3.1. Subjective Experimental Data Analysis
3.1.1. Reliability Analysis
In order to ensure the reliability of the experimental data results, the reliability of the questionnaire was analyzed using the following formula
In Equation (1), is the reliability coefficient of the evaluation scale; n is the number of subjects, where n = 4, 4, 12; is the variance of item i; and is the variance of the total score of the scale.
The experimental data are shown in the
Table 6. We analyzed the reliability of the three dimensions, and the results were >0.7, so the questionnaire had reliability and could be used.
3.1.2. Data Comparison before and after Questionnaire
As shown in
Figure 3,
Figure 4 and
Figure 5, by comparing the data before and after the experiment, we found there was a great difference in the scores before and after the experiment. The scores before the experiment were significantly greater than those after the experiment, so it was proven that there was visual fatigue. After looking at the picture, the subjects had different degrees of fatigue in their eyes.
3.1.3. Relevance
First of all, we conducted a correlation analysis on the data in the questionnaire, trying to explore whether the lighting environment would affect the visual fatigue and whether it will affect the subject’s evaluation of the painting content. This paper used the Pearson correlation coefficient for research, and the formula was as follows:
where
n was the sample size, and
and
were the variable values of two variables, respectively. The analysis results are shown in
Table 7.
The Pearson correlation coefficient measures linear correlation. The greater the absolute value of the correlation coefficient, the stronger the correlation: the closer the correlation coefficient is to 1 or −1, the stronger the correlation, and the closer the correlation coefficient is to 0, the weaker the correlation is. When the correlation coefficient is 0.8–1.0, the correlation is very strong; 0.6–0.8 is a strong correlation; 0.4–0.6 is a moderate correlation; 0.2–0.4 is a weak correlation; 0.0–0.2 is a very weak correlation or no correlation.
Therefore, from
Table 7, we could see that the correlation coefficient between the subjects’ scores for the light environment and painting scores was 0.480, the significance probability value of the two tailed test was 0.000, and the double star sign indicated that the correlation coefficient of 0.480 was significant at the level of 0.01, which indicated that the correlation between the current aura and painting scores was very strong. In addition, the light environment was also relevant to visual fatigue.
First of all, we analyzed the light environment rating and painting rating. It was found that there was a significant correlation between the evaluation of the light environment and the painting. The subjects’ comfort in the light environment had a positive impact on the evaluation of the painting. In addition, there was a significant correlation between the evaluation of the light environment and the degree of fatigue after the experiment. The higher the subjects’ evaluation of the painting, the better the eye state and the lower the degree of visual fatigue. The correlation is shown in
Table 7. The sig values of the light environment and painting, light environment, and visual state after the experiment were less than 0.05, so the correlation coefficient between them was considered to be of significance. According to the Pearson correlation, we could see from the figure that the light environment had a significant correlation with the painting, and the light environment had a significant correlation with the eye state after the experiment. This shows that the light environment would affect the painting and eye state. In addition, the Pearson correlation coefficient between the painting and the light environment was 0.480, indicating that they were positively correlated, that is, the better the subjects evaluated the light environment, the more they liked the painting. The Pearson correlation coefficient between the light environment and the eye state after the experiment was 0.311, indicating that they were also positively correlated. The higher the subjects’ evaluation of the light environment, the better the eye state after the experiment. The subjects mainly rated the light environment from four aspects: light color comfort, relaxation under the light environment, stimulation of the light environment, and love for the light environment, and it was scored from the clarity, love, brightness, and color richness for the painting.
3.1.4. ANOVA Analysis
In the above work, we found that the higher the subjects’ rating of the lighting environment, the smaller their visual fatigue, and the higher their eye comfort. Next, we conducted more detailed research. We explored whether the changes in the light environment and color affected the subjects’ appreciation of the painting. We analyzed the four scores (clear/fuzzy, like/dislike, bright/dim, and rich/monotonic colors) of the painting through one-way ANOVA. The formula used is as follows:
where
is the number of sample observations at the
th level of factor A and the
th level of factor B;
is the mean value of the observed variable at the
th level of factor A;
is the mean value of the observed variable at the
th level of factor B;
is the
th observation value of the
th level of factor A and the
th observation value of the
th level of factor B; and
is the mean value of the observed variables of factors A and B at levels
and
, resectviely.
If the p value is greater than the significant level (0.05), the original hypothesis cannot be rejected, and it is considered that there is no significant difference on the factor level. If the p value is less than the significance level (0.05), the original hypothesis will be rejected and it is considered that there is a significant difference in the level of each factor.
We analyzed whether the different colors of the painting would affect the subjects’ subjective psychological impact on the painting, including clear/fuzzy, like/dislike, bright/dim, and rich/monotonous colors.
From
Table 8, we found that hue had no significant effect on clarity/blur, but it would affect the degree of subjects’ likes/dislikes.
When studying the influence of illuminance on the painting, we found that illuminance had no significant effect on the clarity/fuzziness, like/dislike, and rich/monotonous color observed by the subjects, but had a significant effect on the brightness/dimness of the painting.
Through the above data, we found that the external environment had little effect on the content of the painting itself, which proved that the content of the selected painting would not affect the accuracy of the experimental results.
3.1.5. Questionnaire Analysis
The degree of eye dryness, sleepiness, concentration, and blink frequency of eyes were investigated through a subjective questionnaire. The results are shown in
Figure 6 below.
On the whole, we found that the overall score of eye dryness was low, while the blink frequency was relatively high, indicating that the subjects’ eyes were dry and the blink frequency was relatively infrequent after the experiment. For the degree of eye dryness, it was obvious that 300 lx was the least likely to cause dry eyes, 50 lx was the most likely to cause dry eyes, and 150 lx was also prone to dry eyes. Judging from the degree of sleepiness and concentration, 300 lx did not easily cause sleepiness and distraction, while 150 lx and 50 lx caused sleepiness and concentration, which were in a low state. In terms of blink frequency, the warm tone scores were generally low, resulting in frequent blinking, discomfort in the subjects’ eyes, and the subjects commented that the warm tones were dazzling at 300 lx during the experiment. Therefore, we could judge that 300 lx was the least prone to eye fatigue, and the warm tone painting was more dazzling than the other tones, which was more likely to cause eye fatigue. In the case of 150 lx, the warm tone eye fatigue wa less.
3.2. Objective Data Analysis
Next, when analyzing and recording the change in pupil diameter using objective data, we analyzed the pupil change of each subject in each experiment. If the pupil change exceeded 0.4 mm, it was considered to produce visual fatigue. It was found that 98% of the sample data produced visual fatigue, so we could use the pupil diameter variation coefficient to judge the degree of visual fatigue. The variation coefficient of pupil diameter is shown in
Table 9.
Next, we conducted a one-way multivariate ANOVA to try to explore whether illumination and painting tone would have a significant impact on the subjects. The results are shown in
Table 10 that “illumination intensity”, “painting tone”, and “illumination intensity × painting tone” had significant differences (
p ≤ 0.05).
Next, we only analyzed the influence of light intensity on visual fatigue, and the results are shown in
Figure 7.
Among 50 lx, 150 lx, and 300 lx, the pupil diameter variation coefficient of 300 lx was the smallest, so the eye fatigue degree was the lowest at 300 lx. The pupil diameter variation coefficient of 50 lx was the largest and the fatigue degree was the highest. In addition, the data of 50 lx and 150 lx were similar and significantly larger than 300 lx, so it was obviously soft for the eyes in the light environment of 300 lx. This was consistent with our objective questionnaire data. The objective questionnaire data processing also showed a positive correlation between the light environment and eye comfort. With the improvement of the light environment, the subjects’ eye comfort was significantly enhanced. Therefore, in order to ensure a comfortable state for the eyes in the lighting design of the art museum, the illuminance of the light environment could be enhanced as much as possible while meeting relevant standards. While protecting the paintings of the art museum, it also provided a relatively good visual environment for viewers.
The influence of the tone of the painting on visual fatigue was analyzed separately, and the results are shown in
Figure 8.
The variation coefficient for pupil diameter for cold tone paintings was the smallest, so the cold tone would make the subjects feel more comfortable compared with other tones. The warm tone and middle tone paintings had little difference, and were significantly higher than the cold tone paintings, so the two paintings were more prone to visual fatigue. In addition, the warm tone paintings were the most eye stimulating of the three paintings. During the experiment, two subjects reported that they felt obvious dazzling when viewing warm tone paintings, and compared with the other color paintings, warm tone paintings increased the subjects’ blink frequency.
Through the analysis of the above two data, we found that the changes in painting tone and illumination for the light environment would have an impact on visual fatigue. Is there a role to alleviate or reduce visual fatigue under the joint action of illumination and painting tone? Next, we analyzed the degree of visual fatigue under the joint action of the two.
Considering the influence of both, it was necessary to build a general linear model with single factor variables, and the results are shown in
Figure 9. At the same time, we sorted out the variation coefficients of pupil diameter under various conditions and displayed them in the form of a line chart in
Figure 10.
As mentioned above, the illuminance and the tone of the painting affect the degree of visual fatigue. In this figure, blue is the variation coefficient of the pupil diameter of the cold tone paintings. We found that 150 lx produced visual fatigue relatively easier for cold tone paintings, and it was significantly higher than 50 lx and 300 lx. Therefore, 50 lx for cold tone paintings would reduce stimulation to the eyes. Red refers to warm tone paintings. We found that the coefficient of variation of the pupil diameter of the warm tone was relatively high under the three illuminations. Combined with the subjects’ observations that a warm tone would produce a dazzling phenomenon in the experiment, it was not difficult to find that the eye stimulation of warm tone paintings was significantly higher than that of other paintings. Under the three illuminations, 150 lx would relatively reduce the eye stimulation. Green was the middle tone. The middle tone painting was similar to the cold tone one. The pupil diameter variation coefficient under a certain illumination was significantly higher than the other two, while the pupil diameter variation coefficient under the other two illuminations was lower. For the middle tone painting, 50 lx was relatively high, which means that the middle tone painting was more suitable for higher illumination, and the pupil diameter variation coefficient was lower under 300 lx.
In 50 lx, the variation coefficient of the pupil diameter of the cold tone was the lowest, while the variation coefficient of the pupil diameter of the middle tone was the highest. Therefore, in 50 lx, the cold tone would be more comfortable, while the degree of visual fatigue caused by the middle tone was the highest. In 150 lx, the variation coefficient of the pupil diameter in the cold tone was the highest, while the variation coefficient of the pupil diameter in the middle tone was the lowest. This indicates that for these conditions, it would be more comfortable to view the middle tone, and the cold tone was the most likely to cause visual fatigue. In 300 lx, both middle tone and cold tone were relatively low, while warm tone was high, and warm color was the most likely to cause eye fatigue at this time. According to the above experimental results, when arranging the illuminance intensity of the art museum, we could give priority to 50 lx illuminance for cold tone paintings, 150 lx illuminance for warm tone paintings, and 300 lx illuminance for middle tone paintings.