Next Article in Journal
Ultrasonographic Assessment for Tenosynovitis in Juvenile Idiopathic Arthritis with Ankle Involvement: Diagnostic and Therapeutic Significance
Next Article in Special Issue
Monitoring of Communication Precursors in Extremely Low Birth Weight (ELBW) Newborns by Video Analysis Method: Preliminary Results
Previous Article in Journal
Executive Function Improvement for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Comparative Study between Virtual Training and Physical Exercise Methods
Previous Article in Special Issue
School Readiness in 4-Year-Old Very Preterm Children
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Early Childhood Temperamental Trajectories following Very Preterm Birth and Their Association with Parenting Style

1
Centre for the Developing Brain, School of Biomedical Engineering & Imaging Sciences, King’s College London, London SE1 7EH, UK
2
Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padova, 35151 Padova, Italy
3
Department of Neuroimaging, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, UK
4
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, UK
5
National Perinatal Epidemiology Unit, University of Oxford, Oxford OX3 7LF, UK
6
Neonatal Unit, St George’s Hospital, London SW17 0QT, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Children 2022, 9(4), 508; https://doi.org/10.3390/children9040508
Submission received: 21 January 2022 / Revised: 21 March 2022 / Accepted: 1 April 2022 / Published: 3 April 2022

Abstract

:
Childhood temperament is an early characteristic shaping later life adjustment. However, little is currently known about the stability of early temperament and its susceptibility to the environment in children born very preterm (VPT; <33 weeks’ gestation). Here, we investigated infant-to-childhood temperamental trajectories, and their interaction with parental practices, in VPT children. Maternal reports of infant temperament were collected in 190 infants (mean age: 11.27 months; range 9–18 months) enrolled in the longitudinal Evaluation of Preterm Imaging (ePrime; Eudra: CT 2009-011602-42) study, using the ePrime questionnaire on infant temperament. At 4–7 years of age, further assessments of child temperament (Children’s Behavior Questionnaire—Very Short Form) and parenting style (Arnold’s Parenting Scale) were conducted. Results showed that more difficult temperament in infancy was associated with increased Negative Affectivity in childhood, regardless of parenting practices. This lends support to the stability of early temperamental traits reflecting negative emotionality. In contrast, a lax parenting style moderated the relationship between easy infant temperament and Negative Affectivity at 4–7 years, such that an easier infant temperament was increasingly associated with higher childhood Negative Affectivity scores as parental laxness increased. These results highlight a potential vulnerability of VPT infants considered by their mothers to be easy to handle, as they may be more susceptible to the effects of suboptimal parenting in childhood.

1. Introduction

Very preterm birth (<33 weeks’ gestation) represents approximately 15% of all preterm births (<37 weeks’ gestation), which are currently estimated by the World Health Organization to constitute between 5% and 18% of all births [1,2]. Therefore, attention has increasingly focused on the quality of life of survivors, who are at high risk of developing a range of adverse short- and long-term sequelae [3]. The neurodevelopmental outcomes associated with preterm birth have been summarised into a “preterm behavioural phenotype” characterised by an increased risk of exhibiting inattention, anxiety and socio-emotional difficulties [4].
The identification of specific vulnerabilities associated with preterm birth have stimulated a growing interest in finding early markers that could be used to predict neurodevelopmental outcomes. Temperament is an important aspect of early development, shaping indicators of later life adjustment, including mental health and school adaptation (for a review, see [5]). In a generally accepted summary of several conceptualisations of temperament [6], this refers to “relatively consistent, basic dispositions inherent in the person that underlie and modulate the expression of activity, reactivity, emotionality and sociability”. These biologically based dispositions appear in infancy, and are considered to be relatively stable over time, both when assessed via maternal reports or laboratory measures [7,8,9,10,11]. In preterm individuals, temperamental traits in middle childhood have been shown to be predictive of later behavioural outcomes [12]. However, only a few studies to date have investigated whether temperament in infancy is a stable predictor of childhood temperament in those born preterm, and as such may constitute a useful early indicator of behavioural risk. With an increasing interest in early detection and intervention in developmental psychopathology [13,14], understanding the development of temperament from infancy to childhood following preterm birth may shed light on early mechanisms shaping later outcomes in this population.
Despite its relative stability, children’s temperament is also known to interact in a complex way with environmental and parental factors, with children shaping their development through cascades of individual–context bidirectional effects [15,16,17]. In particular, childhood temperament has been associated with parents’ health and parenting style. For example, negative emotionality has been linked to a strict and controlling maternal discipline, and to an authoritarian parenting style [18,19,20,21]. Similarly, high levels of activity have been associated with maternal strictness [18], but also with a parenting style characterised by authority, warmth and clear rules [21], possibly reflecting parental acceptance of higher activity levels in their children. Finally, children’s inhibition and shyness have been related to maternal overprotectiveness [22,23], although the direction of the relationship remains to be ascertained. Mothers may tend to display overprotective responses to children’s shyness, fear and inhibition [24], or, alternatively, overprotective mothers may be less encouraging of their children’s independence, which in turn may exacerbate feelings of social anxiety [22]. It has been suggested that children’s inhibited reactions and mothers’ overprotective behaviours may reinforce one another over time in an “anxious-coercive cycle” that could lead to the development of anxiety problems [25]. Nevertheless, difficult temperamental characteristics have also been associated with a positive parental style, with parents making positive efforts, as well as providing more contact and stimulation, with their irritable and demanding children [26]. Furthermore, specific temperamental traits could elicit different parenting styles in different parents, and parenting might influence a child’s temperament both via effects on early brain development or via a mechanism of parental modelling of emotional and social behaviours [27].
Child temperament and parenting are thought to interact in shaping child development [23,28,29,30,31,32]. In general, children with a more difficult temperament are more vulnerable to the effects of poor-quality parenting, but also benefit more from positive parenting, in line with the theory of differential susceptibility [33]. These differences in susceptibility can manifest in several aspects of negative childhood adjustment, including externalising and internalising problems, as well as poorer social and cognitive competence [34].
The literature suggests that temperament may differ between preterm (predominantly very preterm and extremely preterm; <28 weeks’ gestation) children and term-born controls, especially regarding the level of activity and self-regulation (for a review, see [35]). In particular, preterm children show a higher activity level, as well as lower attention span and focus, compared with their term-born peers [35]. Both these temperamental traits may be early indicators of behavioural difficulties [12], underlying preterm children’s higher risk of inattention and externalising behaviour problems [36,37]. Preterm children’s temperament development may be affected by maternal prenatal stress, which has been associated with alterations in hormones and neurotransmitters in utero [38], influencing infants’ brain development [39], as well as early emotional responses and reactions to stimuli [40]. We have in fact previously found associations between structural and functional properties of limbic brain regions in the neonatal period and the capacity to regulate emotions later in childhood in very preterm individuals [41,42]. Furthermore, interactions between preterm and full-term child–parent dyads are thought to differ, as very and extremely preterm children show poorer emotional and behavioural regulation, and their parents exhibit more difficulties in being responsive and sensitive to their children’s needs. [43,44]. Understanding temperament-by-parenting interactions on behaviour in preterm samples has therefore been of particular interest. Clark and colleagues [43] observed that extremely preterm children aged 2–4 years had poorer self-regulation compared to term-born controls and that, in the extremely preterm group, a less sensitive parenting style was one of the strongest predictors of self-regulation impairment. Poehlmann et al. [45] found that preterm low-birth-weight infants (<2500 g at birth) were more prone to distress and exhibited high levels of externalising behaviours at 24 months only if they experienced angry and critical parenting at 9 months. Proneness to distress in infancy was also found to be a risk factor for internalising problems at 24 months, but only when accompanied by an intrusive and anxious parenting style. Furthermore, more difficult infants displayed lower Effortful Control at 24 months if they experienced a parenting style characterised by anxiety and intrusiveness [45]. Lastly, Whiteside-Mansell and colleagues [46] reported that preterm low-birth-weight infants with difficult temperament were at increased risk of developing externalising problems when exposed to family conflicts.
To date, there is little research using a longitudinal design to examine whether very early markers of temperament are predictive of later temperamental outcomes following very preterm birth, and to study how these trajectories may be influenced by parenting style. In this study, we therefore investigated whether maternal perceptions of infant temperament were associated with childhood temperament assessed between 4 and 7 years of age, in a sample of very preterm children. We used a simple conceptualisation of infant temperament, defined as being either “easy or “difficult” [11], where an easy infant is responsive and content, while a difficult infant is demanding and tough to deal with. Childhood temperament was captured in terms of standardised measures of Negative Affectivity (overall negative outlook of oneself and the surrounding world), Surgency (a disposition towards impulsivity, intense pleasure seeking and high activity levels) and Effortful Control (a child’s capacity to focus and shift attention, intentionally inhibit a response and respond to low-intensity stimulation and reward) [47]. Furthermore, we explored whether suboptimal parenting influenced temperamental trajectories from infancy to childhood. We hypothesised that (a) early maternal perceptions of infant temperament would be predictive of temperamental traits during childhood, as temperament itself is defined as relatively stable, and (b) infants with a difficult temperament would be more vulnerable to the effects of suboptimal parenting than infants with an easy temperament.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design

A longitudinal study was conducted to evaluate stability of maternal perception of temperamental dimensions between infancy and childhood, and the influence of suboptimal parenting style on these trajectories. Mothers’ reports of child’s temperament were collected when children were 9–18 months and 4–7 years, while parenting style was assessed with maternal self-report when children were 4–7 years.

2.2. Sample

Study participants were 511 very preterm-born infants recruited at birth into the Evaluation of Preterm Imaging study (ePrime, Eudra: CT 2009-011602-42) between April 2010 and July 2013 from hospitals in the North and South-West London Perinatal Network [48]. Eligibility criteria were birth before 33 weeks’ gestation, mothers being over 16 years and not hospital inpatients. Exclusion criteria included major congenital malformation and parents being unable to speak English or subject to child protection proceedings. Of the ePrime cohort, a convenience sample of 251 children (corresponding to 82% of 306 who were eligible and invited to participate) underwent a neurodevelopmental follow-up assessment between the ages of 4 and 7 years (67% age 4, 17% age 5, 13% age 6 and 3% age 7). Written informed consent was given by the children’s carer(s), following protocols approved by Stanmore Research Ethics Committee (14/LO/0677). This study was conducted in line with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).
Here, we report results from 190 participants with complete temperamental data in infancy and at 4–7 years follow-up. Data for both child temperament and parenting style were available for 101 subjects.

2.3. Socio-Demographic Data

An Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) score was derived from parental postcode at the time of infant birth (Department for Communities and Local Government 2011; https://tools.npeu.ox.ac.uk/imd/ (accessed on 3 March 2015)). The IMD measures social risk by comparing seven domains of deprivation within each neighbourhood: income, employment, education skills and training, health and disability, barriers to housing and services, living environment and crime. A higher IMD value indicates higher deprivation.

2.4. Assessment of Infant Temperament

Child’s temperament was assessed in infancy (mean age = 11.27 months, range = 9–18.4 months) via a qualitative maternal questionnaire (ePrime questionnaire on infant temperament). The questionnaire is based on an adjective checklist originally designed in order to assess parents’ perception of their infant while in the neonatal unit [49] and in the months thereafter [50,51], consisting of both positive and negative terms.
The ePrime questionnaire on infant temperament requires the parent to circle as many adjectives as possible from a list that they believe best describe their baby at time of assessment. While this type of checklist has been previously used to assess the proportion of positive and negative adjectives parents use to describe their infant [49,50,51], here we used it to derive scores for “easy” and “difficult” temperament by evaluating only adjectives which incorporate aspects of the infant’s activity level or behaviour (such as “settled” or “agitated”), but not purely descriptive adjectives (such as “beautiful” or “small”). Thus, the “easy” temperament category includes adjectives such as settled, cuddly, active and responsive, while the “difficult” temperament category represents infants that are demanding or tough to deal with (for example being agitated or, alternatively, unresponsive and inactive).
A full description of the ePrime questionnaire on infant temperament can be found in Appendix A.

2.5. Follow-Up Assessment at 4–7 Years

2.5.1. Children’s Behavior Questionnaire—Very Short Form

The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire—Very Short Form (CBQ-VSF) is a 36-item parent-rated questionnaire assessing temperament, which is validated for children aged 3–8 [47]. The CBQ is a widely used tool in preterm research [35], and it is based on Rothbart’s well-accepted psychobiological theory of temperament, which defined temperament as relatively stable and biologically based individual differences in levels of reactivity and self-regulation [52]. In this questionnaire, parents are asked to describe their children in situations occurring in everyday life using a 7-point scale from 1 (extremely untrue of your child) to 7 (extremely true of your child); higher scores therefore indicate higher levels of a temperamental trait. The CBQ-VSF provides scores for three temperamental traits: Surgency, Negative Affectivity and Effortful Control. Surgency is characterised by a disposition toward positive emotions, high activity level and a rapid approach to potential rewards [53]. Negative Affectivity is characterised by shyness, discomfort, anger–frustration, fear, sadness and un-soothability [9]. Effortful Control encompasses voluntary attentional focusing, attentional shifting, inhibitory and activational control of behaviours [52]. All CBQ-VSF subscales have been shown to exhibit adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.66) [47].

2.5.2. Parenting Scale

The Parenting Scale is a 30-item rating scale that measures dysfunctional parenting in discipline situations [54]. Parents are asked to indicate their tendency to use specific discipline practices using a 7-point scale, with dysfunctional parenting style at one end, and supporting parenting style at the other. An example is “When my child misbehaves, I raise my voice or yell; or I speak to my child calmly”. Three different suboptimal parenting styles are evaluated, as well a global index of dysfunctional parenting. Higher scores indicate a poorer disciplinary parenting style. Overreactivity indicates authoritarian and coercive discipline practices. Laxness, in contrast, describes a permissive parent who is inconsistent in providing discipline. Finally, Verbosity refers to a parenting style characterised by lengthy and ineffective verbal reprimands. All subscales of the Parenting Scale have been shown to exhibit adequate internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.63) [54].

2.6. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using R [55] and RStudio [56]. Throughout, we chose a multivariate regression approach (whereby childhood temperament variables were initially included simultaneously as dependent variables, to be predicted by infant temperament, parenting and interactions thereof). This analysis tests the null hypothesis that the coefficient for each predictor is equal to zero across all dependent variables. This method allowed for a principled approach for choosing suitable follow-up analyses, thus reducing the overall number of tests in the context of a large number of possible variable combinations and thereby accounting for likely error inflation due to multiple testing [57,58].
Firstly, a multivariate regression analysis was run in order to explore the relationship between infant and childhood maternal perception of temperament. The three subscales of the CBQ were simultaneously included as dependent variables, and the two infant temperament variables (easy and difficult) were included as predictors in the same model. Non-collinearity of these variables was evaluated using correlation analyses; indeed, there was no significant relationship between easy and difficult infant temperament (r = −0.067, df = 188, p = 0.356). The analysis was performed including the following covariates: sex, gestational age (GA) at birth, IMD score and age at follow-up assessment. Following a significant effect of either easy or difficult temperament (at a significance threshold of p = 0.05), individual follow-up univariate regression models for each CBQ subscale (Negative Affectivity, Surgency and Effortful Control) were conducted to elucidate the nature and direction of the association. These follow-up tests were corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction for three separate models (p = 0.05/3 = 0.017).
To test whether adding parenting style to the original model predicting temperamental outcomes at 4–7 years significantly improved model prediction, multivariate model comparisons for each Parenting Scale subscale (Laxness, Overreactivity and Verbosity) were performed. For each parenting subscale, a multivariate null model (M0), predicting childhood temperament from easy and difficult infant temperament as described above (controlling for sex, GA at birth, IMD score and age at follow-up), was separately tested against two more complex models (M1 and M2) using likelihood ratio F-tests. M1 additionally included the respective parenting subscale and its interaction with easy temperament, while M2 included the parenting subscale and its interaction with difficult temperament. The significance threshold was adjusted using Bonferroni correction for 6 separate model comparisons (one for each parenting variable and separately for its interaction with easy or difficult temperament), resulting in a threshold of p = 0.05/6 = 0.008.
Where either M1 or M2 was shown to explain significantly more variance compared to M0, follow-up univariate tests for each CBQ variable were performed. These tests were further Bonferroni-corrected using a significance threshold of 0.008/3 = 0.003. In the case of a significant interaction between continuous variables, these were further elucidated using simple slope analyses (evaluated at ±1 SD and ±1.5 SD from the mean).
Analysis code is available at https://github.com/lucyvanes/preterm-temperament.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

Socio-demographic characteristics of both samples with complete temperamental data and subsamples with additional parenting data are shown in Table 1. Subsample participants with parenting data (n = 101) did not differ significantly from participants with temperamental but without parenting data (n = 89) on any key maternal or child characteristics listed in Table 1 (as ascertained via t-tests for continuous and chi-squared tests for categorical variables). One exception was that participants excluded from the parenting analysis (i.e., without parenting data) at 4–7 years follow-up assessment were slightly older than those included (mean = 4.5 vs. 5.4, t(188) = 10.53, p < 0.001).

3.2. Association between Infant and Childhood Temperament

A multivariate regression analysis including the CBQ subscales (Negative Affectivity, Surgency and Effortful Control) as dependent variables, and easy and difficult temperaments as predictors (controlling for sex, GA at birth, IMD score and age at follow-up assessment) showed a significant relationship between a difficult infant temperament and childhood temperament (F(3,181) = 4.5, p = 0.004), and between an easy infant temperament and childhood temperament (F(3,181) = 3.1, p = 0.027) (see Table 2 for full multivariate model output).
Follow-up individual linear regressions for each CBQ subscale (see Table 3; Bonferroni-corrected for three separate models; p-threshold = 0.05/03 = 0.017) revealed that a difficult infant temperament was positively associated with childhood Negative Affectivity (β = 0.15, SE = 0.05, p = 0.002), as seen in Figure 1. The association between an easy infant temperament and childhood Negative Affectivity was not significant after Bonferroni correction (β = −0.06, SE = 0.03, p = 0.031). Neither easy or difficult infant temperaments were significantly associated with childhood Surgency or Effortful Control, all ps > 0.017.

3.3. Moderating Effect of Parenting Style on the Association between Infant and Childhood Temperament

Two different multivariate model comparisons for each Parenting Scale’s subscale (Laxness, Overreactivity and Verbosity) were performed, to separately test if the model including (a) parenting and its interaction with easy temperament, or (b) parenting and its interaction with difficult temperament, significantly explained more variance than the original model predicting childhood temperamental outcomes. Results show that only the multivariate model including the interaction between an easy infant temperament and Laxness was significantly more predictive than the null model excluding parenting variables (F(6,182) = 3.54, p = 0.002) (passing Bonferroni correction for six models at a threshold of p = 0.008). No significant associations were found for models which tested the interaction between difficult infant temperament and any parenting measure, and easy infant temperament and Verbosity or Overreactivity.
Follow-up tests were conducted only for the winning model (see Table 4 and Table 5), specifically investigating the effect of Laxness on the trajectory between easy infant temperament and childhood temperament (Negative Affectivity, Surgency and Effortful Control). Results showed that the multivariate effect was due to a significant effect on childhood Negative Affectivity, but not on Surgency or Effortful Control. There was a significant main effect of Laxness (β = −1.08, SE = 0.28, p < 0.001), showing that higher levels of parental Laxness were associated with less childhood Negative Affectivity. There was also a significant main effect of easy infant temperament on Negative Affectivity (β = −0.72, SE = 0.17, p < 0.001), indicating that an easier infant temperament was related to reduced childhood Negative Affectivity. However, this relationship was moderated by parental Laxness, as indicated by a significant interaction between easy infant temperament and Laxness (β = 0.2, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001). As shown in Figure 2, the association between easy infant temperament and childhood Negative Affectivity becomes more positive with increasing Laxness, such that children of parents who rated themselves as laxer showed a positive association between an easy infant temperament and childhood Negative Affectivity. Conversely, for children whose parents considered themselves as less lax, a higher easy infant temperament was associated with a lower level of childhood Negative Affectivity. Simple slope analyses revealed that the effect of an easy infant temperament on childhood Negative Affectivity was significantly negative for low parental laxness both at −1.5 SD (β = −0.29, p < 0.001) and −1 SD (β = −0.21, p < 0.001) from the mean. For high parental laxness, the association between easy infant temperament and childhood Negative Affectivity was non-significantly positive at +1 SD (β = 0.09, p = 0.085) and significantly positive at +1.5 SD (β = 0.16, p = 0.014) from the mean.

4. Discussion

This study aimed to investigate the association between maternal perceptions of infant and childhood temperament and the potential intervening role of parenting styles on these trajectories. Results show that maternal reports of difficult infant temperament are predictive of the childhood temperament dimension of Negative Affectivity at 4–7 years. Furthermore, easy infant temperament interacts with parental laxness in predicting Negative Affectivity in childhood. Results referring to temperamental predictability and the influence of parenting style will be discussed separately.
In line with our hypothesis of an association between infant and childhood temperament, difficult infant temperament showed a significant positive association with childhood Negative Affectivity. This association is in line with the literature on temperamental stability over time in samples of term-born children. For example, Guerin and Gottfried [59] reported that parents maintain a stable perception of their children’s temperament, such that a difficult temperament in infancy predicts negative mood and slow adaptability during childhood. Similar results were discussed by Rothbart et al. [60], who found that laboratory measures of irritability and frustration at 10 months predicted anger, frustration, discomfort, aggression, risk-taking pleasure and guilt–shame at 7 years. This consistency in adverse temperamental qualities suggests that “difficult children” have an early vulnerability for developing and maintaining later adverse behavioural outcomes such as externalising and internalising problems [5,61,62,63,64,65]. Although only a few studies to date have investigated these questions in preterm children, Gracioli and Linhares [66] reported early childhood frustration as one of the main predictors for both externalising and internalising behaviour problems in preterm children. Given the association between difficult temperamental traits in infancy and childhood in our sample, one might speculate that an early difficult temperament contributes to the emergence of the “preterm behavioural phenotype”. Alternatively, it may be an indicator of underlying brain alterations implicated in the emotional development of preterm children [41]. This highlights the importance of considering temperamental traits early in development, as well as the need for further studies exploring the biological substrates of a difficult temperament in preterm children. However, as our study did not have a control group, we cannot exclude the possibility that the effects observed here are not unique to the preterm population. Indeed, some studies have shown that preterm children did not differ from full-term children in terms of difficult temperament, or Negative Affectivity (for a review, [35]). It is therefore possible that difficult temperamental traits follow a similar relatively stable developmental trajectory in both preterm and full-term children.
Notably, an association between easy infant temperament and childhood temperament traits emerged only when taking levels of parental laxness into account. Contrary to our expectation, this temperament-by-parenting interaction was not in line with our hypothesis, according to which children manifesting high levels of negative emotionality would be more vulnerable to negative environmental influences. Rather, in our sample, those children considered to be easier and more manageable by their mothers appeared more vulnerable to parental influences. Specifically, for very preterm children exposed to lower levels of parental laxness, an easy infant temperament and childhood Negative Affectivity were negatively related, such that easier infants expressed less Negative Affectivity in childhood. Conversely, the association was reversed for children whose mothers self-reported themselves as being laxer, such that an easier infant temperament was increasingly associated with higher childhood Negative Affectivity scores as parental laxness increased.
Laxness reflects a disciplinary style characterised by inconsistent reprimands and permissiveness [54], and lax parenting has been found to be related to oppositional behaviour and conduct disorders, as well as to internalising difficulties [67,68,69]. Our results suggest that, for easy infants who are raised in a non-dysfunctional disciplinary context (i.e., low parental laxness), the easy temperamental trajectory follows a positive path characterised by low levels of negative emotionality. This is in line with reports that smiling, laughter and rapid approaches to objects in infancy predict 6–7-year-old approach tendencies and a positive effect [60]. Easy temperament is also related to fewer subsequent negative developmental outcomes and to more adaptive social, cognitive and personality development [70,71]. Non-dysfunctional parenting, as expressed by low laxness, may have a stabilising effect on very preterm infants who display an easy temperament early on, maintaining an adaptive temperamental trajectory that fosters emotion regulation and thus protects them from experiencing high levels of Negative Affectivity. In contrast, lax parenting may contribute to variability in Negative Affectivity in preterm children who were considered as easy in infancy. Overall, this inconsistent and permissive parenting style seems to have a beneficial effect on infants regarded as less easy by mothers but a detrimental effect on those rated as easier.
This counterintuitive relationship can be framed within the concept of “goodness of fit” [72], which posits that it is the interaction between the properties of the environment and the child’s characteristics and style of behaving that determines the course of development. Thus, a more structured, less permissive parenting style may be better suited to easy preterm infants who might be easier to discipline. In contrast, less easy preterm infants may benefit emotionally from a moderately challenging environment characterised by a more permissive parenting style. This is in line with previous findings that high levels of positive parenting can exacerbate inhibition in infants with high negative emotionality [28,32,73]. However, the counterintuitive association between suboptimal parenting and lower Negative Affectivity in less easy infants could also reflect a temporary aspect of a more complex and non-linear developmental trajectory in these children, which may ultimately increase socio-emotional risk. Further research with longer follow-up periods will be necessary in order to characterise the long-term trajectories of these children.
The fact that easy and difficult infant temperaments differ not only in their relationship with childhood temperament but also in their interaction with parenting is of particular interest. Here, it is important to note that maternal perception of easy temperament may be reflective of a wide range of characteristics, from resilience and positive emotionality to withdrawal, passiveness or slower development. Further investigations into the meaning of easy temperament following preterm birth are therefore warranted. However, our results converge with previous findings that highly manageable toddlers are the ones most influenced by maternal control and by non-parental day-care quality [74,75]. In contrast, it has been suggested that a difficult temperament may not constitute vulnerability per se, but rather a factor contributing to non-malleability in children [75], which aligns with our observation of relative stability of this temperamental trait. However, the construct of “difficult” temperament is debated as it may encourage an oversimplified view of temperament [8], although it may nevertheless be a useful construct in a clinical setting as it is easily understandable and recognisable by mothers. Indeed, infant temperament data of this study came from the ePrime questionnaire on infant temperament, a qualitative questionnaire developed for use in a clinical research context. Our findings reporting associations between infant and childhood temperamental traits support the view that even a very simple and rapid questionnaire on maternal perceptions of their infants’ characteristics used as a screening tool in a hospital setting can give a good indication of later temperamental outcomes.
Findings of this study should be interpreted considering the following limitations. First, due to the lack of a control group, we were unable to compare our sample of very preterm children to their full-term peers, and findings may therefore not be specific to the very preterm population. Furthermore, as this study used only maternal reports of both temperament and parenting style, scores might not accurately reflect the intended characteristics and may be affected by respondent bias. Nevertheless, parental reports of childhood temperament have been shown to be very reliable [76,77]. A further limitation is that parental characteristics were measured only at 4–7 years, and we can therefore only speculate that parenting style was stable between infancy and childhood. It is also possible that infants’ temperamental qualities influenced parenting style over early childhood [17]. Moreover, other parental attributes not captured in our study, such as parental temperament, are also likely to exert an effect on childhood temperament [78], which can be usefully addressed in future research.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, our study adds to previous investigations on longitudinal temperamental trajectories and parental influences on these trajectories in preterm samples. Our results suggest that in very preterm children, specific attention should be paid to infants considered as difficult by their mothers, as difficult traits seem to be stable over childhood, thus constituting a risk factor that potentially adds to their specific cognitive and behavioural vulnerabilities. Attention should also be paid to parenting styles in children considered easy to deal with by their mothers, as they may be more vulnerable to suboptimal parenting styles. Specifically, although in a non-dysfunctional parental environment, easy temperament is predictive of lower levels of negative emotionality over time, the presence of a lax parenting style may reverse this association and contribute to an increase in childhood Negative Affectivity. As parental knowledge and self-efficacy are associated with reduced dysfunctional parenting [79], interventions designed to increase parental awareness and skills may be a useful tool for parents of both difficult and easy preterm infants [80]. Future studies should continue to explore temperamental trajectories in the preterm population, comparing preterm and full-term samples and using a multimethod approach, with the aim of early identification and implementation of parent–child interventions in individuals deemed most vulnerable.

Author Contributions

I.L. and L.D.V. contributed equally to this article and should be considered joint first authors. Conceptualisation, A.D.E., I.L., C.N. and L.D.V.; Methodology, L.H., D.K., I.L. and L.D.V.; Formal Analysis, I.L. and L.D.V.; Investigation, S.F.; Data Curation, S.F.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, I.L., C.N. and L.D.V.; Writing—Review and Editing, I.L., L.D.V., C.N., S.C., A.D.E., S.F., L.H., D.K., N.K., M.R., C.S. and A.S.; Supervision, C.N., C.S. and A.S.; Funding Acquisition, A.D.E., S.C., N.K., C.N. and M.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Medical Research Council (UK) (grant nos: MR/S026460/1, MR/K006355/1 and MR/L011530/1), Action Medical Research and Dangoor Education (grant no: GN2606) and uses data acquired during independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Programme Grants for Applied Research Programme (RP-PG-0707-10154). The research was supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre based at Guy’s and St Thomas’ NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London, by the NIHR Clinical Research Facility (CRF) at Guy’s and St Thomas’ and by the MRC Centre for Neurodevelopmental Disorders. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was approved by the Stanmore Research Ethics Committee (14/LO/0677) on 29 May 2014 and was conducted in line with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data and analysis code are available at https://github.com/lucyvanes/preterm-temperament.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A. Description of the ePrime Questionnaire on Infant Temperament

The ePrime questionnaire on infant temperament is part of a larger assessment aimed at monitoring the caregivers’ experience of neonatal care and their child’s early development, and the way in which they are provided with information, as part of the ePrime study (Evaluation of MR imaging to predict long-term outcomes in preterm infants). It is based on an adjective checklist designed to assess parents’ perception of their infant in neonatal care [49] and in the following months [50,51].
The ePrime questionnaire on infant temperament provides the parent with a checklist of adjectives (settled, agitated, beautiful, fragile, large, inactive, weak, active, demanding, attractive, not cuddly, responsive, cuddly, small, alert, difficult, easy, ugly, unresponsive, tough, unattractive, strong, sleepy and contented), asking them to circle as many terms that they believe best describe their baby at time of assessment. A subset of these adjectives was grouped into two subcategories of temperamental style—“easy” and “difficult”—based on the behavioural temperamental theory of Thomas and Chess [72,81] (see Table A1). The three original temperamental categories according to Thomas and Chess’s theory are easy, difficult and slow to warm up. However, since the difficult and slow-to-warm-up categories conceptually overlap, we combined all adjectives that could belong to either category into a single “difficult” category. Thomas and Chess describe both the difficult and slow-to-start baby as characterised by slow adaptability and withdrawal response to novel stimuli, although difficult temperament is also represented by arrhythmicity and high frequency of negative mood [11]. In contrast, easy babies are characterised by rhythmicity, a positive approach to the new, adaptability and a predominant positive mood of mild to moderate intensity.
The remaining, purely descriptive adjectives were classed as “positive” or “negative” adjectives and were not further considered in this study.
Total “easy” and “difficult” scores for each infant were calculated as the sum of adjectives chosen by mothers for each category.
Table A1. Adjectives for each category of the ePrime questionnaire on infant temperament.
Table A1. Adjectives for each category of the ePrime questionnaire on infant temperament.
Easy TemperamentDifficult Temperament
SettledAgitated
ActiveDifficult
ResponsiveTough
CuddlyDemanding
AlertInactive
ContentedUnresponsive
EasyNot cuddly
Sleepy

References

  1. WHO. Preterm Birth. Available online: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/preterm-birth (accessed on 21 October 2021).
  2. Goldenberg, R.L.; Culhane, J.F.; Iams, J.D.; Romero, R. Preterm Birth 1 Epidemiology and Causes of Preterm Birth. Lancet 2008, 371, 75–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Saigal, S.; Doyle, L. An overview of mortality and sequelae of preterm birth from infancy to adulthood. Lancet 2008, 371, 261–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Johnson, S.; Marlow, N. Preterm Birth and Childhood Psychiatric Disorders. Pediatr. Res. 2011, 69, 11R–18R. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Sanson, A.; Hemphill, S.A.; Smart, D. Connections between Temperament and Social Development: A Review. Soc. Dev. 2004, 13, 142–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Goldsmith, H.H.; Buss, A.H.; Plomin, R.; Rothbart, M.K.; Thomas, A.; Chess, S.; Hinde, R.A.; McCall, R.B. Roundtable: What Is Temperament? Four Approaches. Child Dev. 1987, 58, 505–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Kochanska, G.; Murray, K.T.; Harlan, E.T. Effortful Control in Early Childhood: Continuity and Change, Antecedents, and Implications for Social Development. Dev. Psychol. 2000, 36, 220–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Rothbart, M.K. Becoming Who We Are: Temperament and Personality in Development; Reprint ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  9. Rothbart, M.K.; Ahadi, S.A.; Hershey, K.L.; Fisher, P. Investigations of Temperament at Three to Seven Years: The Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 1394–1408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Rothbart, M.K.; Ellis, L.K.; Rueda, M.R.; Posner, M.I. Developing Mechanisms of Temperamental Effortful Control. J. Pers. 2003, 71, 1113–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Thomas, A.; Chess, S. The New York Longitudinal Study: From Infancy to Early Adult Life. In The Study of Temperament: Changes, Continuities, and Challenges; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1986; pp. 39–52. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cassiano, R.G.M.; Gaspardo, C.M.; Furini, G.C.B.; Martinez, F.E.; Linhares, M.B.M. Impact of neonatal risk and temperament on behavioral problems in toddlers born preterm. Early Hum. Dev. 2016, 103, 175–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Forbes, M.K.; Rapee, R.M.; Krueger, R.F. Opportunities for the prevention of mental disorders by reducing general psychopathology in early childhood. Behav. Res. Ther. 2019, 119, 103411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Poletti, M.; Raballo, A. Early intervention in psychiatry through a developmental perspective. NPJ Schizophr. 2021, 7, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Belsky, J.; Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J.; van Ijzendoorn, M.H. For Better and For Worse: Differential Susceptibility to Environmental Influences. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 16, 300–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Feldman, R. Mutual influences between child emotion regulation and parent–child reciprocity support development across the first 10 years of life: Implications for developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 2015, 27, 1007–1023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Sameroff, A. A Unified Theory of Development: A Dialectic Integration of Nature and Nurture. Child Dev. 2010, 81, 6–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Katainen, S.; Raïkkönen, K.; Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. Childhood temperament and mother’s child-rearing attitudes: Stability and interaction in a three-year follow-up study. Eur. J. Pers. 1997, 11, 249–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Laukkanen, J.; Ojansuu, U.; Tolvanen, A.; Alatupa, S.; Aunola, K. Child’s Difficult Temperament and Mothers’ Parenting Styles. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2014, 23, 312–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Lee, E.; Zhou, Q.; Eisenberg, N.; Wang, Y. Bidirectional relations between temperament and parenting styles in Chinese children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2012, 37, 57–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Porter, C.; Hart, C.; Yang, C.; Robinson, C.; Olsen, S.F.; Zeng, Q.; Olsen, J.; Jin, S. A comparative study of child temperament and parenting in Beijing, China and the western United States. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2005, 29, 541–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Coplan, R.J.; Prakash, K.; O’Neil, K.; Armer, M. Do You “Want” to Play? Distinguishing Between Conflicted Shyness and Social Disinterest in Early Childhood. Dev. Psychol. 2004, 40, 244–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Rubin, K.H.; Nelson, L.J.; Hastings, P.; Asendorpf, J. The Transaction between Parents’ Perceptions of their Children’s Shyness and their Parenting Styles. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 1999, 23, 937–957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Kiel, E.J.; Buss, K.A. Associations among Context-specific Maternal Protective Behavior, Toddlers’ Fearful Temperament, and Maternal Accuracy and Goals: Context-Specific Protective Behavior. Soc. Dev. 2012, 21, 742–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Dadds, M.R.; Roth, J.H. Family Processes in the Development of Anxiety Problems. In The Developmental Psychopathology of Anxiety; Oxford University Press (OUP): Oxford, UK, 2001; pp. 278–303. [Google Scholar]
  26. Sanson, A.; Rothbart, M.K. Child Temperament and Parenting. Handb. Parent. 1995, 4, 299–321. [Google Scholar]
  27. Bates, J.E.; Schermerhorn, A.C.; Petersen, I.T. Temperament and Parenting in Developmental Perspective. In Handbook of Temperament; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 425–441. [Google Scholar]
  28. Belsky, J.; Hsieh, K.-H.; Crnic, K. Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys’ externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience? Dev. Psychopathol. 1998, 10, 301–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Morris, A.S.; Silk, J.; Steinberg, L.; Sessa, F.M.; Avenevoli, S.; Essex, M.J. Temperamental Vulnerability and Negative Parenting as Interacting Predictors of Child Adjustment. J. Marriage Fam. 2002, 64, 461–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Sacchi, C.; De Carli, P.; Vieno, A.; Piallini, G.; Zoia, S.; Simonelli, A. Does infant negative emotionality moderate the effect of maternal depression on motor development? Early Hum. Dev. 2018, 119, 56–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Arcus, D. Inhibited and Uninhibited Children: Biology in the Social Context. In Temperament in Context; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2001; pp. 43–60. [Google Scholar]
  32. Chess, S.; Thomas, A. Origins and Evolution of Behavior Disorders: From Infancy to Early Adult Life; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  33. Belsky, J.; Pluess, M. Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 885–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Slagt, M.; Dubas, J.S.; Deković, M.; Van Aken, M.A.G. Differences in sensitivity to parenting depending on child temperament: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2016, 142, 1068–1110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Cassiano, R.G.; Provenzi, L.; Linhares, M.B.M.; Gaspardo, C.M.; Montirosso, R. Does preterm birth affect child temperament? A meta-analytic study. Infant Behav. Dev. 2020, 58, 101417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Allotey, J.; Zamora, J.; Cheong-See, F.; Kalidindi, M.; Arroyo-Manzano, D.; Asztalos, E.; Van Der Post, J.A.M.; Mol, B.W.; Moore, D.; Birtles, D.; et al. Cognitive, motor, behavioural and academic performances of children born preterm: A meta-analysis and systematic review involving 64 061 children. BJOG Int. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. 2018, 125, 16–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Bora, S.; Pritchard, V.E.; Chen, Z.; Inder, T.E.; Woodward, L.J. Neonatal cerebral morphometry and later risk of persistent inattention/hyperactivity in children born very preterm. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 828–838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Bergh, B.R.V.D.; Mulder, E.; Mennes, M.; Glover, V. Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: Links and possible mechanisms. A review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005, 29, 237–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Lautarescu, A.; Pecheva, D.; Nosarti, C.; Nihouarn, J.; Zhang, H.; Victor, S.; Craig, M.; Edwards, A.D.; Counsell, S. Maternal Prenatal Stress Is Associated with Altered Uncinate Fasciculus Microstructure in Premature Neonates. Biol. Psychiatry 2020, 87, 559–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Takegata, M.; Matsunaga, A.; Ohashi, Y.; Toizumi, M.; Yoshida, L.M.; Kitamura, T. Prenatal and Intrapartum Factors Associated with Infant Temperament: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 609020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Kanel, D.; Vanes, L.D.; Pecheva, D.; Hadaya, L.; Falconer, S.; Counsell, S.J.; Edwards, D.A.; Nosarti, C. Neonatal White Matter Microstructure and Emotional Development during the Preschool Years in Children Who Were Born Very Preterm. Eneuro 2021, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Kanel, D.; Vanes, L.D.; Ball, G.; Hadaya, L.; Falconer, S.; Counsell, S.J.; Edwards, A.D.; Nosarti, C. Neonatal amygdala resting-state functional connectivity and socio-emotional development in very preterm children. Brain Commun. 2022, 4, fcac009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Clark, C.; Woodward, L.J.; Horwood, L.; Moor, S. Development of Emotional and Behavioral Regulation in Children Born Extremely Preterm and Very Preterm: Biological and Social Influences. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 1444–1462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Harel-Gadassi, A.; Friedlander, E.; Yaari, M.; Bar-Oz, B.; Eventov-Friedman, S.; Mankuta, D.; Yirmiya, N. Do developmental and temperamental characteristics mediate the association between preterm birth and the quality of mother-child interaction? Infant Behav. Dev. 2020, 58, 101421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Poehlmann, J.; Schwichtenberg, A.J.M.; Shlafer, R.J.; Hahn, E.; Bianchi, J.-P.; Warner, R. Emerging self-regulation in toddlers born preterm or low birth weight: Differential susceptibility to parenting? Dev. Psychopathol. 2011, 23, 177–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Whiteside-Mansell, L.; Bradley, R.H.; Casey, P.H.; Fussell, J.J.; Conners-Burrow, N.A. Triple Risk: Do Difficult Temperament and Family Conflict Increase the Likelihood of Behavioral Maladjustment in Children Born Low Birth Weight and Preterm? J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2008, 34, 396–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Putnam, S.P.; Rothbart, M.K. Development of Short and Very Short Forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. J. Pers. Assess. 2006, 87, 102–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Edwards, A.D.; Redshaw, M.E.; Kennea, N.; Rivero-Arias, O.; Gonzales-Cinca, N.; Nongena, P.; Ederies, M.; Falconer, S.; Chew, A.; Omar, O.; et al. Effect of MRI on preterm infants and their families: A randomised trial with nested diagnostic and economic evaluation. Arch. Dis. Child.-Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2017, 103, F15–F21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Redshaw, M.E.; Harris, A. Maternal perceptions of neonatal care. Acta Paediatr. 1995, 84, 593–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Redshaw, M.E. Mothers of babies requiring special care: Attitudes and experiences. J. Reprod. Infant Psychol. 1997, 15, 109–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Henderson, J.; Carson, C.; Redshaw, M. Impact of preterm birth on maternal well-being and women’s perceptions of their baby: A population-based survey. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e012676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Derryberry, D.; Rothbart, M.K. Arousal, affect, and attention as components of temperament. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 55, 958–966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Putnam, S.P.; Ellis, L.K.; Rothbart, M.K. The Structure of Temperament from Infancy through Adolescence. Adv. Res. Temperam. 2001, 165, 163–180. [Google Scholar]
  54. Arnold, D.S.; O’Leary, S.G.; Wolff, L.S.; Acker, M.M. The Parenting Scale: A measure of dysfunctional parenting in discipline situations. Psychol. Assess. 1993, 5, 137–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2013; Available online: http://www-R-project.org/ (accessed on 30 March 2022).
  56. RStudio Team. RStudio: Integrated Development for R; RStudio, Inc.: Boston, MA, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  57. Dattalo, P. Analysis of Multiple Dependent Variables; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  58. Haase, R.F.; Ellis, M.V. Multivariate Analysis of Variance. J. Couns. Psychol. 1987, 34, 404. [Google Scholar]
  59. Guerin, D.W.; Gottfried, A.W. Temperamental consequences of infant difficultness. Infant Behav. Dev. 1994, 17, 413–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Rothbart, M.K.; Derryberry, D.; Hershey, K.L. Stability of Temperament in Childhood: Laboratory Infant Assessment to Parent Report at Seven Years. In Temperament and Personality Development across the Life Span; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 85–119. [Google Scholar]
  61. Côté, S.M.; Boivin, M.; Liu, X.; Nagin, D.S.; Zoccolillo, M.; Tremblay, R.E. Depression and anxiety symptoms: Onset, developmental course and risk factors during early childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2009, 50, 1201–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Gilliom, M.; Shaw, D.S. Codevelopment of externalizing and internalizing problems in early childhood. Dev. Psychopathol. 2004, 16, 313–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  63. Guerin, D.W.; Gottfried, A.W.; Thomas, C.W. Difficult Temperament and Behaviour Problems: A Longitudinal Study from 1.5 to 12 Years. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 1997, 21, 71–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Hagekull, B. Infant Temperament and Early Childhood Functioning: Possible Relations to the Five-Factor Model. In The Developing Structure of Temperament and Personality from Infancy to Adulthood; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1994; pp. 227–240. [Google Scholar]
  65. Shaw, D.S.; Keenan, K.; Vondra, J.I.; Delliquardi, E.; Giovannelli, J. Antecedents of Preschool Children’s Internalizing Problems: A Longitudinal Study of Low-Income Families. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1997, 36, 1760–1767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Gracioli, S.M.A.; Linhares, M.B.M. Neonatal and temperament variables predict behavior problems of preterm children at toddlerhood. J. Hum. Growth Dev. 2019, 29, 313–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Guajardo, N.R.; Snyder, G.; Petersen, R. Relationships among parenting practices, parental stress, child behaviour, and children’s social-cognitive development. Infant Child Dev. 2009, 18, 37–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Kim, H.-J.; Arnold, D.H.; Fisher, P.H.; Zeljo, A. Parenting and Preschoolers’ Symptoms as a Function of Child Gender and SES. Child Fam. Behav. Ther. 2005, 27, 23–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Snyder, J.J. Reinforcement analysis of interaction in problem and nonproblem families. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1977, 86, 528–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Stams, G.-J.J.M.; Juffer, F.; van Ijzendoorn, M.H. Maternal sensitivity, infant attachment, and temperament in early childhood predict adjustment in middle childhood: The case of adopted children and their biologically unrelated parents. Dev. Psychol. 2002, 38, 806–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Tschann, J.M.; Kaiser, P.; Chesney, M.A.; Alkon, A.; Boyce, W.T. Resilience and Vulnerability among Preschool Children: Family Functioning, Temperament, and Behavior Problems. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1996, 35, 184–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Rabin, A.I.; Thomas, A.; Chess, S.; Birch, H.G. Temperament and Behavior Disorders in Children. Am. J. Psychol. 1968, 81, 612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Arcus, D.; Gardner, S.; Anderson, C. Infant Reactivity, Maternal Style, and the Development of Inhibited and Uninhibited Behavioral Profiles. In Proceedings of the Symposium Conducted at the Biennial Meeting of the International Society for Infant Studies; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  74. Bates, J.E.; Pettit, G.S.; Dodge, K.A.; Ridge, B. Interaction of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive parenting in the development of externalizing behavior. Dev. Psychol. 1998, 34, 982–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Hagekull, B.; Bohlin, G. Day care quality, family and child characteristics and socioemotional development. Early Child. Res. Q. 1995, 10, 505–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Carey, W.B. Validity of Parental Assessments of Development and Behavior. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 1982, 136, 97–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Rothbart, M.K.; Bates, J.E. Temperament. In Handbook of Child Psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2006; Volume 3, pp. 105–176. [Google Scholar]
  78. Rettew, D.C.; Stanger, C.; McKee, L.; Doyle, A.; Hudziak, J.J. Interactions between child and parent temperament and child behavior problems. Compr. Psychiatry 2006, 47, 412–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Morawska, A.; Winter, L.; Sanders, M.R. Parenting knowledge and its role in the prediction of dysfunctional parenting and disruptive child behaviour. Child Care Health Dev. 2009, 35, 217–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Benzies, K.M.; Magill-Evans, J.E.; Hayden, K.A.; Ballantyne, M. Key components of early intervention programs for preterm infants and their parents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 2013, 13, S10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Thomas, A.; Chess, S.; Birch, H.G.; Hertzig, M.E.; Korn, S. Behavioral Individuality in Early Childhood; New York University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1963. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Association between difficult infant temperament and childhood Negative Affectivity (controlling for easy temperament, sex, GA at birth, IMD score and age at follow-up assessment).
Figure 1. Association between difficult infant temperament and childhood Negative Affectivity (controlling for easy temperament, sex, GA at birth, IMD score and age at follow-up assessment).
Children 09 00508 g001
Figure 2. Association between easy infant temperament and childhood Negative Affectivity as a function of parental laxness (controlling for difficult temperament, sex, GA at birth, IMD score and age at follow-up assessment).
Figure 2. Association between easy infant temperament and childhood Negative Affectivity as a function of parental laxness (controlling for difficult temperament, sex, GA at birth, IMD score and age at follow-up assessment).
Children 09 00508 g002
Table 1. Study participants’ socio-demographic characteristics.
Table 1. Study participants’ socio-demographic characteristics.
Sample with Complete Temperamental Data (n = 190)Sample with Complete Temperamental + Parenting Data (n = 101)
Gestational age at birth in weeks, median (range)30.07 (23.86–32.86)29.43 (23.86–32.86)
Birth weight in grams, median (range)1278 (600–2600)1260 (600–2400)
Female, n (%)88 (46.32%)41 (40.59%)
Days in hospital, median (range)45 (3–139)50 (11–139)
Index of multiple deprivation (IMD) quintile, n (%)
  1 (Least Deprived)46 (24.21%)27 (26.73%)
  234 (17.89%)17 (16.83%)
  340 (21.05%)19 (18.81%)
  449 (25.79%)29 (28.71%)
  5 (Most Deprived)21 (11.05%)9 (8.91%)
Corrected age at infant assessment in months, median (range)11.27 (9–18.4)11.30 (9.27–18.4)
Corrected age at follow-up assessment in years, median (range)4.64 (4.18–7.17)5.18 (4.43–7.17)
Mother’s age at infant’s birth (years), median (range)33.53 (19.87–53.45)34.25 (20.08–48.76)
Mother’s age leaving full-time education, at birth, n (%)
  16 or less11 (5.79%)6 (5.94%)
  17–1926 (13.68%)9 (8.91%)
  19 or more142 (74.74%)81 (80.2%)
  Still in full-time education4 (2.11%)1 (0.99%)
  Missing7 (3.68%)4 (3.96%)
Maternal ethnicity, n (%)
  White104 (54.74%)55 (54.46%)
  Black or Black British37 (19.47%)21 (20.79%)
  Asian or Asian British38 (20%)19 (18.81%)
  Mixed3 (1.58%)1 (0.99%)
  Other ethnic group6 (3.16%)4 (3.96%)
  Missing2 (1.05%)1 (0.99%)
Table 2. Results of the multivariate regression analysis jointly regressing childhood temperament (Negative Affect, Surgency and Effortful Control) on infant temperament (easy and difficult), controlling for age, sex, gestational age (GA) and index of multiple deprivation (IMD). An uncorrected significance threshold of p = 0.05 was used.
Table 2. Results of the multivariate regression analysis jointly regressing childhood temperament (Negative Affect, Surgency and Effortful Control) on infant temperament (easy and difficult), controlling for age, sex, gestational age (GA) and index of multiple deprivation (IMD). An uncorrected significance threshold of p = 0.05 was used.
F(3,181)p
Easy infant temperament3.10.027
Difficult infant temperament4.50.004
Sex6.8<0.001
GA1.20.323
IMD1.70.162
Age2.40.072
Table 3. Results of univariate follow-up analyses separately regressing Negative Affectivity, Surgency or Effortful Control on infant temperament (easy and difficult), controlling for age, sex, gestational age (GA) and index of multiple deprivation (IMD). A Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold of p = 0.017 was used.
Table 3. Results of univariate follow-up analyses separately regressing Negative Affectivity, Surgency or Effortful Control on infant temperament (easy and difficult), controlling for age, sex, gestational age (GA) and index of multiple deprivation (IMD). A Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold of p = 0.017 was used.
Dependent Variable: Negative AffectivityDependent Variable: SurgencyDependent Variable: Effortful Control
βpβpβp
Easy infant temperament−0.060.0310.030.308−0.040.085
Difficult infant temperament0.150.002−0.080.1040.020.516
Sex−0.130.885−0.37<0.0010.080.230
GA0.030.1950.150.465−0.010.565
IMD0.010.030<0.010.851<0.010.272
Age−0.040.490−0.080.179−0.120.013
Table 4. Results of the winning multivariate regression analysis jointly regressing childhood temperament (Negative Affect, Surgency and Effortful Control) on easy and difficult infant temperament, parental laxness and its interaction with easy infant temperament, controlling for age, sex, gestational age (GA) and index of multiple deprivation (IMD). A Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold of p = 0.008 was used.
Table 4. Results of the winning multivariate regression analysis jointly regressing childhood temperament (Negative Affect, Surgency and Effortful Control) on easy and difficult infant temperament, parental laxness and its interaction with easy infant temperament, controlling for age, sex, gestational age (GA) and index of multiple deprivation (IMD). A Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold of p = 0.008 was used.
F(3,90)p
Easy infant temperament4.20.008
Difficult infant temperament3.20.027
Parental laxness1.40.250
Parental laxness × easy infant temperament5.80.001
Sex3.70.015
GA0.70.548
IMD1.50.221
Age1.00.387
Table 5. Results of univariate follow-up analyses separately regressing Negative Affectivity, Surgency or Effortful Control on infant temperament, parental laxness and its interaction with easy infant temperament, controlling for age, sex, gestational age (GA) and index of multiple deprivation (IMD). A Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold of p = 0.003 was used.
Table 5. Results of univariate follow-up analyses separately regressing Negative Affectivity, Surgency or Effortful Control on infant temperament, parental laxness and its interaction with easy infant temperament, controlling for age, sex, gestational age (GA) and index of multiple deprivation (IMD). A Bonferroni-corrected significance threshold of p = 0.003 was used.
Dependent Variable: Negative AffectivityDependent Variable: SurgencyDependent Variable: Effortful Control
βpβpβp
Easy infant temperament−0.71<0.001−0.090.645−0.380.009
Difficult infant temperament0.160.015−0.120.0970.020.651
Parental laxness−1.08<0.001−0.100.756−0.580.018
Parental laxness × easy infant temperament0.20<0.0010.040.4600.090.032
Sex−0.030.789−0.350.0080.090.389
GA0.030.170−0.010.630−0.100.725
IMD<0.010.091<0.010.943<0.010.670
Age0.010.842−0.070.408−0.090.122
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lovato, I.; Vanes, L.D.; Sacchi, C.; Simonelli, A.; Hadaya, L.; Kanel, D.; Falconer, S.; Counsell, S.; Redshaw, M.; Kennea, N.; et al. Early Childhood Temperamental Trajectories following Very Preterm Birth and Their Association with Parenting Style. Children 2022, 9, 508. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9040508

AMA Style

Lovato I, Vanes LD, Sacchi C, Simonelli A, Hadaya L, Kanel D, Falconer S, Counsell S, Redshaw M, Kennea N, et al. Early Childhood Temperamental Trajectories following Very Preterm Birth and Their Association with Parenting Style. Children. 2022; 9(4):508. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9040508

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lovato, Irene, Lucy D. Vanes, Chiara Sacchi, Alessandra Simonelli, Laila Hadaya, Dana Kanel, Shona Falconer, Serena Counsell, Maggie Redshaw, Nigel Kennea, and et al. 2022. "Early Childhood Temperamental Trajectories following Very Preterm Birth and Their Association with Parenting Style" Children 9, no. 4: 508. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9040508

APA Style

Lovato, I., Vanes, L. D., Sacchi, C., Simonelli, A., Hadaya, L., Kanel, D., Falconer, S., Counsell, S., Redshaw, M., Kennea, N., Edwards, A. D., & Nosarti, C. (2022). Early Childhood Temperamental Trajectories following Very Preterm Birth and Their Association with Parenting Style. Children, 9(4), 508. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9040508

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop