinder Scientists: Toward a Psychology of Scientific Judgment and Decision Making. In Handbook of the Psychology of Science; Feist, G.J., Gorman, M., Gorman, M.E., Eds.; Springer Publishing Company: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 437–459. [Google Scholar]
- Sander, E.; Jelemenská, P.; Kattmann, U. Towards a better understanding of ecology. J. Biol. Educ. 2004, 40, 119–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ladle, R.J.; Gillson, L. The (im)balance of nature: A public perception time-lag? Publ. Underst. Sci. 2009, 18, 229–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cuddington, K. The “Balance of Nature” Metaphor and Equilibrium in Population Ecology: Biology and Philosophy. Biol. Philos. 2001, 16, 463–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Korfiatis, K.J.; Stamou, A.G.; Paraskevopoulos, S. Images of nature in Greek primary school textbooks. Sci. Educ. 2004, 88, 72–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zimmerman, C.; Cuddington, K. Ambiguous, circular and polysemous: Students’ definitions of the \”balance of nature\” metaphor. Publ. Underst. Sci. 2007, 16, 393–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Potvin, P. The Coexistence Claim and Its Possible Implications for Success in Teaching for Conceptual “Change”. Eur. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2017, 5, 55–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, R.L. Ecology and Field Biology, 5th ed.; HarperCollins: New York, NY, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Sandoval, J. Teaching in subject matter areas: Science. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1995, 46, 355–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roth, W.-M.; Bowen, G.M.; McGinn, M.K. Differences in graph-related practices between high school biology textbooks and scientific ecology journals. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1999, 36, 977–1019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masnick, A.M.; Klahr, D.; Knowles, E.R. Data-driven belief revision in children and adults. J. Cognit. Dev. 2017, 18, 87–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nitz, S.; Meister, S.; Schwanewedel, J.; Upmeier zu Belzen, A. Kompetenzraster zum Umgang mit Liniendiagrammen: Ein Beispiel für Diagnostik im Lehr-Lern-Labor. MNU J. 2018, 6, 393–400. [Google Scholar]
- Meister, S.; Zimmerman, C.; Upmeier zu Belzen, A. Visualizing pre-service biology teachers’ conceptions about population dynamics in ecosystems. Sci. Educ. Rev. Lett. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Upmeier zu Belzen, A.; Engelschalt, P.; Krüger, D. Modeling as Scientific Reasoning—The Role of Abductive Reasoning for Modeling Competence. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sale, J.E.; Lohfeld, L.H.; Brazil, K. Revisiting the quantitative-qualitative debate: Implications for mixed-methods research. Qual. Quant. 2002, 36, 43–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holmqvist, K.; Nyström, M.; Andersson, R.; Dewhurst, R.; Jarodzka, H.; Van de Weijer, J. Eye Tracking: A Comprehensive Guide to Methods and Measures; OUP Oxford: Oxford, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Hartmann, S.; Upmeier zu Belzen, A.; Krüger, D.; Pant, H. Scientific reasoning in higher education. Z. Psychol. 2015, 223, 47–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayring, P. Qualitative Content Analysis: Theoretical Foundation, Basic Procedures and Software Solution; Theoretical Foundation, Basic Procedures and Software Solution: Klagenfurt, Austria, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Shaffer, D.W.; Ruis, A.R. Epistemic network analysis: A worked example of theory-based learning analytics. In Handbook of learning analytics; Lang, C., Siemens, G., Wise, A.F., Gasevic, D., Eds.; Society for Learning Analytics Research: Edmonton, AL, USA, 2017; pp. 175–187. [Google Scholar]
- Roberson, D.B. Test-Wiseness and Background Knowledge: Their Relative Contributions to High Test Performance; Mississippi State University: Mississippi State, MS, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Ampatzidis, G.; Ergazaki, M. Challenging Students’ Belief in the ‘Balance of Nature’ Idea. Sci. Educ. 2018, 27, 895–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shtulman, A. Confidence without Competence in the Evaluation of Scientific Claims. In Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Portland, OR, USA, 11–14 August 2010; Volume 32. [Google Scholar]
- Masnick, A.M.; Morris, B.J. Investigating the development of data evaluation: The role of data characteristics. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 1032–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Berland, L.K.; Lee, V.R. Anomalous graph data and claim revision during argumentation. In Proceedings of the ICLS 2010 Conference Proceedings—9th International Conference of the Learning Sciences, Chicago, IL, USA, 29 June–2 July 2010; Volume 2, pp. 314–315. [Google Scholar]
- Fischer, F.; Chinn, C.A.; Engelmann, K.; Osborne, J. (Eds.) Scientific Reasoning and Argumentation: The Roles of Domain-Specific and Domain-General Knowledge; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Mahler, D.; Bock, D.; Bruckermann, T. Preservice Biology Teachers’ Scientific Reasoning Skills and Beliefs about Nature of Science: How Do They Develop and Is There a Mutual Relationship during the Development? Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beniermann, A.; Mecklenburg, L.; Upmeier zu Belzen, A. Reasoning on Controversial Science Issues in Science Education and Science Communication. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Sub-Process/Task | Task Content | Format of Data Assessment |
---|---|---|
Prediction | Making predictions about population development | Open-ended graphing task combined with open-ended writing task for explanation |
Rating the confidence in the made predictions | Rating scale: percentage scale from 0% (totally unconfident) to 100% (totally confident) | |
Data visual perception (perceptual) | Looking on the presented data sets without a further instruction. | Eye tracking experiment |
Data selection (perceptual) | Selecting data sets | Multiple-choice task |
Data appraisal (perceptual/interpretational) | Rating credibility, relevance, and fit of each data set | Rating scales from 1 (credible/relevant/fitting) to 5 (non-credible/irrelevant/not fitting) |
Data explanation (interpretational) | Explaining each data set | Open-ended writing task |
Data interpretation (interpretational) | Interpreting data sets regarding initial conceptions | Open-ended writing task |
Rating the confidence in the made predictions retrospectively | Rating scale: percentage scale from 0% (totally unconfident) to 100% (totally confident) |
Superior Prediction Groups | Explicit Prediction Groups | Mean Network Model | Description | N 1st | N 2nd | N 3rd |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BoN | Harmonic prey–predator relation (PPR) conception | Participants assigned to this group graphed BoN predictions and explained their predictions with their content knowledge about population models that they connected with conceptions about stability and harmonic prey–predator relationships. | 4 | 3 | 1 | |
Stability conception | Participants assigned to this group graphed BoN predictions and explained their predictions with a general stability conception. | 7 | 3 | 4 | ||
Content knowledge | Participants assigned to this group graphed BoN predictions and explained their predictions with biological content knowledge. They mentioned population models and environmental factors, without connecting these with stability conceptions. | 3 | 2 | 1 | ||
FoN/BoN | Mixed conceptions and content knowledge | Participants assigned to this group graphed BoN predictions and FoN predictions. They explained their predictions with biological content knowledge. They connected their knowledge with divergent conceptions addressing both FoN (natural causes, inharmonic PPR) and BoN (stability, harmonic PPR). | 3 | 4 | 2 | |
Divergent prey–predator relation conceptions | Participants assigned to this group graphed BoN predictions and FoN predictions. They explained their predictions with divergent conceptions about prey–predator relationships addressing both FoN and BoN. | 0 | 1 | 2 | ||
Mixed conceptions and human disturbance | Participants assigned to this group graphed FoN predictions. They explained their predictions with biological content knowledge and FoN related conceptions. They also mentioned human disturbance when explaining their predictions. | 1 | 3 | 1 | ||
FoN | FoN conceptions and content knowledge | Participants assigned to this group graphed FoN predictions. They explained their predictions with biological content knowledge, mostly mentioning population models. They connected their knowledge with FoN-related conceptions. | 2 | 0 | 3 |
To From | BoN | FoN/BoN | FoN |
---|---|---|---|
BoN | n1st-2nd = 8 (* = 1) n2nd-3rd = 6 (* = 1) | n1st-2nd = 6 (* = 3) n2nd-3rd = 1 | n1st-2nd = 0 n2nd-3rd = 2 |
FoN/BoN | n1st-2nd = 0 n2nd-3rd = 3 (* = 2) | n1st-2nd = 4 n2nd-3rd = 7 (* = 3) | n1st-2nd = 0 n2nd-3rd = 1 |
FoN | n1st-2nd = 1 n2nd-3rd = 0 | n1st-2nd = 1 n2nd-3rd = 0 | n1st-2nd = 0 n2nd-3rd = 0 |
Confidence Change Options | N (1st Scenario) | N (2nd Scenario) | N (3rd Scenario) |
---|---|---|---|
Steady confidence | 4 | 8 | 9 |
Steady unconfidence | 4 | 5 | 4 |
Confidence in abeyance | 4 | 4 (+1) | 5 |
Increase to confidence | 5 (+1) 1 | 1 | 1 |
Decrease to unconfidence | 3 | 2 | 1 (+1) |
Reactions | N |
---|---|
Confident confirmation | 14 (* = 3; 35%) |
Undecided confirmation | 4 (10%) |
Unconfident confirmation | 7 (* = 2; 17.5%) |
Confident modification | 4 (* = 1; 10%) |
Undecided modification | 4 (* = 1; 10%) |
Unconfident modification | 7 (* = 3; 17.5%) |
Reactions | Anomalous Data Ratio (BoN:FoN) | ||
---|---|---|---|
2:4 | 3:3 | 4:2 | |
Confident confirmation | n = 5 (* = 1) | n = 6 (* = 2) | n = 3 |
Undecided confirmation | n = 2 | n = 0 | n = 2 |
Unconfident confirmation | n = 2 | n = 1 | n = 4 (* = 2) |
Confirmation (N = 25) | n = 9 (* = 1; 36%) | n = 7 (* = 2; 28%) | n = 9 (* = 2; 36%) |
Confident modification | n = 2 (* = 1) | n = 1 | n = 1 |
Undecided modification | n = 0 | n = 3 (* = 1) | n = 1 |
Unconfident modification | n = 3 (* = 2) | n = 2 (* = 1) | n = 2 |
Modification (N = 15) | n = 5 (* = 3; 33.3%) | n = 6 (* = 2; 40%) | n = 4 (26.7%) |
New Conceptual Knowledge | Initial Conceptual Knowledge | No Explanation | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Only | Plus Procedural and/or Epistemic Knowledge | Only | Plus Procedural and/or Epistemic Knowledge | ||
Skeptical general | Finn_1st Sam_2nd | ||||
Skeptical credibility | Alex_1st Andrea_2nd Andy_2nd | ||||
Skeptical relevance | Jamie_1st Quinn_2nd | ||||
Undecided | Andrea_1st Bente_2nd * Jona_2nd Kay_2nd Noah_1st Noah_2nd | Bente_1st * Chris_1st Chris_2nd Finn_2nd Kim_1st Luca_1st Luca_2nd Quinn_1st Sam_1st | Nicola_2nd | Nicola_1st | |
Not skeptical | Charlie_2nd Jona_1st Kim_2nd * Mika_2nd Toni_1st * | Alex_2nd * Andy_1st Charlie_1st * Jamie_2nd * Kay_1st Mika_1st Robin_1st * Robin_2nd * Sascha_1st Sascha_2nd | Toni_2nd * |
Prediction Group 1st Scenario | Data Interpretation (Extract) | Prediction Group 2nd Scenario |
---|---|---|
Mixed conceptions and content knowledge | “During this time, factors exist that influenced the population density in a negative way (e.g., predators, disasters).” “Similar to prediction, only time period for regeneration of the population density was not correct.” “Confidence highly increased due to the similarities to the data.” | Mixed conceptions and content knowledge |
Prediction Group 2nd Scenario | Data Interpretation (Extract) | Prediction Group 3rd Scenario |
---|---|---|
Stability conception | “Massive changes of environmental circumstances led to the extinction or extreme population fluctuations.” “In 2/3 of the areas, my prediction was the case.” “Without further information about environmental factors, my confidence regarding my prediction will not increase.” | Stability conception |
Prediction Group 1st Scenario | Data Interpretation (Extract) | Prediction Group 2nd Scenario |
---|---|---|
Stability conception | “4 of 6 data sets are supporting my prediction, because of a stable prey-predator relationship.” “2 of 6 data sets show massive fluctuations. Imbalance of prey-predator relationship could also be influenced by other factors.” “Unconfidence due to wrong assumptions and the fact, that population growth cannot be explained only by considering prey-predator relationships.” | Divergent prey-predator-relation conceptions |
Prediction Group 1st Scenario | Data Interpretation (Extract) | Prediction Group 2nd Scenario |
---|---|---|
FoN conceptions and content knowledge | “My prediction did not include extreme events like diseases or influences of weather, but only the development based on prey-predator-relationships.” “My confidence did not change, because some data represent extreme events that were not included into my prediction.” | Harmonic prey–predator relation conception |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Meister, S.; Upmeier zu Belzen, A. Analysis of Data-Based Scientific Reasoning from a Product-Based and a Process-Based Perspective. Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 639. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11100639
Meister S, Upmeier zu Belzen A. Analysis of Data-Based Scientific Reasoning from a Product-Based and a Process-Based Perspective. Education Sciences. 2021; 11(10):639. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11100639
Chicago/Turabian StyleMeister, Sabine, and Annette Upmeier zu Belzen. 2021. "Analysis of Data-Based Scientific Reasoning from a Product-Based and a Process-Based Perspective" Education Sciences 11, no. 10: 639. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11100639
APA StyleMeister, S., & Upmeier zu Belzen, A. (2021). Analysis of Data-Based Scientific Reasoning from a Product-Based and a Process-Based Perspective. Education Sciences, 11(10), 639. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11100639