Experiments on the Efficacy of Drawing for Memorization among Adults and Children with Varying Written Word Memory Capacities: A Two-Way Crossover Design
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Memory of Mental Images
1.2. Picture Superiority Effect
1.3. Drawing for Memory
1.4. Reasons for the Effectiveness of Drawing for Memorization
1.5. Application of Drawing for Memory in Education and Therapy
1.6. Memory Drawing for Specific Subgroups
1.7. Drawing for Memory within Various Conditions
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Research Background
2.2. Research Design and Ethics
2.3. Procedure and Participants
2.3.1. Experiment 1
2.3.2. Experiments 2–4
2.3.3. Experiment 2
2.3.4. Experiment 3
2.3.5. Experiment 4
2.4. Validity
2.5. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Number of Words and Drawings Recalled
3.2. Recalled Written Words and Drawings for Specific Subgroups
4. Discussion
4.1. Who Benefits to the Greatest Extent from Memory Drawing?
4.2. Drawing for Memory across Various Conditions
4.3. Implications for Policy and Practice
4.4. Study Limitations and Future Research
5. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Fernandes, M.A.; Wammes, J.D.; Meade, M.E. The Surprisingly Powerful Influence of Drawing on Memory. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 27, 302–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wammes, J.D.; Meade, M.E.; Fernandes, M.A. Creating a Recollection-Based Memory through Drawing. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2018, 44, 734–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wammes, J.D.; Meade, M.E.; Fernandes, M.A. The Drawing Effect: Evidence for Reliable and Robust Memory Benefits in Free Recall. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2016, 69, 1752–1776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meade, M.E.; Wammes, J.D.; Fernandes, M.A. Drawing as an Encoding Tool: Memorial Benefits in Younger and Older Adults. Exp. Aging Res. 2018, 44, 369–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meade, M.E.; Ahmad, M.; Fernandes, M.A. Drawing Pictures at Encoding Enhances Memory in Healthy Older Adults and in Individuals with Probable Dementia. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2020, 27, 880–901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Meade, M.E.; Wammes, J.D.; Fernandes, M.A. Comparing the Influence of Doodling, Drawing, and Writing at Encoding on Memory. Can. J. Exp. Psychol./Rev. Can. Psychol. Exp. 2019, 73, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ottarsdottir, U. Processing Emotions and Memorising Coursework through Memory Drawing. Art Ther. Online 2018, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ottarsdottir, U. Ethical Concerns When Applying Drawing to Promote Memory: Research Conducted in Iceland. In Exploring Ethical Dilemmas in Art Therapy: 50 Clinicians from 20 Countries Share Their Stories; Routledge: London, UK; Taylor & Francis: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2019; pp. 266–272. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, B.R.T.; Wammes, J.D. Drawing and Memory: Using Visual Production to Alleviate Concreteness Effects. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2021, 28, 259–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wammes, J.D.; Jonker, T.R.; Fernandes, M.A. Drawing Improves Memory: The Importance of Multimodal Encoding Context. Cognition 2019, 191, 103955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wammes, J.D.; Roberts, B.R.T.; Fernandes, M.A. Task Preparation as a Mnemonic: The Benefits of Drawing (and Not Drawing). Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 2365–2372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wammes, J.D.; Meade, M.E.; Fernandes, M.A. Learning Terms and Definitions: Drawing and the Role of Elaborative Encoding. Acta Psychol. 2017, 179, 104–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jalava, S.T.; Wammes, J.D.; Cheng, K. Drawing Your Way to an A: Long-Lasting Improvements in Classroom Quiz Performance Following Drawing. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2023, 30, 1939–1945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tran, S.H.N.; Fernandes, M.A. Age Differences in Effectiveness of Encoding Techniques on Memory. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2023, 31, 479–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tran, S.H.N.; Fernandes, M.A. Drawing Enhances Memory for Emotional Words. Can. J. Exp. Psychol./Rev. Can. Psychol. Exp. 2023, 77, 296–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bower, G. Analysis of a Mnemonic Device: Modern Psychology Uncovers the Powerful Components of Ancient System for Improving Memory. Am. Sci. 1970, 58, 496–510. [Google Scholar]
- Lutz, J. Introduction to Learning and Memory; Brooks/Cole Publishing Company: Pacific Grove, CA, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Paivio, A. Intelligence, Dual Coding Theory, and the Brain. Intelligence 2014, 47, 141–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, D.; Vu, K.-P.L. Effectiveness of Image-Based Mnemonic Techniques for Enhancing the Memorability and Security of User-Generated Passwords. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2010, 26, 705–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalgleish, T.; Navrady, L.; Bird, E.; Hill, E.; Dunn, B.D.; Golden, A.-M. Method-of-Loci as a Mnemonic Device to Facilitate Access to Self-Affirming Personal Memories for Individuals with Depression. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 1, 156–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shepard, R. Recognition Memory for Words, Sentences, and Pictures. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1967, 6, 156–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boldini, A.; Russo, R.; Punia, S.; Avons, S.E. Reversing the Picture Superiority Effect: A Speed—Accuracy Trade-off Study of Recognition Memory. Mem. Cogn. 2007, 35, 113–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Groninger, L.D.; Groninger, D.H.; Stiens, J. Learning the Names of People: The Role of Image Mediators. Memory 1995, 3, 147–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hockley, W.E. The Picture Superiority Effect in Associative Recognition. Mem. Cogn. 2008, 36, 1351–1359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kinjo, H.; Snodgrass, J.G. Is There a Picture Superiority Effect in Perceptual Implicit Tasks? Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 12, 145–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richardson, J.T.E. Mental Imagery and Human Memory; St. Martin’s Press: New York, NY, USA, 1980; ISBN 978-0-312-52975-8. [Google Scholar]
- Stenberg, G.; Radeborg, K.; Hedman, L.R. The Picture Superiority Effect in a Cross-Modality Recognition Task. Mem. Cogn. 1995, 23, 425–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vaidya, C.J.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Picture Superiority in Conceptual Memory: Dissociative Effects of Encoding and Retrieval Tasks. Mem. Cogn. 2000, 28, 1165–1172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waddill, P.J.; McDaniel, M.A. Pictorial Enhancement of Text Memory: Limitations Imposed by Picture Type and Comprehension Skill. Mem. Cogn. 1992, 20, 472–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Weldon, M.S.; Roediger, H.L.; Challis, B.H. The Properties of Retrieval Cues Constrain the Picture Superiority Effect. Mem. Cogn. 1989, 17, 95–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paivio, A.; Csapo, K. Picture Superiority in Free Recall: Imagery or Dual Coding? Cogn. Psychol. 1973, 5, 176–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edwards, D. Art Therapy; Creative Therapies in Practice; Sage Publications: London, UK, 2004; ISBN 978-0-7619-4750-9. [Google Scholar]
- Andrade, J. What Does Doodling Do? Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2010, 24, 100–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Meter, P.; Garner, J. The Promise and Practice of Learner-Generated Drawing: Literature Review and Synthesis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2005, 17, 285–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jonker, T.R.; Wammes, J.D.; MacLeod, C.M. Drawing Enhances Item Information but Undermines Sequence Information in Memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2019, 45, 689–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Exp. 1 Test 1 | Exp. 1 Test 2 | Exp. 1 Test 3a | Exp. 1 Test 3b | Exp. 2 | Exp. 3 | Exp. 4 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Number of participants (N) | 134 | 114 | 100 | 19 | 148 | 47 | 67 |
Age | 9–14 years | 9–14 years | 9–14 years | 9–14 years | adults | adults | adults |
Number of words encoded | 2 × 15 | 2 × 15 | 2 × 15 | 2 × 15 | 2 × 7 | 2 × 5 | 2 × 7 |
Average number of drawings recalled (SD) | 12.0 (2.84) | 5.6 (2.69) | 5.3 (2.85) | 5.4 (3.11) | 6.1 (1.04) | 4.3 (0.93) | 6.5 (0.84) |
Average number of written words recalled (SD) | 12.3 (3.09) | 2.6 (2.53) | 2.4 (2.29) | 1.1 (1.41) | 5.4 (1.83) | 3.5 (1.38) | 5.9 (1.65) |
Median number of drawings recalled | 13 | 5 | 5 | 5 | 6 | 5 | 7 |
Median number of written words recalled | 13 | 2 | 2 | 1 | 6 | 4 | 6 |
Percentage of drawn words recalled out of number of words encoded (%) | 80 | 37 | 35 | 36 | 88 | 86 | 92 |
Percentage of written words recalled out of number of words encoded (%) | 82 | 17 | 16 | 7 | 77 | 70 | 84 |
Average time to encode each word | 40 s | 40 s | 40 s | 40 s | 26 s | 12 s | 26 s |
Time from encoding to recall | immediately after | 3 weeks | 9 weeks | 9 weeks | 45 min | 25 min | 45 min |
Recalling experience in- between | no | no | after 3 weeks | no | no | no | no |
Setting | classroom | classroom | classroom | classroom | lectures | lectures | webinars |
Group 1 Greatest (Md − Mw) | Group 2 Moderate (Md − Mw) | Group 3 Fewest (Md − Mw) | F | p | df | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Exp. 1 Test 1 | −1.86 | −0.84 | 1.76 | 21.97 | <0.001 | (2, 131) |
Exp. 1 Test 2 | 1.08 | 3.53 | 4.37 | 16.54 | <0.001 | (2, 111) |
Exp. 1 Test 3a | 1.50 | 2.55 | 4.54 | 11.38 | <0.001 | (2, 97) |
Exp. 1 Test 3b | 3.92 | 2.33 | 6.43 | 4.05 | 0.038 | (2, 16) |
Experiment 2 | −0.35 | 0.18 | 2.39 | 75.61 | <0.001 | (2, 145) |
Experiment 3 | −0.25 | 0.88 | 1.93 | 16.36 | <0.001 | (2, 44) |
Experiment 4 | −0.26 | −0.05 | 2.05 | 20.91 | <0.001 | (2, 64) |
Group 4 Greatest (Md − Mw) | Group 5 Moderate (Md − Mw) | Group 6 Fewest (Md − Mw) | F | p | df | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Exp. 1 Test 1 | 0.91 | 0.22 | −2.02 | 13.38 | <0.001 | (2, 131) |
Exp. 1 Test 2 | 5.16 | 2.47 | 1.34 | 24.02 | <0.001 | (2, 111) |
Exp. 1 Test 3a | 5.03 | 3.08 | 0.47 | 34.09 | <0.001 | (2, 97) |
Exp. 1 Test 3b | 6.93 | 3.83 | 1.83 | 8.63 | 0.003 | (2, 16) |
Experiment 2 | 0.84 | 0.96 | 0.41 | 1.05 | 0.225 | (2, 145) |
Experiment 3 | 1.06 | 1.06 | 0.33 | 1.48 | 0.241 | (2, 44) |
Experiment 4 | 0.91 | 0.68 | 0.09 | 1.47 | 0.237 | (2, 64) |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Óttarsdóttir, U.G. Experiments on the Efficacy of Drawing for Memorization among Adults and Children with Varying Written Word Memory Capacities: A Two-Way Crossover Design. Educ. Sci. 2024, 14, 470. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14050470
Óttarsdóttir UG. Experiments on the Efficacy of Drawing for Memorization among Adults and Children with Varying Written Word Memory Capacities: A Two-Way Crossover Design. Education Sciences. 2024; 14(5):470. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14050470
Chicago/Turabian StyleÓttarsdóttir, Unnur Guðrún. 2024. "Experiments on the Efficacy of Drawing for Memorization among Adults and Children with Varying Written Word Memory Capacities: A Two-Way Crossover Design" Education Sciences 14, no. 5: 470. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14050470
APA StyleÓttarsdóttir, U. G. (2024). Experiments on the Efficacy of Drawing for Memorization among Adults and Children with Varying Written Word Memory Capacities: A Two-Way Crossover Design. Education Sciences, 14(5), 470. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14050470