Using Theory to Drive Intervention Efficacy: The Role of Dose Form in Interventions for Children with DLD
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Conceptualising Dose Form
3. Key Findings and the Hypothesised Theories Underpinning Them
4. Techniques
4.1. Vocabulary
4.2. Morphosyntax
5. Procedure: Order and Combination of Techniques
Morphosyntax
6. Method of Instruction: Explicit and Implicit Methods
Morphosyntax
7. Intervention Context: Activity, Child-Centered—Clinician Directed, Variability
7.1. Vocabulary
7.2. Morphosyntax
8. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Frizelle, P.; Tolonen, A.-K.; Tulip, J.; Murphy, C.-A.; Saldana, D.; McKean, C. The Impact of Intervention Dose Form on Oral Language Outcomes for Children with Developmental Language Disorder. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2021, 64, 3253–3288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Warren, S.F.; Fey, M.E.; Yoder, P.J. Differential treatment intensity research: A missing link to creating optimally effective communication interventions. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2007, 13, 70–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Proctor-Williams, K. Dosage and distribution in morphosyntax intervention: Current evidence and future needs. Top. Lang. Disord. 2009, 29, 294–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Korat, O.; Graister, T.; Altman, C. Contribution of reading an e-book with a dictionary to word learning: Comparison between kindergarteners with and without SLI. J. Commun. Disord. 2019, 79, 90–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eidsvåg, S.S.; Plante, E.; Oglivie, T.; Privette, C.; Mailend, M.L. Individual versus small group treatment of morphological errors for children with developmental language disorder. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2019, 50, 237–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lüke, C.; Rohlfing, K.; Stenneken, P. Signs and communicative communication in children with a specific language development disorder. Lang. Voice Hear. 2011, 35, e149–e157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fey, M.E.; Leonard, L.B.; Bredin-Oja, S.L.; Deevy, P. A clinical evaluation of the competing sources of input hypothesis. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2017, 60, 104–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Steele, S.C.; Willoughby, L.M.; Mills, M.T. Learning word meanings during reading: Effects of phonological and semantic cues on children with language impairment. Int. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2013, 15, 184–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hassink, J.M.; Leonard, L.B. Within-treatment factors as predictors of outcomes following conversational recasting. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2010, 19, 213–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hoof L van, B.; Hermans, D.; Knoors, H.; Verhoeven, L. Effects of Signs on Word Learning by Children with Developmental Language Disorder. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2019, 62, 1798–1812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proctor-Williams, K.; Fey, M.E. Recast Density and Acquisition of Novel Irregular Past Tense Verbs. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2007, 50, 1029–1047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vogt, S.S.; Kauschke, C. Observing iconic gestures enhances word learning in typically developing children and children with specific language impairment. J. Child Lang. 2017, 44, 1458–1484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Vogt, S.S.; Kauschke, C. With some help from others’ hands: Iconic gesture helps semantic learning in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2017, 60, 3213–3225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith-Lock, K.M.; Leitao, S.; Prior, P.; Nickels, L. The effectiveness of two grammar treatment procedures for children with SLI: A randomized clinical trial. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2015, 46, 312–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yoder, P.J.; Molfese, D.; Gardner, E. Initial mean length of utterance predicts the relative efficacy of two grammatical treatments in pre-schoolers with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2011, 54, 1170–1181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horne, A.J.O.V.; Fey, M.; Curran, M. Do the Hard Things First: A Randomized Controlled Trial Testing the Effects of Exemplar Selection on Generalization Following Therapy for Grammatical Morphology. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2017, 60, 2569–2588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Horne, A.J.O.V.; Curran, M.; Larson, C.; Fey, M.E. Effects of a Complexity-Based Approach on Generalization of Past Tense—Ed and Related Morphemes. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2018, 49, 681–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plante, E.; Tucci, A.; Nicholas, K.; Arizmendi, G.D.; Vance, R. Effective Use of Auditory Bombardment as a Therapy Adjunct for Children with Developmental Language Disorders. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2018, 49, 320–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finestack, L.H.; Fey, M.H. Evaluation of a deductive procedure to teach grammatical inflections to children with language impairment. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2009, 18, 289–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finestack, L.H. Evaluation of an explicit intervention to teach novel grammatical forms to children with disorder. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2018, 61, 2062–2075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aguilar, J.M.; Plante, E.; Sandoval, M. Exemplar variability facilitates retention of word learning by children with specific language impairment. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2018, 49, 72–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Krzemien, M.; Thibaut, J.-P.; Jemel, B.; Levaux, E.; Maillart, C. How do children with developmental language disorder extend novel nouns? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2020, 202, 105010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haebig, E.; Leonard, L.B.; Deevy, P.; Karpicke, J.; Christ, S.L.; Usler, E.; Kueser, J.B.; Souto, S.; Krok, W.; Weber, C. Retrieval-based word learning in young typically developing children and children with development language disorder II: A comparison of retrieval schedules. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2019, 62, 944–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Plante, E.; Ogilvie, T.; Vance, R.; Aguilar, J.M.; Dailey, N.S.; Meyers, C.; Lieser, A.M.; Burton, R. Variability in the Language Input to Children Enhances Learning in a Treatment Context. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2014, 23, 530–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leonard, L.B.; Karpicke, J.; Deevy, P.; Weber, C.; Christ, S.; Haebig, E.; Souto, S.; Kueser, J.B.; Krok, W. Retrieval-based word learning in young typically developing children and children with developmental language disorder I: The benefits of repeated retrieval. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2019, 62, 932–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Riches, N.G.; Faragher, B.; Conti-Ramsden, G. Verb schema use and input dependence in 5-year-old children with specific language impairment (SLI). Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2006, 41, 117–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leonard, L.B.; Deevy, P.; Karpicke, J.D.; Christ, S.; Weber, C.; Kueser, J.B.; Haebig, E. Adjective Learning in Young Typically Developing Children and Children with Developmental Language Disorder: A Retrieval-Based Approach. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2019, 62, 4433–4449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smeets, D.J.H.; van Dijken, M.J.; Bus, A.G. Using electronic storybooks to support word learning in children with severe language impairments. J. Learn. Disabil. 2012, 47, 435–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kouri, T.A.; Winn, J. Lexical learning in sung and spoken story script contexts. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 2006, 22, 293–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, J.M.; Paivio, A. Dual coding theory and education. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1991, 3, 149–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McGregor, K.K.; Newman, R.M.; Reilly, R.M.; Capone, N.C. Semantic representation and naming in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2002, 45, 998–1014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alt, M.; Suddarth, R. Learning novel words: Detail and vulnerability of initial representations for children with specific language impairment and typically developing peers. J. Commun. Disord. 2012, 45, 84–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Archibald, L.M.D.; Gathercole, S.E. The complexities of complex memory span: Storage and processing deficits in specific language impairment. J. Mem. Lang. 2007, 57, 177–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Archibald, L.M.D.; Gathercole, S.E. Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2006, 41, 675–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, K.E. Facilitating children’s syntax acquisition. Dev. Psychol. 1977, 13, 101–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, K.E. Strategies for first language teaching. In The Teachability of Language; Rice, M.L., Schiefelbush, R.L., Eds.; Paul H. Brookes: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1989; pp. 263–310. [Google Scholar]
- Goldberg, A.E. Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Ambridge, B.; Pine, J.M.; Rowland, C.F. Children use verb semantics to retreat from overgeneralization errors: A novel verb grammaticality judgment study. Cogn. Linguist. 2011, 22, 303–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ambridge, B.; Lieven, E.V. Child Language Acquisition: Contrasting Theoretical Approaches; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Ambridge, B.; Pine, J.M.; Rowland, C.F.; Chang, F.; Bidgood, A. The retreat from overgeneralization in child language acquisition: Word learning, morphology, and verb argument structure. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2013, 4, 47–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Li, P.; Shirai, Y. The Acquisition of Lexical and Grammatical Aspect; De Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Dudai, Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2004, 55, 51–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Karpicke, J.D.; Roediger, H.L. The critical importance of retrieval for learning. Science 2008, 319, 966–968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hashimoto, T.; Usui, N.; Taira, M.; Kojima, S. Neural enhancement and attenuation induced by repetitive recall. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2011, 96, 143–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ullman, M.T.; Pierpont, E.I. Specific Language Impairment is not Specific to Language: The Procedural Deficit Hypothesis. CORTEX 2005, 41, 399–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lum, J.A.G.; Ramsden, G.C.; Page, D.; Ullman, M.T. Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. CORTEX 2012, 48, 1138–1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van der Lely, H.K.J. Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from Grammatical-SLI. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2005, 9, 53–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rice, M.; Wexler, K.; Redmond, S.M. Grammaticality Judgements of an extended Optional Infinitive Grammar: Evidence from English Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1999, 42, 943–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howard, M.; Kahana, M. A distributed representation of temporal context. J. Math. Psychol. 2002, 46, 269–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lehman, M.; Malmberg, K. A buffer model of encoding and temporal correlations in retrieval. Psychol. Rev. 2013, 120, 155–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Roediger, H.L.; Karpicke, J.D. Intricacies of spaced retrieval: A resolution. In Successful Remembering and Successful Forgetting; Psychology Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 41–66. [Google Scholar]
- Karpicke, J.; Lehman, M.; Aue, W. Retrieval-based learning: An episodic context account. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation; Ross, B., Ed.; Elsevier: Waltham, MA, USA, 2014; Volume 61, pp. 238–284. [Google Scholar]
- Karpicke, J. Retrieval-based learning: A decade of progress. In Cognitive Psychology of Memory, Volume 2. Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference; Wixted, J.T., Ed.; Academic Press: Oxford, UK, 2017; pp. 487–514. [Google Scholar]
- Robertson, E.K.; Joanisse, M.F.; Desroches, A.S.; Ng, S. Categorical speech perception deficits distinguish language and reading impairments in children. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 753–767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vandewalle, E.; Boets, B.; Ghesquière, P.; Zink, I. Auditory processing and speech perception in children with specific language impairment: Relations with oral language and literacy skills. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 33, 344–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spackman, M.P.; Fujiki, M.; Brinton, B.; Nelson, D.; Allen, J. The ability of children with language impairment to recognize emotion conveyed by facial expression and music. Commun. Disord. Q. 2006, 26, 131–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolfe, D.E.; Hom, C. Use of melodies as structural prompts for learning and retention of sequential verbal information by preschool students. J. Music Theory 1993, 30, 100–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wallace, W.T. Memory for music effect of melody on recall of text. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1994, 20, 1471–1485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wigram, T.; Saperston, B.; West, R. The Art and Science of Music Therapy: A Handbook; Harwood Academic Publishers: Reading, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Kemler Nelson, D.G.; Russell, R.; Duke, N.; Jones, K. Two-year-olds will name artifacts by their functions. Child Dev. 2000, 71, 1271–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Alt, M. Statistical Learning: How it Relates to Speech-Language Pathology. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 2018, 49, 631–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bybee, J. Language, Usage, and Cognition; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M. The usage-based theory of language acquisition. In The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language; Bavin, E.L., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2009; pp. 69–88. [Google Scholar]
- Gentner, D.; Markman, A.B. Structure-mapping in analogy and similarity. Am. Psychol. 1997, 52, 45–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gentner, D.; Medina, J. Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition 1998, 65, 263–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leroy, S.; Parisse, C.; Maillart, C. Analogical reasoning in children with specific language impairment. Clin. Linguist. Phon. 2012, 26, 380–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gomez, R.L. Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 13, 431–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldwater, M.B.; Tomlinson, M.T.; Echols, C.H.; Love, B.C. Structural priming as structure-mapping: Children use analogies from previous utterances to guide sentence production. Cogn. Sci. 2011, 35, 156–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Casenhiser, D.; Goldberg, A.E. Fast mapping between a phrasal form and meaning. Dev. Sci. 2005, 6, 500–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Techniques | The specific teaching behaviours/actions thought to effect change. e.g., providing word definitions (vocabulary), recasting, imitation (morphosyntax). |
Procedure | The order or combination of technique delivery. e.g., word exposures followed by word definitions (vocabulary); recasting followed by auditory bombardment (morphosyntax). |
Method of Instruction | How techniques are delivered, i.e., implicit only versus implicit plus explicit instructions. e.g., Word exposures alone versus exposures coupled with detailed definitions of targeted words (vocabulary); recasting versus recasting with an explicit explanation of the grammatical rule targeted (morphosyntax). |
Intervention Contexts | This has 3 subcomponents
|
Component | Vocabulary | Findings | Morphosyntax | Findings |
Techniques | ||||
Korat, O., Graister, T., & Altman, C. (2019) [4] Manipulation of semantic supports for new word learning in e-book activity. Short dictionary explanation vs. explanation given in the story’s context vs. a combination of the two. Country—Israel | Type of semantic support did not affect receptive word learning. Dictionary support most effective for word use in children with SLI. Explanation in context most effective when word definition was outcome. Combined approach best for those with pre-intervention higher language levels. | Eidsvåg, S. S., Plante, E., Oglivie, T., Privette, C., & Mailend, M.L. (2019) [5] Modelling versus Enhanced conversational recast treatment. Treatment given individually and in pairs. Country—United States | Positive effects shown for treatment given individually and in pairs for targeted morphemes. Children in the paired condition showed no significant gains in their ability to produce their partner’s target morpheme (where the target was only modelled). Individual treatment resulted in greater spontaneous use of target morphemes (using enhanced conversational recast treatment). | |
Lüke, C., Rohlfing, K., & Stenneken, P. (2011) [6] New word learning in a play context, with prosodic emphasis and semantic elaboration. Using simultaneous sign and speech vs. signs alone. Country—Germany | No statistical difference in number of words learned expressively or receptively 1 week post-intervention, but a statistical trend in favour of the gesture group. | Fey, M. E., Leonard, L. B., Bredin-Oja, S. L., & Deevy, P. (2017) [7] Story modelling, retelling, and recasting with competing input sources (CSI, e.g., competing interrogative form) vs. traditional approach with no competing features. Country—United States | CSI group showed much greater gains in their use of is than traditional group. No significant group differences in the production of 3 s or the control morpheme -ed. | |
Steele, S. C., Willoughby, L. M., & Mills, M. T. (2013) [8] word learning task in 4 conditions—phonological vs. semantic vs. phonological-semantic vs. control. Phonological = segmentation and blending tasks (modelled by therapist and then completed by the child). Semantic = child-friendly definitions and use of word associations/synonyms by therapist and child. Combined = phonological and semantic elements as described. Country—United States | Children with LI performesignificantly better in the semantic condition relative to the control condition. Despite the higher dose, phonological condition performance was similar to the control and combined fairly similar to semantic (but not significantly different from control). | Hassink, J. M., & Leonard, L. B. (2010) [9] Variability in conversational recasting—recasts following child utterances that were prompted by clinicians vs. clinicians’ recasts of subject-less sentences vs. clinicians’ noncorrective recasts. Country—United States | Short and long term gains in the use of 3rd person singular were associated with clinicians’ use of non-corrective recasts Recasts of subject-less sentences were associated with poorer outcomes. | |
van Berkel-van Hoof, L., Hermans, D., Knoors, H., & Verhoeven, L. (2019) [10] Pseudoword learning with iconic signs vs. no signs, using pre-recording video and pictures. Country—The Netherlands | Children with DLD learned more words with sign than without (immediately post-intervention). Signs did not influence children’s speed of response. | Proctor-Williams, K., & Fey, M. E. (2007) [11] Modelling + recasting v’s modelling alone (in play-based activity). Country—United States | No difference in accuracy of verb production whether recasts were included in the dose form or not. | |
Vogt, S. S., & Kauschke, C. (2017a and b) [12,13] Word learning in the context of a story supported by iconic vs. attention-getting gestures (both with speech). Country—Germany | Iconic co-speech gestures improved children’s comprehension, naming, semantic knowledge and word definitions to a greater degree than observing attention-directing gestures. | Smith-Lock, K. M., Leitao, S., Prior, P., & Nickels, L. (2015) [14] Recasting versus recasting + cueing (cueing procedure designed to elicit a correct production following an error to begin with). Techniques differed in the adults’ response to the child’s error). Country—Australia | Cueing + recasting group made significantly more progress than the recasting only group [who showed a negligible effect size] No group differences in maintenance of treatment effects 8 weeks post-treatment. | |
Yoder, P. J., Molfese, D., & Gardner, E. (2011) [15] Grammatical recasting (Broad target recasts BTR) versus prompting followed by a recast or model (Milieu language teaching MLT). Country—United States | For children with an MLU of 1.84, MLT was superior to BTR in facilitating grammatical development (despite its lower dose). For children with higher MLU, both treatments yielded similar responses. | |||
Component | Vocabulary | Findings | Morphosyntax | Findings |
Procedure | ||||
Van Horne, A. J. O., Fey, M., & Curran, M. (2017) [16] Complexity-based approach, manipulation in the order of verb presentation, easy to hard, or hard to easy. Country—United States | For overall verb set (target and generalisation verbs), gains in accuracy were significantly greater for hard-first group. Hard first group also made greater gains on all untreated verbs. No differences in time in therapy or progress made on verbs targeted during intervention. | |||
Owen Van Horne, A. J., Curran, M., Larson, C., & Fey, M. E. (2018) [17] Complexity-based approach, manipulation in the order of verb presentation, easy to hard, or hard to easy. Country—United States | On structured probes, the hard group first advantage (2017) no longer evident at follow-up. Hard group first showed greater gains post-treatment and at follow-up in spontaneous language samples. | |||
Plante, E., Tucci, A., Nicholas, K., Arizmendi, G. D., & Vance, R. (2018) [18] Enhanced conversational recast treatment preceded or followed by auditory bombardment (high-density presentations of target morphemes in short sentences). Country—United States | More children responded to treatment in bombardment last condition. No significant difference between bombardment. First and last conditions on morpheme use in probes; spontaneous morpheme use; unique utterances containing target morphemes. No generalisation to untreated morphemes. | |||
Component | Vocabulary | Findings | Morphosyntax | Findings |
Method of Instruction | ||||
Finestack, L.H., & Fey, M.H. (2009) [19] Deductive (explicit) v’s inductive (implicit) types of auditory prompts given (using a computer presentation). Country—United States | Although the deductive group heard fewer recasts than the inductive group, more children in the deductive group successfully used the novel morpheme in the teaching probe (10 v’s 3), the generalization probe (10 v’s 3), and the maintenance probe (7 v’s 2). | |||
Finestack, L. H. (2018) [20] Implicit only vs. implicit + explicit methods of instruction. Country—United States | Explicit instruction enhanced morphological learning. Based on combined performance across 3 targets the explicit-implicit group showed an advantage on acquisition, maintenance, and generalization probes. On individual targets the explicit-implicit group showed a significant advantage on the gender morpheme only. | |||
Component | Vocabulary | Findings | Morphosyntax | Findings |
Intervention Context | ||||
Aguilar, J. M., Plante, E., & Sandoval, M. (2018) [21] Word exposures—through the presentation of physical objects with high variability vs. no variability. Country—United States | Three weeks after the intervention the high variability group was able to identify more objects using new object exemplars of the same class than the no variability group. No differences between groups during the intervention. | Krzemien, M., Seret, E., & Maillart, C. (2020) [22] Exposure to a novel construction (NP- NP- V) in two conditions (high or progressive variability). High variability condition—sentences had no words in common. Progressive alignment condition—initially a proportion of the sentences had words in common, sentences became progressively distinct. Country—Belgium | For the novel construction children with DLD performed better in the progressive alignment condition (at chance) than in the high variability condition (below chance). They also performed better on the transitive than novel construction. | |
Haebig, E., Leonard, L. B., Deevy, P., Karpicke, J., Christ, S.L., Usler, E., Kueser, J.B., Souto, S., Krok, W., & Weberb, C. (2019) [23] Word learning using spaced retrieval (practice with contextual changes (RRCR)) versus immediate retrieval without any intervening linguistic material (IR). Retrieval practice involved using picture prompts to recall word names and definition. Country—United States | Despite a lower dose in the spaced retrieval practice condition, word retrieval exercises in which there were intervening words presented, assisted word learning and retention more than repeatedly retrieving and producing a word with no contextual change. | Plante, E., Ogilvie, T., Vance, R., Aguilar, J. M., Dailey, N. S., Meyers, C., Lieser, A.M., & Burton, R. (2014) [24] Manipulation of variability in the linguistic input—high versus low variability in conversational recast treatment. Country—United States | Overall gains were modest. Only those in high variability condition showed significant change in their use of target v’s control morphemes. High variability group also spontaneously produced significantly more inflected verb types. More children in the high variability condition showed a strong treatment effect. | |
Leonard, L. B., Karpicke, J., Deevy, P., Weber, C., Christ, S., Haebig, E., Souto, S., Keueser, J.B. & Krok, W. (2019) [25] Word learning using retrieval practice with contextual changes (RRCR) versus repeated study with no retrieval practice (RS). Country—United States | All but one child with DLD recalled more words in RRCR than in RS condition. The RRCR condition also resulted in better word meaning recall. DLD children showed weaker initial coding than TD children but this was no longer evident one week post-intervention. | Riches, N. G., Faragher, B., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006) [26] Modelling with either a noun/pronoun or noun-only frame in the subject/object slots (to generalise verbs from a non-transitive to a transitive frame). Country—United Kingdom | Generalisation of the novel verb to a transitive frame was not dependent on the frame used during the training sessions. | |
Leonard, L. B., Deevy, P., Karpicke, J. D., Christ, S., Weber, C., Kueser, J. B., & Haebig, E (2019) [27] Word learning using retrieval practice with contextual changes (RRCR) versus repeated study with no retrieval practice (RS). Country—United States | Children with DLD showed higher recall and greater recognition accuracy for adjectives learned in the RRCR condition than in the RS condition—with a large effect. There was no effect of condition for adjective recognition in the TD group. | |||
Smeets, D. J. H., van Dijken, M. J., & Bus, A. G. (2012) [28] Experiment 1: Word exposures through electronic stories (static and video, the latter with music and sounds). Experiment 2: Word exposures through electronic stories (static and video, both with and without music and sounds). Country—The Netherlands | Static books were more effective for word learning than those using video with music and sounds (based on a sentence completion task). Video and static stories were equally effective in children’s word-learning. Music and sounds interfered with children’s learning in both contexts. The interference was greater for those with lower levels of language. | |||
Kouri, T. A., & Winn, J. (2006) [29] Word exposures through story-telling and acting out. Story scripts given in sung or spoken form. Country—United States | No significant differences in the number of words understood in the sung or spoken conditions (Quick incidental learning). The second sung condition session showed greater spontaneous initiations of novel words. |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Frizelle, P.; McKean, C. Using Theory to Drive Intervention Efficacy: The Role of Dose Form in Interventions for Children with DLD. Children 2022, 9, 859. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9060859
Frizelle P, McKean C. Using Theory to Drive Intervention Efficacy: The Role of Dose Form in Interventions for Children with DLD. Children. 2022; 9(6):859. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9060859
Chicago/Turabian StyleFrizelle, Pauline, and Cristina McKean. 2022. "Using Theory to Drive Intervention Efficacy: The Role of Dose Form in Interventions for Children with DLD" Children 9, no. 6: 859. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9060859
APA StyleFrizelle, P., & McKean, C. (2022). Using Theory to Drive Intervention Efficacy: The Role of Dose Form in Interventions for Children with DLD. Children, 9(6), 859. https://doi.org/10.3390/children9060859