Next Article in Journal
Childhood Arthritis and Rheumatology Research for Dreams Come True Remission
Next Article in Special Issue
Early Childhood Temperamental Trajectories following Very Preterm Birth and Their Association with Parenting Style
Previous Article in Journal
“It Is a Whole Different Life from the Life I Used to Live”: Assessing Parents’ Support Needs in Paediatric Palliative Care
Previous Article in Special Issue
Increased Risk of Parental Instability for Children Born Very Preterm and Impacts on Neurodevelopmental Outcomes at Age 12
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

School Readiness in 4-Year-Old Very Preterm Children

1
Abigail Wexner Research Institute, Nationwide Children’s Hospital, Columbus, OH 43215, USA
2
Department of Pediatrics, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
3
Department of Epidemiology, Rollins School of Public Health, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
4
Department of Pediatrics, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2022, 9(3), 323; https://doi.org/10.3390/children9030323
Submission received: 10 January 2022 / Revised: 10 February 2022 / Accepted: 21 February 2022 / Published: 1 March 2022

Abstract

:
The aims of this study were to identify the aspects of school readiness that best distinguish very preterm (VPT) preschoolers from full-term (FT) controls, determine the extent to which readiness problems in the VPT group reflected global cognitive weaknesses or more specific deficits, and identify distinct profiles of readiness problems. Fifty-three VPT (gestational age ≤ 30 weeks) 4-year-olds were compared to 38 FT (gestational age ≥ 37 weeks) controls on measures of global cognitive ability, executive function, motor skills, early literacy and numeracy, and psychosocial functioning. Latent class analysis (LCA) was also conducted to identify individual readiness profiles. The VPT group had the most pronounced difficulties on tests of spatial and nonverbal cognitive abilities, executive function, motor skills, phonological processing, and numeracy. The VPT group also had sex-related difficulties in processing speed, social functioning, and emotion regulation. These differences were evident in analyses of both continuous scores and rates of deficits. The VPT group’s difficulties in motor skills, and VPT females’ difficulties in social functioning and emotion regulation, were evident even when controlling for global cognitive ability. LCA suggested four profiles of readiness, with the majority of the VPT group assigned to profiles characterized by relative weaknesses in either cognitive abilities or psychosocial functioning or by more global readiness problems. The findings support the need to evaluate multiple aspects of school readiness in VPT preschoolers and inform efforts to design more targeted early educational interventions.

1. Introduction

Very preterm (VPT) children, typically defined as those born at a gestational age (GA) < 32 weeks, are at higher risk for deficits in academic achievement than full-term (FT) children [1,2]. These problems persist across the school-age years, are inversely related to the degree of prematurity at birth, and are preceded by earlier developmental delays [1,2,3,4,5,6].
According to the American Academy of Pediatrics [7] and an updated technical report [8], school readiness covers a broad range of characteristics conducive to learning, including health and physical/motor development, social–emotional adjustment, approaches to learning as exemplified by motivation and self-control, communication skills, and general knowledge (including knowledge of letters and numbers) and cognition. More recently, the concept of school readiness has been expanded to include factors outside the child, such as family, school, and community supports, that can foster readiness if present or hamper it if absent [8,9]. In an Australian sample, a delay in at least one of these characteristics was observed for 44% of VPT 5-year-olds compared to 16% of FT controls [10]. Researchers in New Zealand documented generalized delays in school readiness skills at age 4 years in VPT children compared to FT controls [4]. In the latter study, delays in multiple areas of readiness were three times more common in the VPT group compared to the FT group and predicted later deficits in academic achievement at ages 6 and 9 years. Other studies provide additional evidence for deficits in cognitive and emotional self-regulation in VPT preschoolers and associations of these deficits with learning problems [11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19].
Findings indicating that some VPT children have selective deficits in executive function, motor skills, and mathematics, while others appear free of any discernable impairments, raises the possibility of individual variability in school readiness profiles [20,21,22]. Individual variability is also supported by findings that suggest different profiles of school readiness skills in community samples of children [9,23,24] and of behavior problems in extremely preterm (GA < 28 weeks) children [25]. Using cluster or latent class analysis (LCA), these studies identified profiles suggesting that some children have pervasive problems in readiness skills or behavior while others have more selective deficits or are functioning well in all areas.
Findings from two more recent studies that employed LCA provide additional support for different profiles of cognitive, motor, and behavior outcomes at early school age. In following a large French sample of VPT children (GA < 32 weeks) to age 5.5 years for the EPIPAGE-2 Study, Twilhaar and colleagues [26] identified four profiles of cognitive, motor, and psychosocial characteristics. One profile was characterized by outcomes that were comparable to those of a FT sample, a second by more difficulties in psychosocial adjustment than in cognitive and motor skills, a third by more problems in cognitive and motor abilities than in psychosocial adjustment, and a fourth by difficulties in all areas relative to the FT children. A study of another sample of 5-year-old VPT children (GA < 30 weeks) and a FT comparison group also identified four profiles [27]. One profile was characterized by age-typical cognitive, motor, language, and psychosocial characteristics, a second by mild cognitive, motor, and language deficits in the context of more typical psychiatric ratings, a third by parent reports of pronounced psychosocial problems, and a fourth by teacher reports of relatively severe symptoms of attention problems and autism spectrum disorder.
However, evidence of individual differences in profiles of school readiness among VPT preschoolers is limited. The relationship between cognitive or motor deficits and behavior problems in these children is also unclear. Although cognitive impairments are associated with behavior problems in VPT cohorts [28,29,30], further research is needed to examine the possibility of relatively isolated deficits in behavior or performance-based measures of cognitive and motor functioning in preschoolers.
The objectives of this study were to enhance knowledge of the effects of VPT birth on school readiness in three ways. First, we compared groups of VPT preschoolers and FT controls on multiple measures of readiness to identify the breadth and magnitude of readiness problems in the VPT group and to confirm that our cohort had deficits similar to those observed in seminal studies of VPT preschoolers [8,9,10,13,17,20,24,25,26]. The two groups were compared on both continuous measures of readiness and rates of deficits on these measures. A second objective was to examine the extent to which problems in different readiness domains varied independently of one another. Thirdly, we conducted LCA to examine evidence for distinct profiles of readiness and their association with group membership.
Information regarding the type and magnitude of VPT preschoolers’ deficits in readiness will help in targeting interventions for these children’s most pressing needs. Evidence for distinct types or profiles of deficits will also contribute to an improved understanding of individual variability in readiness problems and will guide in creating assessments of readiness that are sensitive to the full range of readiness problems. Based on research on the consequences of VPT birth in young children, we hypothesized that VPT preschoolers would differ from FT controls on a broad array of readiness measures, but that the magnitude of these differences would vary across measures. Consistent with past evidence for individual variability in outcomes of VPT birth, we also anticipated multiple types and patterns of group differences.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

As described in studies of resilience and positive adaptation in VPT preschoolers [31,32], VPT children were recruited by contacting families of children treated in the Follow-Up program for a network of neonatal intensive care units affiliated with Nationwide Children’s Hospital in Columbus, Ohio USA. To examine outcomes in a higher-risk VPT preschool sample, only children with GA ≤ 30 weeks were included. Children had to be 4 years of age at the time of recruitment. Children from non-English speaking families and those with genetic abnormalities known to affect cognition were excluded. Given our interests in examining performance-based measures of readiness, we also excluded children with severe sensory impairment. To capture as many children as possible within the recruitment period (September 2018 to June 2019), VPT preschoolers were recruited according to age (older to younger) from a larger hospital-based sample of VPT children. The VPT group comprised 53 of 60 eligible children. Seven children were not seen because of missed or canceled appointments. Comparison of these 7 children to the participants failed to reveal significant differences in GA, age at assessment, or sex (ps > 0.05). FT controls included 38 children recruited at 4 years of age during the same period as the VPT group. Recruitment was from flyers emailed to hospital employees who had volunteered for research.
Table 1 summarizes the birth and demographic characteristics of the two groups. The VPT group had a significantly lower z-score composite of SES (zSES) than the FT group (p < 0.001), with zSES defined as the mean of sample z-scores for measures of caregiver education, occupation, and census-based median family income [33]. The groups did not differ significantly in age at assessment, sex, or race.

2.2. Procedures and Measures

One group of examiners administered a battery of readiness tests to the children while different examiners supervised caregivers’ completion of child behavior rating scales. Children’s birth status was not shared with the child examiners. Child testing was completed in four half-hour sessions spanning approximately three hours, with breaks between the sessions. Tasks were administered in one of three orders, with children assigned randomly to an order. The study was approved by the hospital Institutional Review Board and caregivers provided informed consent before participation.
Measures of readiness were divided into the five domains listed in Table 2: global cognitive ability, executive function and processing speed, motor skills, early literacy and numeracy skills, and behavior problems. Because of the extensive measures of behavior problems provided by the Conners Early Childhood-Parent questionnaire (Conners EC-P) [36], only scores for total behavior problems and for subdomains previously shown to be adversely affected by preterm birth were considered [37]. Scores were age-adjusted for measures with normative standards. Adjustments were made for chronological rather than corrected age given that school readiness is typically evaluated based on age since birth. Age norms were not available for the Zoo Go No Go Test [38] and Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC) [39]; thus, raw scores were used in analyses of these measures. Table 2 includes brief descriptions of the traits assessed by each measure and citations that document acceptable levels of reliability and validity. Test validity is further documented by previous research with young VPT or very low birth weight (<1500 g) children using these or similar tests [11,12,40,41,42,43].

2.3. Analysis

Mixed model analyses were used to examine group differences. Covariates in these and other analyses included sex and zSES. A random effect accounted for dependence between sibling participants. The relative magnitudes of group differences were assessed in terms of effect size (ES).
The possibility that the VPT and FT groups differed along multiple dimensions of school readiness was examined in two ways. First, the above-noted mixed model analyses were repeated controlling for the Differential Ability Scales, 2nd Edition General Conceptual Ability (DAS-II GCA) [44] to determine if group differences could be attributed to differences in global cognitive ability. Second, a general estimating equation (GEE) analysis was conducted to examine the possibility of multiple independent associations of readiness measures with group membership. Predictors in this analysis were limited to the one measure from each of the five domains that discriminated the groups with the largest ES.
Additional analyses using GEE were conducted to compare the groups (coded as FT = 0 and VPT = 1) on rates of deficits. Deficits were defined as scores ≥ 1SD below the mean on the performance measures and ERC Emotion Regulation, and scores ≥ 1SD above the mean on all ratings of behavior problems except for ERC Emotion Regulation. To determine if the groups differed in rates of deficits, the number of deficits was tallied across individuals, and the groups were compared on rates of two or more deficits. Although group differences in analyses using both mixed models and GEE were similar whether scores were adjusted for chronological age or for GA, only results from analysis of scores adjusted for chronological age are reported here.
Because of the descriptive nature of the study and limited sample size, an uncorrected p-level of <0.05 was applied to determine statistical significance. Information on the magnitude of effects (EFs) was provided by Cohen’s d for group comparisons on continuous measures and by odds ratios (and 95% confidence intervals) for results from GEEs. Small, medium and large effects from mixed models were defined as Cohen’s d’s of 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8, respectively [51]. Cohen’s d was calculated using the estimated marginal means and standard errors. These values adjust for the effects of the covariates (e.g., zSES) on the point estimates.
LCA was used to investigate individual differences in profiles of deficits. Variables included in these analyses were most of those listed in Table 2. Measures excluded from these analyses were the DAS-II GCA in order to focus on potential variations across profiles in the verbal, nonverbal, and spatial ability scores. Subscales of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool (BRIEF-P) [50] and Movement Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd Edition (MABC-2) [47] were also excluded to limit the number of variables. The Children’s Test of Phonological Processing, 2nd Edition (CTOPP-2) [46] rapid naming tests were excluded because of missing data due to many children’s inability to consistently name the stimuli. Analyses were conducted using Mplus version 8 and SAS 9.4. For families with multiple participants, one child from each family was randomly selected to avoid biases related to family clustering, with preference given to children with the most complete data. Seventeen children (15 PT, 2 FT) were excluded on this basis, resulting in a total sample of 75 children for LCA (39 VPT, 36 FT).
LCA models were run with 1 to 5 latent classes. To determine the best-fitting model, each model was compared to the model with one fewer class. The best-fitting model was selected based on fit statistics and interpretability. Model fit was assessed using the Bayesian information criteria (BIC), Akaike information criteria (AIC), adjusted BIC, Vuong–Lo–Mendell-Rubin test, Lo–Mendell test, and bootstrap likelihood ratio test. Lower values on BIC, AIC and adjusted BIC indicated better model fit. A significant p-value on the Vuong–Lo–Mendell–Rubin test, Lo–Mendel-Rubin test, or bootstrap likelihood ratio test indicated a better fit for the less parsimonious model (e.g., the 2-class over the 1-class model). Once the best-fitting model was determined, it was used to classify children into their most likely class. Entropy was used to determine classification accuracy. The value of entropy ranges from 0 to 1, with higher values indicating better accuracy. Finally, analyses of variance were conducted to compare the children assigned to each class on the readiness measures, group membership, sex, and zSES.

3. Results

3.1. Group Differences in Continuous Measures of Readiness

The VPT group had significantly lower scores on all tests except the NIH Toolbox Dimensional Change Card Sorting Test [45] and all caregiver behavior ratings except the Conners EC-P Global Index, Inattention/Hyperactivity, and Anxiety scales, BRIEF-P Inhibitory Self-Control indices, and Flexibility indices, and ERC Lability/Negativity (see Table 3). Large ESs were found for DAS-II GCA, Nonverbal Ability, Spatial Ability, Recall of Digits-Forward, and Early Number Concepts; MABC-2 Total, Manual Dexterity, and Balance; and the Phonological Awareness subtest of the Test of Preschool Early Literacy (TOPEL) [48]. Analyses also revealed significant group × sex interactions for the CTOPP-2 Rapid Symbolic Naming, Conners EC-P Social Functioning, and ERC Emotion Regulation, as well as a significant group × zSES interaction for MABC-2 Aiming and Catching. VPT boys had lower CTOPP-2 Rapid Symbolic Naming scores than FT boys (p = 0.002; ES = −1.18). Compared to FT girls, VPT girls had higher Conners EC-P Social Functioning (i.e., worse social functioning) (p < 0.001, ES = 1.42) and lower ERC Emotion Regulation (p < 0.001, ES = 1.20).
Main effects for sex and zSES were significant in analyses of several of the measures (data not shown). Boys had higher scores than girls on DAS-II Recall of Digits-Forward and lower scores on the MABC-2 Total. Higher zSES was associated with higher scores on DAS-II Spatial Ability and TOPEL Print Knowledge and with lower ratings of behavior problems on the Conners EC-P Global Index and Inattention/Hyperactivity; BRIEF GEC, Inhibitory Self-Control, Flexibility, and Emergent Metacognition; and ERC Lability/Negativity.
Even when adjusting for the DAS-II GCA, differences remained significant for MABC-2 Total (p = 0.002, ES = 0.74) and Manual Dexterity (p = 0.015, ES = 0.60). Consistent with results from analyses that did not adjust for the DAS-II GCA, these analyses also revealed significant group × sex interactions for Conners EC-P Social Functioning (p < 0.001) and ERC Emotion Regulation (p = 0.003). VPT girls had significantly higher ratings of problems than FT girls on Conners EC-P Social Functioning (p < 0.001, ES = 1.21) and lower ratings on ERC Emotion Regulation (p = 0.003, ES = −1.11). Group differences in these measures were not significant for boys.
In the GEE analysis that included the measure from each domain with the largest ES as predictors of group membership (DAS-II GCA, Recall of Digits-Forward, and Early Number Concepts; MABC-2 Total; and BRIEF-P Emergent Metacognition Index), the MABC-2 Total was the only measure significantly associated with group independently of the other predictors, beta (standard error) = 0.35 (0.14), p = 0.011.

3.2. Group Differences in Rates of Deficits

Similar to results from mixed model analyses, odds of deficits were significantly higher for the VPT group than for the FT group on most of the measures (see Table 4). Rates of deficits on the performance tests for which group differences were significant ranged from 18% to 74% for the VPT group compared to 0% to 26% for the FT group. A significantly higher proportion of the VPT group also had deficits on Conners EC-P Social Functioning, BRIEF-P GEC, Inhibitory Self-Control, and Emergent Metacognition. Rates of deficits on these measures ranged from 32% to 57% for the VPT group compared to 3% to 16% for the FT group. Deficits in multiple readiness measures were also significantly higher in the VPT group compared to the FT group (83% versus 32%).

3.3. Readiness Profiles

Model fit statistics were better for the 4- and 5-class models relative to the 1-, 2- and 3-class models (Table 5), thus the 1- to 3-class models were rejected. The 5-class model compared less favorably to the 4-class model based on Entropy and BIC. The 4-class model, shown in Figure 1, was selected as it was more interpretable and resulted in lower BIC indices and higher entropy relative to the 5-class model.
The 37 children assigned to latent class 1 had the best readiness outcomes, with scores on performance tests and behavior ratings falling well within the average range. Only eight VPT children were in this class (22% of class). Scores on all performance tests were lower for the 20 participants in latent class 2 than for those in class 1. Fourteen of these children (70%) were from the VPT group. Although latent class 2 participants had higher test scores than those in latent classes 3 or 4, they had more behavior problems than the 14 children in latent class 3, of whom 13 (93%) were from the VPT group. The four participants assigned to latent class 4, all from the VPT group, had the worst outcomes on all readiness measures.
Although group separation is optimized in LCA, support for these interpretations was provided by pairwise comparisons of readiness scores for the four latent classes. Specifically: (1) each of the four latent classes differed significantly from the other classes on the DAS-II Verbal and Spatial Ability and Conners EC-P Anxiety; (2) class 1 had significantly higher test scores than classes 2 and 3 on DAS-II Verbal, Nonverbal, and Spatial composites, DAS-II Recalling of Digits-Forward, TOPEL Print Knowledge and Phonological Awareness, DAS-II Early Number Concepts, and MABC-2, as well as significantly lower ratings on Conners EP-C Anxiety; and (3) class 2 had higher scores than class 3 on TOPEL Phonological Awareness but significantly higher ratings of behavior problems on Conners EC-P Inattention/Hyperactivity and Anxiety, BRIEF-P GEC, and ERC Lability/Negativity. The four latent classes also differed significantly in group membership (significantly higher proportion of the FT group in class 1 compared to the class 3), zSES (significantly higher in class 1 compared to class 2), and GA (significantly higher in class 1 compared to classes 1, 2, and 3).

4. Discussion

This study compared 4-year-old VPT children to FT controls on a wide range of measures of school readiness to identify measures of readiness on which the VPT group had the greatest difficulty and to examine variations in readiness problems. The VPT group performed most poorly relative to the FT group on tests of spatial ability, verbal working memory, motor performance, phonological processing, and number skills. Problems in psychosocial functioning that distinguished the VPT group from FT controls were in areas of executive function, and, at least for females, in emotion regulation and social functioning, although the ESs corresponding to these differences were not as large as those for group differences on the aforementioned performance measures. Group differences in rates of deficits were also the most pronounced for the performance measures. For example, nearly one-third of VPT children had deficits in global cognition compared to none of the FT preschoolers. Deficits similar to those observed in this study are reported in previous studies of both VPT preschoolers [4,10,11,17,22,52] and school-age VPT children [16,33,37,53,54,55,56,57]. The present study adds to this literature by assessing the variability of impairment displayed by VPT preschoolers across a range of readiness measures.
Although differences between the VPT and FT group were largely independent of sex and zSES in this and other studies [18], deficits in CTOPP-2 Rapid Symbolic Naming were found only for VPT boys, and more caregiver-rated problems on the Conners EC-P Social Functioning and weakness in ERC Emotion Regulation were evident only for VPT girls. Despite inconsistencies in reports of sex-related differences in the consequences of VPT birth, some previous research supports the possibility of more pronounced effects for males than for females [58]. We are unaware of evidence to suggest that females are more vulnerable than males to adverse socioemotional consequences of VPT birth. Potential explanations include gender differences in early socialization processes [59] or gender-related biases in parent ratings of socioemotional functioning [60]. We also found that differences in favor of the FT group on motor skills, as assessed by the MABC-2 Total, were more pronounced among children with lower zSES. The moderating effect of SES on group differences in motor ability is similar to findings suggesting more adverse effects of maternal insensitivity on academic achievement for low birth weight children compared to normal birth weight youth [61], and raises the possibility that readiness problems in VPT preschoolers may be exacerbated by environmental disadvantage. The latter finding is also consistent with evidence for a greater intervention effect on motor outcome for VPT children at higher social risk [62]. However, support from our study for sex- and zSES-related differences in the effects of VPT birth were limited to these measures. In view of the lack of more consistent evidence for these factors as moderators of VPT outcomes from this study or past research, caution is advised in interpreting these findings pending larger-scale studies of VPT preschoolers’ school readiness.
Most of the group differences in school readiness were not significant when adjusting for the DAS-II GCA, suggesting that problems in school readiness for many VPT preschoolers are associated with weaknesses in global cognitive ability. The finding of higher rates of multiple deficits in the VPT preschoolers compared to the FT group also points to pervasive problems in readiness in many VPT children. The results confirm previous findings of pervasive developmental deficits in VPT cohorts, as well as observations of associations between cognitive weaknesses and behavior problems in these children [28,29].
However, other findings from this study indicate that some children may have more selective problems in readiness. To begin with, even controlling for the DAS-II GCA, the VPT group relative to FT controls performed more poorly on measures of motor skills, and VPT girls were rated by caregivers as having more problems in social functioning and lower levels of emotion regulation relative to FT girls. Second, in GEE analysis that included a set of the most discriminating readiness measures from each of the five domains as predictors of group membership, motor ability discriminated the groups independently of other measures of readiness. These results underscore motor skills as a critical domain in assessment of readiness [4,10,63,64], and suggest that, at least for females, some socio-emotional aspects of school readiness may be affected independently of their cognitive competencies.
Third, findings from the LCA provided support for four individual patterns of readiness competencies. Classes 1 and 4, respectively, identified preschoolers with either strengths or weaknesses across all readiness domains. The other two classes were characterized by scores on performance tests that fell between those of classes 1 and 4 but differed in their profiles of relative strengths and weaknesses. Relative to the children assigned to class 3, those in class 2 performed at somewhat higher levels on tests of cognitive and motor skills but also had higher caregiver ratings of behavior problems. These findings are consistent with research suggesting that some VPT children display age-typical outcomes across multiple developmental domains, while others have either selective or generalized impairments [21,22,25,26,27,31,41]. Research examining readiness profiles in community samples of kindergarteners or first graders reveals similar findings [9,23,24]. As was evident in the present sample, these studies identified subgroups of children with uniformly positive or negative outcomes across readiness domains, as well as subgroups showing dissociations between cognitive and behavioral aspects of readiness. Evidence for latent classes comprising both VPT and FT preschoolers (i.e., classes 1–3) is also consistent with results from prior LCAs that have classified children from both these groups into the same behavioral profiles [25,65].
These findings need to be considered in the context of several study limitations. First, because of the numerous school readiness measures examined, the relatively small sample, and the lack of correction for multiple comparisons, findings are viewed more as hypothesis-generating than as hypothesis-driven. The results likely depended on the measures administered and on sample characteristics. Assessments were comprehensive, but some aspects of readiness, such as children’s health status, physical development, and temperament, were not evaluated. The present study also did not consider the level of support provided by families and communities as additional contributors to school readiness [8,9].
Data from all children who participated in the study were included in the analysis, and the VPT group was similar to other VPT cohorts in terms of mean GA and neonatal complications [31]. However, recruitment did not ensure representativeness relative to the broader VPT population. Findings may be more informative of the nature of readiness problems in preschoolers capable of engaging in testing than of the outcomes of VPT birth more generally. Additionally, FT preschoolers were recruited from a pool of families that had volunteered for research and that had higher mean zSES than the families of VPT preschoolers. Although zSES was controlled in analyses, the VPT and FT groups were not matched on background characteristics and it is unclear how representative the FT group was of the broader regional community of preschool children.
Evidence from LCA of distinct profiles of readiness is also preliminary and requires replication with larger samples. Samples larger than the present one are commonly recommended in conducting LCA, though smaller samples may be needed in LCA involving more and higher-quality indicators [66,67]. The substantial number of readiness measures assessed in this study may thus have contributed to the reasonable fit indices obtained by the present LCA. The four profiles of readiness characteristics identified by the LCA are also similar to those reported in previous studies of older VPT children. Additional studies are nonetheless needed to further our understanding of the nature of individual differences in readiness among VPT preschoolers. Examination of associations of profile types with subsequent academic progress and with medical and environmental risk factors would also be useful, as has been conducted in slightly older samples of VPT children [9,26,27].
Despite these limitations, the results have important clinical implications for assessing, monitoring, and treating readiness difficulties in VPT preschoolers. The sensitivity of methods for identifying readiness problems would be enhanced by assessing the aspects of readiness most likely to be affected by VPT birth. Based on the present findings, these domains include performance on tests of spatial and nonverbal cognition, executive function, motor skills, phonological processing, and numeracy. Findings also support the inclusion of caregiver ratings of dysexecutive behavior and problems in social functioning and emotion regulation. Readiness problems may be manifest in global weaknesses in test performance and psychosocial adjustment, or may manifest as milder or more selective deficits. Inclusion of tests of motor skills and measures of social functioning and emotion regulation appears particularly important in designing assessments of readiness, given the evidence for weaknesses in these areas that are independent of global cognitive ability.
Weaknesses in multiple readiness measures in 70% of the VPT group underscore the critical need for universal monitoring of readiness skills in this population before school entry [5,10,15,68]. Beyond surveillance, results of monitoring assessments allow efficient and specific targeting of interventions to address aspects of readiness most impaired in VPT preschoolers. These approaches might entail efforts to remediate skill deficiencies or to accommodate for them in ways that minimize disruptions to the learning process. The need for broader-based preschool interventions is underscored by VPT preschoolers’ difficulties in multiple readiness domains, along with the potential negative consequences of delayed readiness on longer-term educational and employment outcomes [4,5,10,19]. Exemplary interventions are programs that involve both school- and family-based activities and that address children’s socioemotional functioning, as well as their specific weaknesses in cognitive, motor, and early literacy and numeracy skills [13,19]. As similar proportions of the VPT and FT groups were enrolled in preschool, the present findings align with previous evidence suggesting that preschool attendance alone, absent interventions directed to the specific needs of VPT children, is unlikely to narrow the “preschool readiness gap” [69].
Follow-up of the present sample into early school age would be informative in investigating readiness measures and profiles as predictors of school-age achievement. Although associations of readiness measures with later achievement in both VPT preschoolers and larger community samples are well-documented [4,19,23,24], further research is required to identify the best predictors. Evidence that certain measures of readiness are highly predictive of subsequent achievement would argue for their inclusion in preschool assessments. A better understanding of the sources of readiness problems will also require more research on the neonatal, early developmental, and environmental factors associated with these deficits. The present findings are consistent with previous research in documenting associations of greater sociodemographic disadvantage with difficulties on several of the readiness measures [5,17,63,70]. However, more emphasis on the relation of readiness to other, potentially modifiable, characteristics of the family or preschool setting would have greater utility in developing interventions.

5. Conclusions

The findings from this study suggest that effects of VPT birth on school readiness are most likely to be manifest in tests of spatial, nonverbal, and motor skills, executive function, phonological processing, and early number knowledge. Adverse effects on behavioral aspects of readiness, such as behavioral manifestations of executive deficits and problems in social functioning and emotion regulation, are also evident, but are less pronounced or related to sex. Variability in the types of readiness problems displayed by VPT preschoolers is supported by findings of independent effects of VPT birth on global cognition and motor skills and by variable profiles of readiness skills in the VPT group. These results will help guide the development of more effective approaches to identify those VPT children in need of more intensive early educational interventions and may also help target those interventions. Efforts to replicate the present findings with larger and more representative samples are critical in refining methods for identifying individual variations in readiness and informing interventions targeted to specific profiles of strengths and weaknesses.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.G.T., N.L.M., M.L.N., A.d.S.; methodology, H.G.T., N.L.M., M.L.N., A.d.S.; formal analysis, D.M.V., R.B., T.A.B., S.U.; investigation, M.L.N., A.d.S., T.A.B.; data curation, T.A.B., R.B.; writing—original draft preparation, H.G.T., D.M.V., R.B.; writing—review and editing, H.G.T., D.M.V., M.L.N., R.B., T.A.B., A.d.S., S.U., N.L.M.; supervision, H.G.T., N.L.M.; project administration, H.G.T., N.L.M.; funding acquisition, H.G.T., N.L.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Abigail Wexner Research Institute at Nationwide Children’s Hospital, cost center 40310-0008.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Nationwide Children’s Hospital (protocol IRB18-00446, date of approval August 2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available from the corresponding author subject to approval by the investigators, institutional review, and an approved data use agreement. The data are not yet publicly available pending sufficient time for the investigators to report on the findings.

Acknowledgments

The authors appreciate the assistance of Kerry Orton and Stephanie Burkhardt in project coordination and of examiners Sandra Glazer, Hanan Guzman, Kaleigh Hague, Brianna Laney, Julia Newton, Katelyn Keener, Megan Lightfoot, Ashley Widmayer, Gabrielle Moots, Caitlin Kjeldsen, Lindsey Pietruszewski, and Brianna Sowers. We acknowledge Joseph Rausch, in the Center for Biobehavioral Health at Nationwide Children’s Hospital and Nori Minich in the Department of Pediatrics at Case Western Reserve University for statistical consultation. We also acknowledge Sasha Key, of Vanderbilt University for creating a computer-based version of the Zoo Game Go No Go Test for this study and as a collaborator on the larger school readiness project. We are also grateful to the children and families who participated in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Johnson, S.; Wolke, D.; Hennessy, E.; Marlow, N. Educational Outcomes in Extremely Preterm Children: Neuropsychological Correlates and Predictors of Attainment. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2011, 36, 74–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Pritchard, V.E.; Clark, C.A.; Liberty, K.; Champion, P.R.; Wilson, K.; Woodward, L.J. Early school-based learning difficulties in children born very preterm. Early Hum. Dev. 2009, 85, 215–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Aylward, G.P. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of infants born prematurely. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2014, 35, 394–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Pritchard, V.E.; Bora, S.; Austin, N.C.; Levin, K.J.; Woodward, L.J. Identifying very preterm children at educational risk using a school readiness framework. Pediatrics 2014, 134, e825–e832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Shah, P.E.; Kaciroti, N.; Richards, B.; Lumeng, J.C. Gestational age and kindergarten school readiness in a national sample of preterm infants. J. Pediatr. 2016, 178, 61–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Taylor, H.G.; Klein, N.; Anselmo, M.G.; Minich, N.; Espy, K.A.; Hack, M. Learning problems in kindergarten students with extremely preterm birth. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2011, 165, 819–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. High, P.C.; The Pediatrics Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption and Dependent Care and Council on School Health. School readiness. Pediatrics 2008, 121, e1008–e1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Williams, P.G.; Lerner, M.A.; Council On Early Childhood; Council On School Health; Sells, J.; Alderman, S.L.; Hashikawa, A.; Mendelsohn, M.A.; Mc Fadden, T.; Navsaria, D.; et al. School readiness. Pediatrics 2019, 144, e20191766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Pan, Q.; Trang, K.T.; Love, H.R.; Templin, J. School readiness profiles and growth in academic achievement. Front. Educ. 2019, 4, 127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Roberts, G.; Lim, J.; Doyle, L.W.; Anderson, P.J. High rates of school readiness difficulties at 5 years of age in very preterm infants compared with term controls. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2011, 32, 117–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Baron, I.S.; Erickson, K.; Ahronovich, M.D.; Baker, R.; Litman, F.R. Neuropsychological and behavioral outcomes of extremely low birth weight at age three. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2011, 36, 5–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Baron, I.S.; Kerns, K.A.; Muller, U.; Ahronovich, M.D.; Litman, F.R. Executive functions in extremely low birth weight and late-preterm preschoolers: Effects on working memory and response inhibition. Child Neuropsychol. 2012, 18, 586–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Blair, C.; Raver, C.C. School readiness and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological approach. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2015, 66, 711–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Clark, C.A.C.; Pritchard, V.E.; Woodward, L.J. Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 46, 1176–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Foster-Cohen, S.H.; Friesen, M.D.; Champion, P.R.; Woodward, L.J. High prevalence/low severity language delay in preschool children born very preterm. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2010, 31, 658–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Hasler, H.M.; Akshoomoff, N. Mathematics ability and related skills in preschoolers born very preterm. Child Neuropsychol. 2019, 25, 162–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Jones, K.M.; Champion, P.R.; Woodward, L.J. Social competence of preschool children born very preterm. Early Hum. Dev. 2013, 89, 795–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Taylor, H.G.; Klein, N.; Espy, K.A.; Schluchter, M.; Minich, N.; Stilp, R.; Hack, M. Effects of extreme prematurity and kindergarten neuropsychological skills on early academic progress. Neuropsychology 2018, 32, 809–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Woodward, L.J.; Lu, Z.; Morris, A.; Healey, D. Self regulatory skills during early childhood are associated with later mental health and educational achievement in very preterm and typically developing children. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2017, 31, 404–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Anderson, P.J. Neuropsychological outcomes of children born very preterm. Semin. Fetal Neonatal Med. 2014, 19, 90–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Anderson, P.J.; Doyle, L.W. Cognitive and educational deficits in children born extremely preterm. Semin. Perinatol. 2008, 32, 51–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Woodward, L.J.; Moor, S.; Hood, K.M.; Champion, P.R.; Foster-Cohen, S.; Inder, T.E.; Austin, N.C. Very preterm children show impairments across multiple neurodevelopmental domains by age 4 years. Arch. Dis. Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2009, 94, F339–F344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Hair, E.; Halle, T.; Terry-Humen, E.; Lavelle, B.; Calkins, J. Children’s school readiness in the ECLS-K: Predictions to academic, health, and social outcomes in first grade. Early Child. Res. Q. 2006, 21, 431–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Konold, T.R.; Pianta, R.C. Empirically-derived, person-oriented patterns of school readiness in typically-developing children: Description and prediction to first-grade achievement. Appl. Dev. Sci. 2005, 9, 174–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Burnett, A.C.; Youssef, G.; Anderson, P.J.; Duff, J.; Doyle, L.W.; Cheong, J.L.Y.; Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. Exploring the “preterm behavioral phenotype” in children born extremely preterm. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2019, 40, 200–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Twilhaar, E.S.; Pierrat, V.; Marchand-Martin, L.; Benhammou, V.; Kaminski, M.; Ancel, P.Y. Profiles of functioning in 5.5-year-old very preterm born children in france: The EPIPAGE-2 study. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Lean, R.E.; Lessov-Shlaggar, C.N.; Gerstein, E.D.; Smyser, T.A.; Paul, R.A.; Smyser, C.D.; Rogers, C.E. Maternal and family factors differentiate profiles of psychiatric impairments in very preterm children at age 5-years. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2020, 61, 157–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Alduncin, N.; Huffman, L.C.; Feldman, H.M.; Loe, I.M. Executive function is associated with social competence in preschool-aged children born preterm or full term. Early Hum. Dev. 2014, 90, 299–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Delobel-Ayoub, M.; Arnaud, C.; White-Koning, M.; Casper, C.; Pierrat, V.; Garel, M.; Burguet, A.; Roze, J.C.; Matis, J.; Picaud, J.C.; et al. Behavioral problems and cognitive performance at 5 years of age after very preterm birth: The EPIPAGE Study. Pediatrics 2009, 123, 1485–1492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Witt, A.; Theurel, A.; Tolsa, C.B.; Lejeune, F.; Fernandes, L.; de Jonge, L.; Monnier, M.; Bickle Graz, M.; Barisnikov, K.; Gentaz, E.; et al. Emotional and effortful control abilities in 42-month-old very preterm and full-term children. Early Hum. Dev. 2014, 90, 565–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. De Silva, A.; Neel, M.L.; Maitre, N.; Busch, T.; Taylor, H.G. Resilience and vulnerability in very preterm 4-year-olds. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2021, 35, 904–924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Neel, M.L.; de Silva, A.; Taylor, H.G.; Benninger, K.; Busch, T.; Hone, E.; Moore-Clingenpeel, M.; Pietruszewski, L.; Maitre, N.L. Exceeding expectations after perinatal risks for poor development: Associations in term- and preterm-born preschoolers. J. Perinatol. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Orchinik, L.J.; Taylor, H.G.; Espy, K.A.; Minich, N.; Klein, N.; Sheffield, T.; Hack, M. Cognitive outcomes for extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children in kindergarten. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 1067–1079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Mohammad, K.; Scott, J.N.; Leijser, L.M.; Zein, H.; Afifi, J.; Piedboeuf, B.; de Vries, L.S.; van Wezel-Meijler, G.; Lee, S.K.; Shah, P.S. Consensus Approach for Standardizing the Screening and Classification of Preterm Brain Injury Diagnosed with Cranial Ultrasound: A Canadian Perspective. Front. Pediatr. 2021, 9, 618236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Papile, L.A.; Burstein, J.; Burstein, R.; Koffler, H. Incidence and evolution of subependymal and intraventricular hemorrhage: A study of infants with birth weights less than 1500 gm. J. Pediatr. 1978, 92, 529–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Conners, C.K. Conners Early Childhood.; Multi-Health Systems, Inc.: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  37. Johnson, S.; Marlow, N. Preterm birth and childhood psychiatric disorders. Pediatr. Res. 2011, 69, 11R–18R. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lamm, C.; Walker, O.L.; Degnan, K.A.; Henderson, H.A.; Pine, D.S.; McDermott, J.M.; Fox, N.A. Cognitive control moderates early childhood temperament in predicting social behavior in 7-year-old children: An ERP study. Dev. Sci. 2012, 17, 667–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Shields, A.; Cicchetti, D. Emotion regulation among school-age children: The development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Dev. Psychol. 1997, 33, 906–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Griffiths, A.; Morgan, P.; Anderson, P.J.; Doyle, L.W.; Lee, K.J.; Spittle, A.J. Predictive value of the movement assessment battery for children—Second Edition at 4 years, for motor impairment at 8 years in children born preterm. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2017, 59, 490–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Taylor, H.G.; Minich, N.; Schluchter, M.; Espy, K.A.; Klein, N. Resilience in Extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight kindergarten children. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2019, 25, 362–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Wiley, C.P.; Wedeking, T.; Galindo, A.M. Test review: C.K. Test Review: CK Conners. Conners early childhood manual. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2013, 31, 606–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Zelazo, P.D.; Muller, U.; Frye, D.; Marcovitch, S.; Argitis, G.; Boseovski, J.; Chiang, J.K.; Hongwanishkul, D.; Schuster, B.V.; Sutherland, A. The development of executive function in early childhood. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 2003, 68, vii-137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Elliot, C.D. Differential Ability Scales, 2nd ed.; Harcourt Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  45. Casaletto, K.B.; Umlauf, A.; Beaumont, J.; Gershon, R.; Slotkin, J.; Akshoomoff, N.; Heaton, R.K. Demographically corrected normative standards for the English version of the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2015, 21, 378–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Wagner, R.; Torgensen, J.; Rashotte, C.; Pearson, N. Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing, 2nd ed.; PRO-ED: Austin, TX, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  47. Henderson, S.E.; Sugden, D.A.; Barnett, A. Movement Assessment Battery for Children (Movement ABC-2): Examiner’s Manual, 2nd ed.; Harcourt Assessment: London, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  48. Lonigan, C.J.; Wagner, R.K.; Torgensen, J.K.; Rashotte, C.A. TOPEL: Test of Preschool Early Literacy; Pro-Ed: Austin, TX, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  49. Wilson, S.B.; Lonigan, C.J. An evaluation of two emergent literacy screening tools for preschool children. Ann. Dyslexia 2009, 59, 115–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Gioia, G.A.; Espy, K.A.; Esquith, P.K. BRIEF-P: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool Version; Psychological Assessment Resources: Lutz, FL, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  51. Cohen, J. A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 112, 155–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Pitchford, N.; Johnson, S.; Scerif, G.; Marlow, N. Early indications of delayed cognitive development in preschool children born very preterm: Evidence from domain-general and domain-specific tasks. Infant Child Dev. 2011, 20, 400–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Baron, I.S.; Brandt, J.; Ahronovich, M.D.; Baker, R.; Erickson, K.; Litman, F.R. Selective deficit in spatial location memory in extremely low birth weight children at age six: The PETIT study. Child Neuropsychol. 2012, 18, 299–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Larson, J.C.G.; Barson, I.S.; Erickson, K.; Ahronovich, M.D.; Baker, R.; Litman, F.R. Neuromotor outcomes at school age after extremely low birth weight: Early detection of subtle signs. Neuropsychology 2011, 25, 66–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Scott, M.N.; Taylor, H.G.; Fristad, M.A.; Klein, N.; Espy, K.A.; Minich, N.; Hack, M. Behavior disorders in extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight children in kindergarten. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2012, 33, 202–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Taylor, H.G. Neurodevelopmental origins of social competence in very preterm children. Semin. Fetal Neonatal. Med. 2020, 25, 101108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Taylor, H.G.; Clark, C.A.C. Executive function in children born preterm: Risk factors and implications for outcome. Semin. Perinatol. 2016, 40, 520–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. O’Driscoll, D.N.; McGovern, M.; Greene, C.M.; Molloy, E.J. Gender disparities in preterm neonatal outcomes. Acta Paediatr. 2018, 107, 1494–1499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Underwood, M.K.; Galen, B.R.; Paquette, J.A. Top ten challenges for understanding gender and aggression in children: Why can’t we all just get along? Soc. Dev. 2001, 10, 248–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Najman, J.M.; Williams, G.M.; Nikles, J.; Spence, S.; Bor, W.; O’Callaghan, M.; Le Brocque, R.; Andersen, M.J.; Shuttlewood, G.J. Bias influencing maternal reports of child behaviour and emotional state. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2001, 36, 186–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Jaekel, J.; Pluess, M.; Belsky, J.; Wolke, D. Effects of maternal sensitivity on low birth weight children’s academic achievement: A test of differential susceptibility versus diathesis stress. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2015, 56, 693–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Spittle, A.J.; Treyvaud, K.; Lee, K.J.; Anderson, P.J.; Doyle, L.W. The role of social risk in an early preventative care programme for infants born very preterm: A randomized controlled trial. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2018, 60, 54–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Benavente-Fernandez, I.; Synnes, A.; Grunau, R.E.; Chau, V.; Ramraj, C.; Glass, T.; Cayam-Rand, D.; Siddiqi, A.; Miller, S.P. Association of socioeconomic status and brain injury with neurodevelopmental outcomes of very preterm children. JAMA Netw. Open 2019, 2, e192914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Duncan, A.F.; Bann, C.M.; Maitre, N.L.; Peralta-Carcelen, M.; Hintz, S.R.; Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health; Development Neonatal Research Network. Hand function at 18–22 months is associated with school-age manual dexterity and motor performance in children born extremely preterm. J. Pediatr. 2020, 225, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Johnson, S.; Waheed, G.; Manktelow, B.N.; Field, D.J.; Marlow, N.; Draper, E.S.; Boyle, E.M. Differentiating the preterm phenotype: Distinct profiles of cognitive and behavioral development following late and moderately preterm birth. J. Pediatr. 2018, 193, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Weller, B.E.; Bowen, N.K.; Faubert, S.J. Latent class analysis: A guide to best practice. J. Black Psychol. 2020, 46, 287–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Wurpts, I.C.; Geiser, C. Is adding more indicators to a latent class analysis beneficial or detrimental? Results of a monte-carlo study. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Chung, P.J.; Opipari, V.P.; Koolwijk, I. Executive function and extremely preterm children. Pediatr. Res. 2017, 82, 565–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Chen, J.H.; Claessens, A.; Msall, M.E. Prematurity and school readiness in a nationally representative sample of Australian children: Does typically occurring preschool moderate the relationship? Early Hum. Dev. 2014, 90, 73–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  70. Doyle, L.W.; Cheong, J.L.; Burnett, A.; Roberts, G.; Lee, K.J.; Anderson, P.J.; Victorian Infant Collaborative Study, G. Biological and social influences on outcomes of extreme-preterm/low-birth weight adolescents. Pediatrics 2015, 136, e1513–e1520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Latent class differences in mean z scores for readiness measures.
Figure 1. Latent class differences in mean z scores for readiness measures.
Children 09 00323 g001
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
CharacteristicGroup
VPT (n = 53)FT (n = 38)
Neonatal and early developmental status:
 Child age, mean (SD)4.7 (0.29)4.6 (0.29)
 Gestational age, mean in weeks (SD, range) a,b27.3 (1.9, 23–30)39.1 (1.1, 37–41)
 Birth weight in grams, mean (SD, range) a,b944 (270, 369–1644)3249 (435, 2325–4167)
 Multiple birth c, n (%)10 (45) 2 (11)
 Medical complications
  Grade III-IVH or PVHI d, n (%)6 (11.32)-
  Periventricular leukomalacia e, n (%)10 (18.9)-
  Bronchopulmonary dysplasia requiring home oxygen, n (%)16 (30.2)-
  Intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR), n (%)7 (13.2)-
  Culture-positive sepsis, n (%)14 (26.4)-
  Necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC) f, n (%)6 (11.3)-
  Retinopathy of prematurity (ROP) requiring laser surgery, n (%)4 (7.5)-
Family demographic characteristics:
 Child sex: male, n (%)27 (50.9)18 (47.4)
 Child race:
  White, n (%)35 (66)28 (73.7)
  Black/African American, n (%)10 (18.9)3 (7.9)
  Asian, n (%)1 (1.9)1 (2.6)
  More than one race, n (%)7 (13.2)6 (15.8)
 Child enrolled in preschool, n (%)46 (86.8)32 (84.2)
 zSES Composite, mean (SD) a−0.25 (0.73)0.35 (0.59)
Note. VPT = very preterm; FT = full term; IVH = intraventricular hemorrhage; PVHI = periventricular hemorrhagic infarction; zSES = sample z score composite of socioeconomic status. a Significant difference between groups, p < 0.05. b Data on these variables obtained from the medical record for all VPT children. These data were also obtained from the medical record for a majority of the FT group and from parent report when the medical record was not accessible. c Because several of the children were members of multiples and one was a sibling pair, a total of 39 families with VPT children and 36 with FT children participated in the study. d Grade III IVH is defined as hemorrhage with ventricular dilation and PVHI as hemorrhagic infarction in the periventricular region [34,35]. e PVL describes white matter injury. Consistent with other studies [34], we defined PVL as echogenecities in white matter that did not resolve on subsequent imaging and/or cysts or other evidence of white matter injury as identified by a radiologist. f Defined as Bells stage II or above and/or requiring surgical intervention.
Table 2. Readiness measures.
Table 2. Readiness measures.
Domain/MeasureReferenceDescriptionScore
Global cognitive ability (DAS-II):[44] Standard Scores
 DAS-II General Conceptual Ability Global ability composite
 DAS-II Verbal Ability Verbal comprehension and naming
 DAS-II Nonverbal Ability Nonverbal reasoning
 DAS-II Spatial Ability Perceptual motor skills
Executive function and processing speed:
 NIH Toolbox Dimensional Change Card Sort[45]Cognitive ability/rule shiftingT-score a
 DAS-II Recall of Digits—Forward[44]Verbal working memoryT-score
 Zoo Game Go No Go Task b[38]Attention and inhibitionProportion correct
 CTOPP-2 Rapid Symbolic Naming c[46]Speed of namingStandard score
Motor skills (MABC-2):[47] Scaled scores
 Total Composite of motor skills
 Manual Dexterity Fine motor skills
 Balance Gross motor balance
 Aiming and Catching Eye–hand coordination
Early literacy and numeracy:
 TOPEL Print Knowledge[48,49]Letter names and soundsStandard score
 TOPEL Phonological Awareness Ability to identify phonemesStandard score
 DAS-II Early Number Concepts[44]Counting and math problem solvingT-score
Behavior ratings:
 Conners EC-P Total Problems, Inattention/Hyperactivity, Anxiety, Social Functioning[36,42]Symptoms of overall problems and in areas of attention, externalizing, and socializationT-scores
 BRIEF-P Global Executive Composite, Inhibitory Self-Control Index, Flexibility Index, Emergent Metacognition Index[50]Behavior symptoms of executive dysfunctionT-scores
 ERC Lability/Negativity, Emotion Regulation[39]Symptoms of mood lability and ability to regulate emotionsRaw scores
Note. DAS-II = Differential Ability Scales, 2nd Edition; CTOPP-2 = Children’s Test of Phonological Processing, 2nd Edition; MABC-2 = Movement Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd Edition; TOPEL = Test of Preschool Early Literacy; Conners EC-P = Conners Early Childhood—Parent; BRIEF-P = Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool Version; ERC = Emotion Regulation Checklist. a Age-adjusted T-score. b The Zoo Go No Go Test was administered via computer during collection of evoked response potential data. The task served to index and time a series of trials as they appeared in the EEG stream, though only test performance was considered in this study. c For children with scorable performance on both Rapid Color Naming and Rapid Object Naming, CTOPP-2 Rapid Symbolic Naming was the composite of these two subtests. For the several children who were unable to complete one of these subtests due to lack of knowledge of color or object names, the Total score was based on the score for the completed subtest.
Table 3. Group differences in readiness measures.
Table 3. Group differences in readiness measures.
VPT GroupFT Group
School Readiness MeasureM (SE)M (SE)βpCohen’s d
Global/cognitive ability (DAS-II):
 General Conceptual Ability92.17 (1.88)108.97 (2.08)−16.80<0.001−1.29
 Verbal Ability97.17 (1.97)107.12 (2.21)−9.950.002−0.72
 Nonverbal Ability95.63 (2.02)108.29 (2.22)−12.66<0.001−0.92
 Spatial Ability90.08 (1.88)106.03 (2.05)−15.95<0.001−1.23
Executive function and processing speed:
 DCCS Age Corrected 94.53 (2.15)100.05 (2.10)−5.520.078−0.42
 DAS-II Recall of Digits-Forward43.94 (1.77)56.75 (1.98)−12.81<0.001−1.22
 Go No Go0.62 (0.03)0.75 (0.03)−0.130.003−0.70
 CTOPP−2 Rapid Symbolic
Naming a
96.15 (2.80)105.67 (2.77)−16.680.031−0.57
Motor skills (MABC−2):
 Total5.52 (0.42)8.91 (0.45)−3.39<0.001−1.18
 Manual Dexterity4.90 (0.42)8.29 (0.47)−3.40<0.001−1.16
 Balance6.92 (0.40)9.61 (0.46)−2.69<0.001−0.96
 Aiming and Catching b8.59 (0.43)10.46 (0.51)1.930.043−0.79
Early literacy and numeracy:
 TOPEL Print Knowledge96.80 (2.22)107.26 (2.39)−10.460.003−0.69
 TOPEL Phonological Awareness92.37 (2.08)106.53 (2.31)−14.16<0.001−1.00
 DAS-II Early Number Concepts45.29 (1.27)55.89 (1.49)−10.60<0.001−1.31
Behavior ratings:
 Conners EC-P Global Index
Total
56.23 (1.69)55.23 (1.88)1.000.7020.09
 Conners EC-P Inattention/Hyperactivity55.81 (1.72)56.42 (1.97)−0.610.822−0.05
 Conners EC-P Anxiety56.88 (1.74)55.46 (1.93)1.420.5980.13
 Conners EC-P Social
Functioning c
53.73 (1.29)47.68 (1.48)−12.610.0010.64
 BRIEF-P GEC58.55 (1.66)51.31 (1.91)7.240.0070.61
 BRIEF-P Inhibitory Self-Control Index56.60 (1.76)51.89 (2.00)4.710.0920.38
 BRIEF-P Flexibility Index54.67 (1.53)50.04 (1.73)4.630.0560.43
 BRIEF-P Emergent Metacognition Index60.18 (1.59)51.42 (1.91)8.770.0010.76
 ERC Lability/Negativity27.74 (0.95)26.58 (1.10)1.150.4470.17
 ERC Emotion Regulation d26.31 (0.40)28.39 (0.48)3.570.003−0.60
Note. VPT = very preterm; FT = full term; M (SE) = mean (standard error); DAS-II = Differential Ability Scales, 2nd Edition; DCCS = Dimensional Change Card Sort; CTOPP-2 = Children’s Test of Phonological Processing, 2nd Edition; MABC-2 = Movement Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd Edition; TOPEL = Test of Preschool Early Literacy; Conners EC-P = Conners Early Childhood Parent rating; BRIEF-P = Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool version; GEC = Global Executive Composite; ERC = Emotion Regulation Checklist. a Group × sex interaction significant (p = 0.047); significantly lower scores on CTOPP-2 Rapid Symbolic Naming for VPT group compared to FT group only among males (p = 0.003, EF = −1.12). b Group × zSES interaction significant (p = 0.050); more pronounced weakness on MABC-2 Aiming and Catching for VPT group relative to FT group at lower levels of zSES. c Group × sex interaction significant (p = 0.001); significantly higher ratings of problems in social functioning for VPT group relative to FT group only among females (p < 0.001, EF = 1.38). d Group × zSES interaction significant (p = 0.004); significantly lower ratings of self-regulation for VPT group relative to FT group only among females (p < 0.001, EF = −1.42).
Table 4. Group differences in rates of deficits in school readiness.
Table 4. Group differences in rates of deficits in school readiness.
VPT Group
(n = 53)
FT Group
(n = 38)
Area of DeficitN (%)N (%)pOR (95% CI)
Global/cognitive ability (DAS-II):
 General Conceptual Ability16 (31)0 (0)0.01731.85 (1.85, 548.40)
 Verbal Ability9 (18)0 (0)0.03720.84 (1.21, 358.86)
 Nonverbal Ability10 (20)1 (3)0.02110.96 (1.43, 83.82)
 Spatial Ability20 (42)1 (3)0.01414.10 (1.73, 115.11)
Executive function and processing speed:
 DCCS Age Corrected11 (26)3 (8)0.0405.00 (1.07, 23.29)
 DAS-II Recall of Digits, Forward14 (28)1 (3)0.00715.43 (2.09, 113.90)
 Go No Go16 (37)2 (6)0.0136.67 (1.49, 29.86)
 CTOPP-2 Rapid Symbolic Naming6 (18)3 (9)0.6541.39 (0.33, 5.91)
Motor skills (MABC-2):
 Total33 (62)6 (16)<0.0011.61 (1.31, 1.98)
 Manual Dexterity39 (74)9 (24)<0.0011.58 (1.27, 1.97)
 Balance18 (34)6 (16)0.0501.22 (1.00, 1.48)
 Aiming and Catching17 (32)2 (5)0.0031.25 (1.08, 1.45)
Early literacy and numeracy:
 TOPEL Print Knowledge14 (28)1 (3)0.04310.08 (1.08, 94.43)
 TOPEL Phonological Awareness13 (26)0 (0)0.03122.87 (1.33, 394.68)
 DAS-II Early Number Concepts12 (24)0 (0)0.03920.00 (1.17, 342.69)
Behavior problems:
 Conners EC-P Global Index
Total
16 (30)8 (21)0.8880.92 (0.28, 3.04)
 Conners EC-P
Inattention/Hyperactivity
20 (38)10 (26)0.6661.25 (0.46, 3.38)
 Conners EC-P Anxiety19 (36)9 (24)0.4421.53 (0.52, 4.47)
 Conners EC-P Social
Functioning
17 (32)1 (3)0.03314.77 (1.24, 176.29)
 BRIEF-P GEC28 (53)4 (11)0.0016.90 (2.10, 22.72)
 BRIEF-P Inhibitory Self-Control Index25 (47)6 (16)0.0393.34 (1.07, 10.48)
 BRIEF-P Flexibility Index18 (34)4 (11)0.1462.63 (0.71, 9.69)
 BRIEF-P Emergent Metacognition Index30 (57)3 (8)<0.00112.81 (3.70, 44.33)
 ERC Lability/Negativity12 (23)5 (13)0.5771.45 (0.39, 5.42)
 ERC Emotion Regulation13 (25)1 (3)0.0021.22 (1.08, 1.39)
Multiple Deficits:44 (83)12 (32)0.0011.42 (1.15, 1.76)
Note. VPT = very preterm; FT = full term; OR (95% CI) = odds ratio (95% confidence interval); DAS-II = Differential Ability Scales, 2nd Edition; DCCS = Dimensional Change Card Sort; CTOPP-2 = Children’s Test of Phonological Processing, 2nd Edition; MABC-2 = Movement Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd Edition; TOPEL = Test of Preschool Early Literacy; Conners EC-P = Conners Early Childhood Parent version; BRIEF-P = Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool version; GEC = Global Executive Composite; ERC = Emotion Regulation Checklist.
Table 5. Model fit statistics for latent class analysis (LCA).
Table 5. Model fit statistics for latent class analysis (LCA).
Fit StatisticsClass 1Class 2Class 3Class 4Class 5
AIC8224.1097972.7237890.2937843.9287807.723
BIC8298.2688086.288043.2488036.288039.472
SSA-BIC8197.4137931.8457835.2337774.6867724.298
Entropy-0.9440.9630.9660.958
LMR-285.385116.4380.36565.277
p-value (LMR)-0.010.270.570.77
BLRT-285.385116.4380.36565.277
p-value (BLRT)-<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001
Note. AIC = Akaike information criteria; BIC = Bayesian information criteria; SSA-BIC = Sample size adjusted BIC; LMR = Lo–Mendell–Rubin test; BLRT = bootstrap likelihood ratio test. Lower values on AIC, AIC, and adjusted BIC indicate better model fit. A significant p-value on the Vuong–Lo–Mendell–Rubin test, Lo–Mendel–Rubin test, or bootstrap likelihood ratio test indicate a better fit for the less parsimonious model.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Taylor, H.G.; Vrantsidis, D.M.; Neel, M.L.; Benkart, R.; Busch, T.A.; de Silva, A.; Udaipuria, S.; Maitre, N.L. School Readiness in 4-Year-Old Very Preterm Children. Children 2022, 9, 323. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9030323

AMA Style

Taylor HG, Vrantsidis DM, Neel ML, Benkart R, Busch TA, de Silva A, Udaipuria S, Maitre NL. School Readiness in 4-Year-Old Very Preterm Children. Children. 2022; 9(3):323. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9030323

Chicago/Turabian Style

Taylor, H. Gerry, Daphne M. Vrantsidis, Mary Lauren Neel, Rebekah Benkart, Tyler A. Busch, Aryanne de Silva, Shivika Udaipuria, and Nathalie L. Maitre. 2022. "School Readiness in 4-Year-Old Very Preterm Children" Children 9, no. 3: 323. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9030323

APA Style

Taylor, H. G., Vrantsidis, D. M., Neel, M. L., Benkart, R., Busch, T. A., de Silva, A., Udaipuria, S., & Maitre, N. L. (2022). School Readiness in 4-Year-Old Very Preterm Children. Children, 9(3), 323. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9030323

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop