Monitoring of Communication Precursors in Extremely Low Birth Weight (ELBW) Newborns by Video Analysis Method: Preliminary Results
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Griffiths Mental Development Scale (GMDS-R)
2.2. Video Recording Session
2.3. Statistical Analysis
3. Results
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Gráf, R.; Kalmár, M.; Harnos, A.; Boross, G.; Nagy, A. Reading and spelling skills of prematurely born children in light of the underlying cognitive factors. Cogn. Process. 2021, 22, 311–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pugliese, M.; Rossi, C.; Guidotti, I.; Gallo, C.; Della Casa, E.; Bertoncelli, N.; Coccolini, E.; Ferrari, F. Preterm birth and developmental problems in infancy and preschool age Part II: Cognitive, neuropsychological and behavioural outcomes. J. Matern. Fetal Neonatal Med. 2013, 26, 1653–1657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Imafuku, M.; Kawai, M.; Niwa, F.; Shinya, Y.; Myowa, M. Audiovisual speech perception and language acquisition in preterm infants: A longitudinal study. Early Hum. Dev. 2019, 128, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foster-Cohen, S.; Edgin, J.O.; Champion, P.R.; Woodward, L.J. Early delayed language development in very preterm infants: Evidence from the MacArthur-Bates CDI. J. Child Lang. 2007, 34, 655–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sansavini, A.; Guarini, A.; Savini, S.; Broccoli, S.; Justice, L.; Alessandroni, R.; Faldella, G. Longitudinal trajectories of gestural and linguistic abilities in very preterm infants in the second year of life. Neuropsychology 2011, 49, 3677–3688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- van Noort-van der Spek, I.L.; Franken, M.-C.J.P.; Weisglas-Kuperus, N. Language Functions in Preterm Children: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Pediatrics 2012, 129, 745–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Liberman, Z.; Woodward, A.L.; Keysar, B.; Kinzler, K.D. Exposure to multiple languages enhances communication skills in infancy. Dev. Sci. 2017, 20, e12420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mcmahon, E.; Wintermark, P.; Lahav, A. Auditory brain development in premature infants: The importance of early experience. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2012, 1252, 17–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dehaene-Lambertz, G.; Hertz-Pannier, L.; Dubois, J.; Mériaux, S.; Roche, A.; Sigman, M.; Dehaene, S. Functional organization of perisylvian activation during presentation of sentences in preverbal infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 14240–14245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Saito, Y.; Kondo, T.; Aoyama, S.; Fukumoto, R.; Konishi, N.; Nakamura, K.; Kobayashi, M.; Toshima, T. The function of the frontal lobe in neonates for response to a prosodic voice. Early Hum. Dev. 2007, 83, 225–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gratier, M.; Devouche, E.; Guellaï, B.; Infanti, R.; Yilmaz, E.; Parlato-Oliveira, E. Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Watt, N.; Wetherby, A.; Shumway, S. Prelinguistic Predictors of Language Outcome at 3 Years of Age. J. Speech Lang. Hear Res. 2006, 49, 1224–1237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandormael, C.; Schoenhals, L.; Hüppi, P.; Filippa, M.; Tolsa, C.B. Language in Preterm Born Children: Atypical Development and Effects of Early Interventions on Neuroplasticity. Neural Plast. 2019, 2019, 6873270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ertmer, D.J.; Jung, J.; Kloiber, D.T. Beginning to Talk Like an Adult: Increases in Speech-Like Utterances in Young Cochlear Implant Recipients and Toddlers with Normal Hearing. Am. J. Speech Lang Pathol. 2013, 22, 591–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Morgan, L.; Wren, Y.E. A Systematic Review of the Literature on Early Vocalizations and Babbling Patterns in Young Children. Commun. Disord. Q. 2018, 40, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tait, M. The Role of Singing in the Social and Linguistic Development of Nursery-Aged Deaf Children. Ph.D. Thesis, Nottingham University, Nottingham, UK, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Tait, M.; Lutman, M.E.; Nikolopoulos, T.P. Communication development in young deaf children: Review of the video analysis method. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2001, 61, 105–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tait, M.; Nikolopoulos, T.P.; Lutman, M.E.; Wilson, D.; Wells, P. Video analysis of preverbal communication behaviors: Use and reliability. Deaf. Educ. Int. 2001, 3, 38–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wadnerkar Kamble, M.; Lam-Cassettari, C.; James, D.M. Communication Skills and Communicative Autonomy of Prelinguistic Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Children: Application of a Video Feedback Intervention. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Olswang, L.; Stoel-Gammon, C.; Coggins, T.; Carpenter, R. Assessing Prelinguistic and Early Linguistic Behaviors in Developmentally Young Children; University of Washington Press: Seattle, WA, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Bortolini, U. In Proceedings of Diagnosi Precoce e Prevenzione dei Disturbi del Linguaggio e della Comunicazione, Conegliano, Veneto, 9–11 November 1992. Available online: https://opac.bncf.firenze.sbn.it/bncf-prod/resource?uri=BVE0091994&found=1 (accessed on 20 February 2022).
- Griffith, R. GMDS-R: Griffiths Mental Development Scales, Revised: O-2 Anni: Edizione Italiana a Cura di Francesca Maria Battaglia e Margherita Savoini; Revisione 1996 di Michael Huntley; Giunti O.S., Organizzazioni Special: Firenze, Italy, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M.; Carpenter, M.; Liszkowski, U. A New Look at Infant Pointing. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 705–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caselli, M.C.; Rinaldi, P.; Stefanini, S.; Volterra, V. Early Action and Gesture “Vocabulary” and Its Relation with Word Comprehension and Production. Child Dev. 2012, 83, 526–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Déak, G.O. Interrelations of language and cognitive development. In Encyclopedia of Language Development; UC San Diego Permalink: San Diego, CA, USA, 2014; pp. 284–291. Available online: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/2833j0rj (accessed on 20 February 2022).
- De Groote, I.; Vanhaesebrouck, P.; Bruneel, E.; Dom, L.; Durein, I.; Hasaerts, D.; Laroche, S.; Oostra, A.; Ortibus, E.; Roeyers, H.; et al. Outcome at 3 Years of Age in a Population-Based Cohort of Extremely Preterm Infants. Obstet. Gynecol. 2007, 110, 855–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stipdonk, L.W.; Franken, M.-C.J.P.; Dudink, J. Language outcome related to brain structures in school-aged preterm children: A systematic review. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0196607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brignoni-Pérez, E.; Morales, M.C.; Marchman, V.A.; Scala, M.; Feldman, H.M.; Yeom, K.; Travis, K.E. Listening to Mom in the NICU: Effects of increased maternal speech exposure on language outcomes and white matter development in infants born very preterm. Trials 2021, 22, 444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neri, E.; De Pascalis, L.; Agostini, F.; Genova, F.; Biasini, A.; Stella, M.; Trombini, E. Parental Book-Reading to Preterm Born Infants in NICU: The Effects on Language Development in the First Two Years. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Linsell, L.; Malouf, R.; Morris, J.; Kurinczuk, J.J.; Marlow, N. Prognostic Factors for Poor Cognitive Development in Children Born Very Preterm or With Very Low Birth Weight: A Systematic Review. JAMA Pediatr. 2015, 169, 1162–1172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McGowan, E.C.; Vohr, B.R. Neurodevelopmental Follow-up of Preterm Infants: What Is New? Pediatr. Clin. N. Am. 2019, 66, 509–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Communication Precursors 12 | 12 Months CA (n = 45) | 24 Months CA (n = 45) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SD | Mean | SD | |
Eye Contacts | 171.38 | 78.49 | 229.36 | 76.46 |
Vocal Turns (VT) with eye contacts | 1.74 | 1.05 | 4.67 | 2.67 |
Vocal Turns (VT) without eye contacts | 1.10 | 0.82 | 2.76 | 2.13 |
Gestural Turns | 5.31 | 1.76 | 3.36 | 1.56 |
Utterances | 3.56 | 3.50 | 23.26 | 12.83 |
Difference in Means | Std. Dev | Paired Difference 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Lower | Upper | |||
Eye contacts | −57.97 | 74.55 | −81.21 | −34.74 |
Vocal turns | −2.92 | 2.20 | −3.61 | −2.23 |
Gestural turns | 1.95 | 1.97 | 1.33 | 2.56 |
Non-looking vocal turns | −1.66 | 1.93 | −2.26 | −1.06 |
Utterances | −19.69 | 12.47 | −23.737 | −15.64 |
Means | Std. Dev | Neurodevelopmental Impairment (%) | |
---|---|---|---|
Locomotor Scale | 100.09 | 14.08 | 6.6% |
Personal Social Scale | 106.91 | 15.02 | 8.8% |
Hearing and Language Scale | 94.35 | 20.53 | 17.8% |
Eye and Hand Coordination Scale | 107.85 | 13.88 | 4.4% |
Performance Scale | 102.62 | 11.11 | 6.6% |
General Quotient | 102.26 | 14.92 | 8.8% |
Locomotor | Personal Social | Hearing Language | Eye and Hand | Performance | General | Quotient | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Eye contacts | Pearson CC | 0.156 | 0.110 | 0.336 | 0.287 | 0.062 | 0.182 |
Sig. (2-tails) | 0.431 | 0.510 | 0.045 (*) | 0.073 | 0.366 | 0.273 | |
Vocal turns | Pearson CC | 0.056 | 0.416 | 0.327 | 0.127 | 0.082 | 0.345 |
Sig. (2-tails) | 0.414 | 0.002 (**) | 0.052 | 0.588 | 0.401 | 0.034 (*) | |
Gestual turns | Pearson CC | 0.028 | 0.142 | 0.130 | 0.131 | 0.093 | 0.089 |
Sig. (2-tails) | 0.681 | 0.396 | 0.451 | 0.057 | 0.174 | 0.558 | |
Non-looking | |||||||
vocal turns | Pearson CC | 0.184 | 0.110 | 0.288 | 0.087 | 0.125 | 0.289 |
Sig. (2-tails) | 0.401 | 0.510 | 0.890 | 0.207 | 0.068 | 0.102 | |
Utterances | Pearson CC | 0.118 | 0.485 | 0.350 | 0.099 | 0.125 | 0.401 |
Sig. (2-tails) | 0.087 | 0.002 (**) | 0.036 (*) | 0.151 | 0.068 | 0.013 (*) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sundas, L.; Palma, S.; Pugliese, M.; Roversi, M.F.; Apa, E.; Berardi, A.; Genovese, E.; Monzani, D. Monitoring of Communication Precursors in Extremely Low Birth Weight (ELBW) Newborns by Video Analysis Method: Preliminary Results. Children 2022, 9, 602. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9050602
Sundas L, Palma S, Pugliese M, Roversi MF, Apa E, Berardi A, Genovese E, Monzani D. Monitoring of Communication Precursors in Extremely Low Birth Weight (ELBW) Newborns by Video Analysis Method: Preliminary Results. Children. 2022; 9(5):602. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9050602
Chicago/Turabian StyleSundas, Laura, Silvia Palma, Marisa Pugliese, Maria Federica Roversi, Enrico Apa, Alberto Berardi, Elisabetta Genovese, and Daniele Monzani. 2022. "Monitoring of Communication Precursors in Extremely Low Birth Weight (ELBW) Newborns by Video Analysis Method: Preliminary Results" Children 9, no. 5: 602. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9050602
APA StyleSundas, L., Palma, S., Pugliese, M., Roversi, M. F., Apa, E., Berardi, A., Genovese, E., & Monzani, D. (2022). Monitoring of Communication Precursors in Extremely Low Birth Weight (ELBW) Newborns by Video Analysis Method: Preliminary Results. Children, 9(5), 602. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9050602