How Do Five- to Six-Year-Old Children Interpret a Burning Candle?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Precursor Models and Explanations in Young Children
1.2. Previous Research on the Concept of Matter and Its Transformations
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Data Collection
2.3. Data Analysis
3. Results
Alberto: ‘It will melt’.
Researcher: ‘Why?’
Alberto: ‘Because what is inside melts.’
Researcher: ‘Alicia, what do you think is going to happen?’
Alicia: ‘The candle is going to turn orange, yellow and red, because then it will come out like a heart.’
Researcher: ‘We have a candle, we are going to light it, and then cover it with a glass […]. You have to think about what is going to happen.’
Beatriz: ‘There is condensation on the glass. Because when the droplets are so hot due to the fire, they may also condense […]. A candle… a glass that covers it… and this is what is tarnished [talking as she explains her drawing].’
Blanca: ‘Perhaps the candle that is lit will go out because it (the glass) is very cold.’
Researcher: ‘Why will the candle go out?’
Borja: ‘Because the cold can extinguish anything.’
Blas: ‘The candle will burn the glass because the glass is made of iron and the candle will burn it […]. In a cartoon that I saw, the lava from a mountain fell on some cars, and they put iron and the lava destroyed it.’
Researcher: ‘But the vessel that we are going to cover the candle with is made of glass.’
Blas: ‘But it can melt too.’
Researcher: ‘Now can you explain your drawings?’
Alicia: ‘There was a cloud inside the glass and it was very windy, and the candle went out.’
Amanda: ‘Because it [the glass] has no outlet and there was wind inside, it extinguished the candle.’
Researcher: ‘And why did it go out?’
Blas: ‘Because there was air inside.’
Researcher: ‘And is there no air in here [in the classroom]?’
Bruno: ‘Yes, but here [in the classroom] you do not really know where it is because the air moves much more.’
Researcher: ‘Why does it not go out when I do not cover it? You can see that candles stay lit for a long time when they are not covered.
Belinda: ‘Because there was no air coming in.’
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Spektor-Levy, O.; Baruch, Y.K.; Mevarech, Z. Science and scientific curiosity in pre-school—The teacher’ s point of view. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2013, 35, 2226–2253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- French, L. Science as the center of a coherent, integrated early childhood curriculum. Earl. Child. Res. Quar. 2004, 19, 138–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eshach, H.; Fried, M.N. Should science be taught in Early Childhood? J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 2005, 14, 315–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhn, D.; Pease, M. Do Children and Adults Learn Differently? J. Cong. Devel. 2006, 7, 279–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koliopoulos, D.; Christidou, V.; Symidala, I.; Koutsiouba, M. Pre-energy reasoning in preschool children. Rev. Sci. Math. ICT Educ. 2009, 3, 123–140. [Google Scholar]
- Feu, M.T. Experimentar con materiales en el 0-6 [Experimenting with materials in 0-6]. Rev. Aula Infant. 2009, 52, 7–10. [Google Scholar]
- Areljung, S.; Ottander, C.; Due, K. “Drawing the Leaves Anyway”: Teachers Embracing Children’s Different Ways of Knowing in Preschool Science Practice. Res. Sci. Educ. 2017, 47, 1173–1192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hadenfeldt, J.C.; Liu, X.; Neumann, K. Framing students’ progression in understanding matter: A review of previous research. Stud. Sci. Educ. 2014, 50, 181–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Concari, S.B. Las teorías y modelos en la explicación científica: Implicaciones para la enseñanza de las ciencias [Theories and models in scientific explanation: Implications for teaching science]. Ciên. Educ. 2001, 7, 85–94. [Google Scholar]
- Jones, N.A.; Ross, H.; Lynam, T.; Perez, P.; Leitch, A. Mental models: An interdisciplinary synthesis of theory and methods. Ecol. Soc. 2011, 16, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Craik, K.J.W. The Nature of Explanation, 1st ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1943. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson-Laird, P.N. Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference and Consciousness, 6th ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert, J. Models and modelling: Routes to more authentic science education. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2004, 2, 115–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, M.; Lin, J. Investigating the relationship between students’ views of scientific models and their development of models. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2015, 37, 2453–2475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reinfried, S.; Tempelmann, S. The impact of secondary school students’ preconceptions on the evolution of their mental models of the greenhouse effect and global warming. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2014, 36, 304–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greca, I.M.; Moreira, M.A. Mental models, conceptual models, and modelling. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2000, 22, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Canedo-Ibarra, S.P.; Castelló-Escandell, J.; García-Wehrle, P.; Gómez-Galindo, A.; Morales-Blake, A.R. Cambio conceptual y construcción de modelos científicos precursores en educación infantil [Conceptual change and construction of precursory scientific models in early childhood education]. Rev. Mex. Inv. Educ. 2012, 17, 691–727. [Google Scholar]
- Ravanis, K.; Papandreou, M.; Kampeza, M.; Vellopoulou, A. Teaching activities for the construction of a precursor model in 5- to 6-year-old children’s thinking: The case of thermal expansion and contraction of metals. Eur. Earl. Child. Educ. Res. J. 2013, 21, 514–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koliopoulos, D.; Tantaros, S.; Papandreou, M.; Ravanis, K. Preschool children’s ideas about floating: A qualitative approach. J. Sci. Educ. 2004, 5, 21–24. [Google Scholar]
- Weil-Barais, A. Constructivist approaches and the teaching of science. Prospects 2001, 31, 187–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rodrigues, R.F.; Pereira, A.P.D. Explicações no ensino de ciências: Revisando o conceito a partir de três distinções básicas [Explanations in science education: Reviewing the concept from three basic distinctions]. Ciên. Educ. 2018, 24, 43–56. [Google Scholar]
- Oliveira, D.K.B.; Justi, R.; Mendonça, P.C.C. The use of representations and argumentative and explanatory situations. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2015, 37, 1402–1435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilbert, J.K.; Boulter, C.; Rutherford, M. Models in explanations, Part 1: Horses for courses? Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1998, 20, 83–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Braaten, M.; Windschitl, M. Working toward a stronger conceptualization of scientific explanation for science education. Sci. Educ. 2011, 95, 639–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Legare, C.H. The contributions of explanation and exploration to children’s scientific reasoning. Child Dev. Pers. 2014, 8, 101–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piaget, J. The Child’s Conception of the World; Routledge: London, UK, 1929. [Google Scholar]
- Christidou, V.; Hatzinikita, V. Preschool children’s explanations of plant growth and rain formation: A comparative analysis. Res. Sci. Educ. 2006, 36, 187–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peterson, S.M.; French, L. Supporting young children’s explanations through inquiry science in preschool. Earl. Child. Res. Quar. 2008, 23, 395–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hickling, A.K.; Wellman, H.M. The emergence of children’s causal explanations and theories: Evidence from everyday conversation. Dev. Psych. 2001, 37, 668–683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christidou, V. Accounting for natural phenomena. Int. J. Learn. 2005, 12, 21–28. [Google Scholar]
- Berzonsky, M.D. The role of familiarity in children’s explanations of physical causality. Child Dev. 1971, 42, 705–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saçkes, M.; Flevares, L.M.; Trundle, K.C. Four- to six-year-old children’s conceptions of the mechanism of rainfall. Earl. Child. Res. Quar. 2010, 25, 536–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cruz-Guzmán, M.; García-Carmona, A.; Criado, A.M. Aprendiendo sobre los cambios de estado en educación infantil mediante secuencias de pregunta-predicción-comprobación experimental [Sequences of question-prediction-teaching to learn about the states of matter in preschool education]. Enseñ. Cienc. 2017, 35, 175–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bar, V.; Galili, I. Stages of children’s views about evaporation. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1994, 16, 157–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tytler, R.; Peterson, S. Deconstructing learning in science: Young children’s responses to a classroom sequence on evaporation. Res. Sci. Educ. 2000, 30, 339–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kambouri, M.; Michaelides, A. Using drama techniques for the teaching of early years science: A case study. J. Emerg. Sci. 2014, 7, 7–14. [Google Scholar]
- Malleus, E.; Kikas, E.; Marken, T. Kindergarten and primary school children’s everyday, synthetic, and scientific concepts of clouds and rainfall. Res. Sci. Educ. 2017, 47, 539–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Özmen, H. Effect of animation enhanced conceptual change texts on 6th grade students’ understanding of the particulate nature of matter and transformation during phase changes. Comp. Educ. 2011, 57, 1114–1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hesse, J.J.; Anderson, C.W. Students’ conceptions of chemical change. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1992, 29, 277–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ahtee, M.; Varjola, I. Students’ understanding of chemical reaction. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1998, 20, 305–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prain, V.; Tytler, R.; Peterson, S. Multiple representation in learning about evaporation. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2009, 31, 787–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eilks, I.; Moellering, J. Seventh-grade students’ understanding of chemical reactions: Reflections from an action research interview study. Eura. J. Math. Sci. Technol. Educ. 2007, 3, 271–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meheut, M.; Saltiel, E.; Tiberghien, A. Pupils’ conceptions (11–12 years old) of combustion. Eur. J. Sci. Educ. 1985, 7, 83–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- BouJaoude, S.B. A study of the nature of students’ understandings about the concepto of burning. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1991, 28, 689–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prieto, T.; Watson, R.; & Dillon, J. Pupils’ understanding of combustion. Res. Sci. Educ. 1992, 22, 331–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kypraios, N.; Papageorgiou, G.; Stamovlasis, D. The role of some individual differences in understanding chemical changes: A study in Secondary Education. Int. J. Environ. Sci. Educ. 2014, 9, 413–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adadan, E.; Irving, K.E.; Trundle, K.C. Impacts of multi-representational instruction on high school students’ conceptual understandings of the particulate nature of matter. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2009, 31, 1743–1775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patton, M.Q. Qualitative Research and Evaluation, 3rd ed.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Savery, J.R. Overview of problem-based learning: Definitions and distinctions. In Essential Readings in Problem-Based Learning, 1st ed.; Walker, A., Leary, H., Hmelo-Silver, C.E., Ertmer, P.A., Eds.; Purdue University Press: West Lafayette, IN, USA, 2015; Section 1; pp. 5–15. [Google Scholar]
- García-Carmona, A.; Criado, A.M.; Cañal, P. Alfabetización científica en la etapa 3–6 años: Un análisis de la regulación estatal de enseñanzas mínimas [Scientific literacy at the 3–6 year-old stage: An analysis of Spain’s national curriculum]. Enseñ. Cienc. 2014, 32, 131–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Löfgren, L.; Helldén, G. A longitudinal study showing how students use a molecule concept when explaining everyday situations. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2009, 31, 1631–1655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- White, R.T.; Gunstone, R.F. Probing Understanding; The Falmer Press: London, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Yan, F.; Talanquer, V. Students’ ideas about how and why chemical reactions happen: Mapping the conceptual landscape. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2015, 37, 3066–3092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martins, I.P.; Veiga, L. Early Science Education: Exploring familiar contexts to improve the understanding of some basic scientific concepts. Eur. Earl. Child. Educ. Res. J. 2001, 9, 69–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klaar, S.; Öhman, J. Action with friction: A transactional approach to toddlers’ physical meaning making of natural phenomena and processes in preschool. Eur. Earl. Child. Educ. Res. J. 2012, 20, 439–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Types of Explanations | Examples | |
---|---|---|
4. Scientific | Rainwater evaporates and becomes a cloud when it condenses [32]. | |
3. Synthetic | Clouds are made of snow and bring water. Fallen rainwater mixes with seawater and the snow falls and returns to clouds [32]. | |
2. Naturalistic | Agentive | Why can’t you see the sugar now? It went down into the water and when we stirred it, it broke [30]. |
Non-agentive | Why did the marble go down? Because it is heavy. […] It is made of iron [30]. | |
1. Non-naturalistic | Teleological | How come it rains? It rains because the plants need to be watered [30]. |
Intentional | Why does it float [the cork]? Because it is very careful. […] It keeps its eyes open [30]. | |
Metaphysical | Where does the rain come from? It comes from God. God pours water from the sky [30].Why does the paper clip stick onto the magnet? Because it’s [the magnet] got glue on it [30]. |
Types of Explanation | Prediction Phase | Conclusion Phase | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Group A (N = 8) | Students | f | Students | f | |
Scientific | - | 0/8 | - | 0/9 | |
Synthetic | - | 0/8 | - | 0/9 | |
Naturalistic | Agentive | - | 0/8 | Alberto, Alexandre, Andrea, Amanda, Aurora, Alicia | 6/9 |
Non-agentive | Alberto, Adrián, Alexandre, Andrea, Amanda, Aitana, Aurora | 7/8 | Adrián, Aitana, Aurora | 3/9 | |
Non-naturalistic | Teleological | - | 0/8 | - | 0/9 |
Intentional | Alicia | 1/8 | - | 0/9 | |
Metaphysical | - | 0/8 | - | 0/9 | |
Group B (N = 14) | Students | f | Students | f | |
Scientific | - | 0/14 | Belinda, Benjamin | 2/15 | |
Synthetic | Beatriz, Blanca, Belinda, Berta, Brenda, Borja, Balbino, Baltasar, Benedicto, Boris | 10/14 | - | 0/15 | |
Naturalistic | Agentive | Blas, Bernardo, Bruno, Benjamin | 4/14 | Beatriz, Blanca, Berta, Brenda, Borja, Balbino, Baltasar, Benedicto, Boris, Blas, Bernardo, Bruno, Benjamin | 13/15 |
Non-agentive | - | 0/14 | - | 0/15 | |
Non-naturalistic | Teleological | - | 0/14 | - | 0/15 |
Intentional | - | 0/14 | - | 0/15 | |
Metaphysical | - | 0/14 | - | 0/15 |
Categories | Prediction Phase | Conclusion Phase | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Students | f | Students | f | |
A condensation process takes place on the walls of the glass. | Beatriz, Berta, Brenda, Baltasar, Benedicto, Boris, Belinda | 7/22 | - | 0/24 |
The candle explodes. | Amanda | 1/22 | - | 0/24 |
The candle melts. | Alberto, Adrián, Alexandre, Andrea | 4/22 | Alberto, Alexandre | 2/24 |
The candle changes color, and a heart comes out. | Alicia | 1/22 | - | 0/24 |
The candle emits light. | Aitana, Aurora | 2/22 | - | 0/24 |
The glass burns or melts. | Blas, Bernardo, Bruno, Benjamin | 4/22 | - | 0/24 |
Stain appears on the glass. | - | 0/22 | Adrián, Aitana, Aurora | 3/24 |
The candle goes out because of the cold temperature. | Blanca, Borja, Balbino | 3/22 | Blanca, Borja, Balbino, Berta, Brenda | 5/24 |
The candle goes out because there is air/wind inside the vessel. | - | 0/22 | Alicia, Amanda, Aurora, Andrea, Blas, Bernardo, Bruno, Benjamin, Benedicto, Boris | 10/24 |
The candle goes out because of the lack of oxygen/air. | - | 0/22 | Benjamin, Belinda | 2/24 |
The candle goes out because of moisture. | - | 0/22 | Beatriz, Baltasar | 2/24 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sesto, V.; García-Rodeja, I. How Do Five- to Six-Year-Old Children Interpret a Burning Candle? Educ. Sci. 2021, 11, 213. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050213
Sesto V, García-Rodeja I. How Do Five- to Six-Year-Old Children Interpret a Burning Candle? Education Sciences. 2021; 11(5):213. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050213
Chicago/Turabian StyleSesto, Vanessa, and Isabel García-Rodeja. 2021. "How Do Five- to Six-Year-Old Children Interpret a Burning Candle?" Education Sciences 11, no. 5: 213. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050213
APA StyleSesto, V., & García-Rodeja, I. (2021). How Do Five- to Six-Year-Old Children Interpret a Burning Candle? Education Sciences, 11(5), 213. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci11050213