Hand-Feel Touch Cues and Their Influences on Consumer Perception and Behavior with Respect to Food Products: A Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. A Sense of Touch
2.1. Concept and Terminology
2.2. Perception of Touch Cues
3. Factors Influencing Hand-Feel Touch Perception
3.1. Product-Related Factors
3.2. Consumer-Related Factors
3.2.1. Physiological and Demographic Factors
3.2.2. Psychological Factors
3.3. External Interface-Related Factors
3.3.1. Container, Tableware, and Cutlery Items
3.3.2. Packaging
4. Effects of Hand-Feel Touch Cues on Perceptions of Other Sensory Modules
4.1. Visual Perception
4.2. Auditory Perception
4.3. Olfactory Perception
4.4. Gustatory Perception
4.5. Oral Somatosensory Perception
5. Effects of Hand-Feel Touch Cues on Consumer Emotion and Behavior
5.1. Consumer Emotions
5.2. Consumer Purchase Behavior
6. Applications to Food and Beverage Industries and Future Research
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Meiselman, H.L.; Hirsh, E.S.; Popper, R.D. Sensory, hedonic and situational factors in food acceptance and consumption. In Food Acceptability; Thomson, D.M., Ed.; Elsevier: London, UK, 1988; pp. 77–87. [Google Scholar]
- Ng, M.; Chaya, C.; Hort, J. The influence of sensory and packaging cues on both liking and emotional, abstract and functional conceptualisations. Food Qual. Pref. 2013, 29, 146–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Spence, C. Sensory expectations based on product-extrinsic food cues: An interdisciplinary review of the empirical evidence and theoretical accounts. Food Qual. Pref. 2015, 40, 165–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- García-Segovia, P.; Harrington, R.J.; Seo, H.S. Influences of table setting and eating location on food acceptance and intake. Food Qual. Pref. 2015, 39, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peters-Texeira, A.; Badrie, N. Consumers’ perception of food packaging in Trinidad, West Indies and its related impact on food choices. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 2005, 29, 508–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koutsimanis, G.; Getter, K.; Behe, B.; Harte, J.; Almenar, E. Influences of packaging attributes on consumer purchase decisions for fresh produce. Appetite 2012, 59, 270–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Prendergast, P.G.; Pitt, L. Packaging, marketing, logistics and the environment: Are there trade-offs? Int. J. Phys. Distrib. Logist. Manag. 1996, 26, 60–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silayoi, P.; Speece, M. Packaging and purchase decisions: An exploratory study on the impact of involvement level and time pressure. Br. Food J. 2004, 106, 607–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Becker, L.; van Rompay, T.J.; Schifferstein, H.N.; Galetzka, M. Tough package, strong taste: The influence of packaging design on taste impressions and product evaluations. Food Qual. Pref. 2011, 22, 17–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lefebvre, A.; Bassereau, J.F.; Pensé-Lheritier, A.M.; Rivère, C.; Harris, N.; Duchamp, R. Recruitment and training of a sensory expert panel to measure the touch of beverage packages: Issue and methods employed. Food Qual. Pref. 2010, 21, 156–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCabe, D.B.; Nowlis, S.M. The effect of examining actual products or product descriptions on consumer preference. J. Consum. Psychol. 2003, 13, 431–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fenko, A.; Schifferstein, H.N.; Hekkert, P. Shifts in sensory dominance between various stages of user-product interactions. Appl. Ergon. 2010, 41, 34–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hultén, B.; Broweus, N.; Van Dijk, M. Sensory Marketing; Palgrave Macmillan: Hampshire, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Grohmann, B.; Spangenberg, E.R.; Sprott, D.E. The influence of tactile input on the evaluation of retail product offerings. J. Retail. 2007, 83, 237–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knapp, M.L.; Hall, J.A. Nonverbal Communication in Human Interaction, 4th ed.; Harcourt Brace College: Fort Worth, TX, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Hertenstein, M.J.; Keltner, D. Touch communicates distinct emotions. Emotion 2006, 6, 528–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Desmet, P.M. Three levels of product emotion. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Kansei Engineering and Emotion Research, Paris, France, 2–4 March 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Ortony, A.; Clore, G.L.; Collins, A. The Cognitive Structure of Emotions, 1st ed.; Cambridge University Press: Melbourne, Australia, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Simmonds, G.; Spence, C. Thinking inside the box: How seeing products on, or through, the packaging influences consumer perceptions and purchase behaviour. Food Qual. Pref. 2017, 62, 340–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Velasco, C.; Spence, C. Multisensory Packaging, 1st ed.; Palgrave Macmillan: Cham, Switzerland, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Dalenberg, J.R.; Gutjar, S.; Ter Horst, G.J.; de Graaf, K.; Renken, R.J.; Jager, G. Evoked emotions predict food choice. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e115388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gutjar, S.; Dalenberg, J.R.; de Graaf, C.; de Wijk, R.A.; Palascha, A.; Renken, R.J.; Jager, G. What reported food-evoked emotions may add: A model to predict consumer food choice. Food Qual. Pref. 2015, 45, 140–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samant, S.S.; Chapko, M.J.; Seo, H.S. Predicting consumer liking and preference based on emotional responses and sensory perception: A study with basic taste solutions. Food Res. Int. 2017, 100, 325–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilkinson, C.; Dijksterhuis, G.B.; Minekus, M. From food structure to texture. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 2000, 11, 442–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foster, K.D.; Grigor, J.M.V.; Cheong, J.N.; Yoo, M.J.Y.; Bronlund, J.E.; Morgenstern, M.P. The role of oral processing in dynamic sensory perception. J. Food Sci. 2011, 76, R49–R61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sherrington, C.S. Cutaneous sensations. In Textbook of Physiology; Schäfer, E.A., Young, J., Eds.; Pentland: Edinburgh, UK, 1900; Volume 2, pp. 920–1001. [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, J.J. Observations on active touch. Psychol. Rev. 1962, 69, 477–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loomis, J.M.; Lederman, S.J. Tactual Perception. In Handbook of Perception: Cognitive Processes and Performance; Boff, K.R., Kaufman, L., Thomas, J.P., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: New York, NY, USA, 1986; Volume 2, pp. 31.1–31.41. [Google Scholar]
- Gunther, E.; O’Modhrain, S. Cutaneous Grooves: Composing for the Sense of Touch. J. New Music Res. 2003, 32, 369–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rovan, J.; Hayward, V. Trends in Gestural Control of Music: Typology of Tactile Sounds and Their Synthesis in Gesture-Driven Computer Music Performance; Editions IRCAM: Paris, France, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Greenspan, J.D.; Bolanowski, S.J. Psychophysics of Tactile Perception. In Pain and Touch; Kruger, L., Ed.; Academic Press: Salt Lake, UT, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Lederman, S.J.; Klatzky, R.L. Haptic perception: A tutorial. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2009, 71, 1439–1459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kappers, A.M.; Bergmann Tiest, W.M. Tactile and haptic perceptual organization. In The Oxford Handbook of Perceptual Organization; Wagemans, J., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Proske, U.; Gandevia, S.C. The kinaesthetic senses. J. Physiol. 2009, 587, 4139–4146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lindstrom, M. Brand Sense: How to Build Powerful Brands through Touch, Taste, Smell, Sight & Sound; Kogan Page Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Lopetcharat, K.; McDaniel, M. Sensory Analysis of Foods. In Methods of Analysis of Food Components and Additives; Ötles, S., Ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2005; pp. 261–302. [Google Scholar]
- Srinivasan, M.A. Surface deflection of primate fingertip under line load. J. Biomech. 1989, 22, 343–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Srinivasan, M.A.; Dandekar, K. An investigation of the mechanics of tactile sense using two-dimensional models of the primate fingertip. J. Biomed. Eng. 1996, 118, 48–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johansson, R.S.; Landström, U.; Lundström, R. Responses of mechanoreceptive afferent units in the glabrous skin of the human hand to sinusoidal skin displacements. Brain Res. 1982, 244, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, K.O.; Lamb, G.D. Neural mechanisms of spatial tactile discrimination: Neural patterns evoked by braille-like dot patterns in the monkey. J. Physiol. 1981, 310, 117–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Löfvenberg, J.; Johansson, R.S. Regional differences and interindividual variability in sensitivity to vibration in the glabrous skin of the human hand. Brain Res. 1984, 301, 65–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blake, D.T.; Hsiao, S.S.; Johnson, K.O. Neural coding mechanisms in tactile pattern recognition: The relative contributions of slowly and rapidly adapting mechanoreceptors to perceived roughness. J. Neurosci. 1997, 17, 7480–7489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, K.O.; Phillips, J.R. Tactile spatial resolution I. Two-point discrimination, gap detection, grating resolution, and letter recognition. J. Neurophysiol. 1981, 46, 1177–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Westling, G.; Johansson, R.S. Responses in glabrous skin mechanoreceptors during precision grip in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 1987, 66, 128–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bensmaïa, S.J.; Hollins, M. Pacinian representations of fine surface texture. Percept. Psychophys. 2005, 67, 842–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Johnson, K.O. The roles and functions of cutaneous mechanoreceptors. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2001, 11, 455–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kutter, A.; Hanesch, C.; Rauh, C.; Delgado, A. Impact of proprioception and tactile sensations in the mouth on the perceived thickness of semi-solid foods. Food Qual. Pref. 2011, 22, 193–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knibestöl, M.; Vallbo, A.B. Single unit analysis of mechanoreceptor activity from the human glabrous skin. Acta Physiol. Scand. 1970, 80, 178–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Olausson, H.; Wessberg, J.; Kakuda, N. Tactile directional sensibility: Peripheral neural mechanisms in man. Brain Res. 2000, 866, 178–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edin, B.B.; Johansson, N. Skin strain patterns provide kinaesthetic information to the human central nervous system. J. Physiol. 1995, 487, 243–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proske, U.; Gandevia, S.C. The proprioceptive senses: Their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. Physiol. Rev. 2012, 92, 1651–1697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chambers, M.R.; Andres, K.H.; von Duering, M.; Iggo, A. The structure and function of the slowly adapting type II mechanoreceptor in hairy skin. Q. J. Exp. Physiol. Cogn. Med. Sci. 1972, 57, 417–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Edin, B.B. Quantitative analyses of dynamic strain sensitivity in human skin mechanoreceptors. J. Neurophysiol. 2004, 92, 3233–3243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stevens, J.C. Thermal sensibility. In The Psychology of Touch; Heller, M.A., Ed.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1991; pp. 61–90. [Google Scholar]
- Voets, T.; Droogmans, G.; Wissenbach, U.; Janssens, A.; Flockerzi, V.; Nilius, B. The principle of temperature-dependent gating in cold- and heat-sensitive TRP channels. Nature 2004, 430, 748–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schepers, R.J.; Ringkamp, M. Thermoreceptors and thermosensitive afferents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 34, 177–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pedersen, S.F.; Owsianik, G.; Nilius, B. TRP channels: An overview. Cell Calcium 2005, 38, 233–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- González-Ramírez, R.; Chen, Y.; Liedtke, W.B.; Morales-Lázaro, S.L. Chapter 8—TRP Channels and Pain. In Neurobiology of TRP Channels, 2nd ed.; Emir, R., Luti, T., Eds.; CRC Press/Taylor & Francis: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Eliav, E.; Gracely, R.H. Chapter 3—Measuring and assessing pain. In Orofacial Pain and Headache; Sharav, Y., Benoliel, R., Eds.; Elsevier: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2008; pp. 45–56. [Google Scholar]
- Gingras-Lessard, F.; Frasnelli, J. Basic physiology of the intranasal trigeminal system. In Flavour: From Food to Perception; Guichard, E., Salles, C., Morzel, M., Le Bon, A.M., Eds.; Wiley Blackwell: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Peier, A.M.; Reeve, A.J.; Andersson, D.A.; Moqrich, A.; Earley, T.J.; Hergarden, A.C.; Story, G.M.; Colley, S.; Hogenesch, J.B.; McIntyre, P.; et al. A heat-sensitive TRP channel expressed in keratinocytes. Science 2002, 296, 2046–2049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Güler, A.D.; Lee, H.; Iida, T.; Shimizu, I.; Tominaga, M.; Caterina, M. Heat-evoked activation of the ion channel, TRPV4. J. Neurosci. 2002, 22, 6408–6414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Watanabe, H.; Vriens, J.; Suh, S.H.; Benham, C.D.; Droogmans, G.; Nilius, B. Heat-evoked activation of TRPV4 channels in a HEK293 cell expression system and in native mouse aorta endothelial cells. J. Biol. Chem. 2002, 277, 47044–47051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Story, G.M.; Gereau, R.W. Numbing the senses: Role of TRPA1 in mechanical and cold sensation. Neuron 2006, 50, 177–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dubin, A.E.; Patapoutian, A. Nociceptors: The sensors of the pain pathway. J. Clin. Investig. 2010, 120, 3760–3772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Belmonte, C.; Viana, F. Nociceptor Responses. In Encyclopedia of Neuroscience; Squire, L.R., Ed.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2009; pp. 1191–1198. [Google Scholar]
- Constable, P.D.; Hinchcliff, K.W.; Done, S.H.; Grünberg, W. Veterinary Medicine, 11th ed.; Saunders Ltd.: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- National Research Council (US). Committee on Recognition and Alleviation of Pain in Laboratory Animals. Mechanisms of Pain. In Recognition and Alleviation of Pain in Laboratory Animals; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2009. Available online: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK32659/ (accessed on 8 June 2019).
- Ackerley, R.; Carlsson, I.; Wester, H.; Olausson, H.; Wasling, H.B. Touch perceptions across skin sites: Differences between sensitivity, direction discrimination and pleasantness. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winkelmann, R.K. The Mucocutaneous End-Organ. JAMA Dermatol. 1957, 76, 225–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vallbo, A.B.; Olausson, H.; Wessberg, J. Unmyelinated afferents constitute a second system coding tactile stimuli of the human hairy skin. J. Neurophysiol. 1999, 81, 2753–2763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vallbo, A.B.; Olausson, H.; Wessberg, J.; Kakuda, N. Receptive field characteristics of tactile units with myelinated afferents in hairy skin of human subjects. J. Physiol. 1995, 483, 783–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vallbo, A.; Olausson, H.; Wessberg, J.; Norrsell, U. A system of unmyelinated afferents for innocuous mechanoreception in the human skin. Brain Res. 1993, 628, 301–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guinard, J.X.; Mazzucchelli, R. The sensory perception of texture and mouthfeel. Trends Food Sci. Technol. 1996, 7, 213–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weinstein, S. Intensive and extensive aspects of tactile sensitivity as a function of body part, sex, and laterality. In The Skin Senses; Kenshalo, D.R., Charles, C., Eds.; Thomas Publishers: Springfield, IL, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Aktar, T.; Chen, J.; Ettelaie, R.; Holmes, M. Evaluation of the sensory correlation between touch sensitivity and the capacity to discriminate viscosity. J. Sens. Stud. 2015, 30, 98–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hamalainen, H.; Jarvilehto, T. Peripheral neural basis of tactile sensations in man: 1. Effect of frequency and probe area on sensations elicited by single mechanical pulses on hairy and glabrous skin of the hand. Brain Res. 1981, 219, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jarvilehto, T.; Hamalainen, H.; Soininen, K. Peripheral neural basis of tactile sensations in man: 11. Characteristics of human mechanoceptors in the hairy skin and correlations of their activity with tactile sensations. Brain Res. 1981, 219, 13–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hamalainen, H.A.; Warren, S.; Gardner, E.P. Differential Sensitivity to Airpuffs on Human Hairy and Glabrous Skin. Somatosens. Res. 1985, 2, 281–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Engelen, L.; Van der Bilt, A. Oral physiology and texture perception of semisolids. J. Texture Stud. 2008, 39, 83–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hollins, M.; Bensmaia, S.; Karlof, K.; Young, F. Individual differences in perceptual space for tactile textures: Evidence from multidimensional scaling. Percept. Psychophys. 2000, 62, 1534–1544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schindler, W.D.; Hauser, P.J. 3—Softening finishes. In Chemical Finishing of Textiles, 10th ed.; Schindler, W.D., Hauser, P.J., Eds.; Woodhead Publishing: Cambridge, UK, 2004; pp. 29–42. [Google Scholar]
- Szczesniak, A.S.; Bourne, M.C. Sensory Evaluation of Food Firmness. J. Texture Stud. 1969, 1, 52–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barrett, D.M.; Garcia, E.; Wayne, J. Textural modification of processing tomatoes. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 1998, 38, 173–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ranatunga, C.L.; Jayaweera, H.H.; Ariyaratne, T.R. Evaluation of finger-feel firmness as a subjective measurement of tomato quality degradation at the retail market. Trop. Agric. Res. 2008, 20, 134–142. [Google Scholar]
- Spence, C.; Hobkinson, C.; Gallace, A.; Piqueras-Fiszman, B. A touch of gastronomy. Flavour 2013, 2, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Otegbayo, B.; Aina, J.; Sakyi-Dawson, E.; Bokanga, M.; Asiedu, R. Sensory texture profiling and development of standard rating scales for pounded yam. J. Texture Stud. 2005, 36, 478–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pereira, R.B.; Bennett, R.J. In-hand sensory evaluation of textural characteristics in model processed cheese analogues. J. Texture Stud. 2002, 33, 255–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sudha, M.L.; Rajeswari, G.; Rao, G.V. Influence of defatted soy flour and whey protein concentrate on dough rheological characteristics and quality of instant vermicelli. J. Texture Stud. 2011, 42, 72–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smitha, S.; Rajiv, J.; Begum, K.; Indrani, D. Effect of hydrocolloids on rheological, microstructural and quality characteristics of parotta—An unleavened Indian flat bread. J. Texture Stud. 2008, 39, 267–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ukpabi, U.J.; Omodamiro, R.M.; Ikeorgu, J.G.; Asiedu, R. Sensory evaluation of amala from improved water yam (Dioscorea alata) genotypes in Nigeria. Afr. J. Biotechnol. 2008, 7, 1134–1138. [Google Scholar]
- Shittu, T.A.; Adedokun, I.I. Comparative evaluation of the functional and sensory characteristics of three traditional fermented cassava products. J. Nat. Sci. Eng. Technol. 2010, 9, 106–116. [Google Scholar]
- Saxena, V.P. Temperature distribution in human skin and subdermal tissues. J. Theor. Biol. 1983, 102, 277–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weitz, J. Vibratory sensitivity as a function of skin temperature. J. Exp. Psychol. 1941, 28, 21–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, B.G. The effect of skin temperature on vibrotactile sensitivity. Percept. Psychophys. 1977, 21, 243–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bolanowski, S.J.; Verrillo, R.T. Temperature and criterion effects in a somatosensory subsystem: A neurophysiological and psycho-physical study. J. Neurophysiol. 1982, 48, 836–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Green, B.G.; Lederman, S.L.; Stevens, J.C. The effect of skin temperature on the perception of roughness. Sens. Process. 1979, 3, 327–333. [Google Scholar]
- Provins, K.A.; Morton, R. Tactile discrimination and skin temperature. J. Appl. Physiol. 1960, 15, 155–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thornbury, J.M.; Mistretta, C.M. Tactile sensitivity as a function of age. J. Gerontol. 1981, 36, 34–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kenshalo, D.R. Somesthetic Sensitivity in young and dlderly humans. J. Gerontol. 1986, 41, 732–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Desrosiers, J.; Hebert, R.; Bravo, G.; Rochette, A. Age-related changes in upper extremity performance of elderly people: A longitudinal study. Exp. Gerontol. 1999, 34, 393–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaluga, E.; Kostiukow, A.; Samborski, W.; Rostkowska, E. Tactile sensitivity on the hands skin in rheumatic patients. Adv. Dermatol. Allergol. 2014, 31, 139–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Verrillo, R.T. Age Related Changes in the Sensitivity to Vibration. J. Gerontol. 1980, 35, 185–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Verrillo, R.T.; Bolanowski, S.J.; Gescheider, G.A. Effect of aging on the subjective magnitude of vibration. Somatosens. Mot. Res. 2002, 19, 238–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stevens, J.C. Aging and spatial acuity of touch. J. Gerontol. 1992, 47, 35–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowden, J.L.; McNulty, P.A. Age-related changes in cutaneous sensations in the healthy human hand. Age 2013, 35, 1077–1089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Murata, J.; Murata, S.; Hiroshige, J.; Ohtao, H.; Horie, J.; Kai, Y. The Influence of Age-related Changes in Tactile Sensibility and Muscular Strength on Hand Function in Older Adult Females. Int. J. Gerontol. 2010, 4, 180–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lautenbacher, S.; Strian, F. Similarities in age differences in heat pain perception and thermal sensitivity. Funct. Neurol. 1991, 6, 129–135. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Gibson, S.J.; Farrell, M. A Review of Age Differences in the Neurophysiology of Nociception and the Perceptual Experience of Pain. Clin. J. Pain 2004, 20, 227–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Guergova, S.; Dufour, A. Thermal sensitivity in the elderly: A review. Ageing Res. Rev. 2011, 10, 80–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Al-Nuaimi, Y.; Sherratt, M.J.; Griffiths, C.E. Skin health in older age. Maturitas 2014, 79, 256–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kozlowska, A. Studying tactile sensitivity—Population approach. Anthropol. Rev. 1998, 61, 3–30. [Google Scholar]
- Goff, G.D.; Rosner, B.S.; Detre, T.; Kennard, D. Vibration perception in normal man and medical patients. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 1965, 28, 503–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pickering, G.; Jourdan, D.; Eschalier, A.; Dubray, C. Impact of Age, Gender and Cognitive Functioning on Pain Perception. Gerontology 2002, 48, 112–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blankenburg, M.; Boekens, H.; Hechler, T.; Maier, C.; Krumova, E.; Scherens, A.; Magerl, W.; Aksu, F.; Zernikow, B. Reference values for quantitative sensory testing in children and adolescents: Developmental and gender differences of somatosensory perception. PAIN 2010, 419, 76–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tsay, A.; Allen, T.J.; Proske, U.; Giummarra, M.J. Sensing the body in chronic pain: A review of psychophysical studies implicating altered body representation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015, 52, 221–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Frohlich, P.F.; Meston, C.M. Tactile Sensitivity in Women with Sexual Arousal Disorder. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2005, 34, 207–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barbacena, I.L.; Lima, A.C.; Barros, A.T.; Freire, R.C.; Pereira, J.R. Comparative Analysis of Tactile Sensitivity between Blind, Deaf and Unimpaired People. Int. J. Adv. Media Commun. 2009, 3, 249–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldreich, D.; Kanics, I.M. Tactile Acuity is Enhanced in Blindness. J. Neurosci. 2003, 23, 3439–3445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Goldreich, D.; Kanics, I.M. Performance of blind and sighted humans on a tactile grating detection task. Percept. Psychophys. 2006, 68, 1363–1371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Stevens, J.C.; Foulke, E.; Patterson, M.Q. Tactile Acuity, Aging, and Braille Reading in Long-Term Blindness. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 1996, 2, 91–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Legge, G.E.; Madison, C.; Vaughn, B.N.; Cheong, A.M.; Miller, J.C. Retention of high tactile acuity throughout the life span in blindness. Percept. Psychophys. 2008, 70, 1471–1488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kelley, N.J.; Schmeichel, B.J. The effects of negative emotions on sensory perception: Fear but not anger decreases tactile sensitivity. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ashkinazi, I.Y.; Vershinina, E.A. Pain Sensitivity in Chronic Psychoemotional Stress in Humans. Neurosci. Behav. Physiol. 1999, 29, 333–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Levenson, R.W.; Carstensen, L.L.; Friesen, W.V.; Ekman, P. Emotion, physiology, and expression in old age. Psychol. Aging 1991, 6, 28–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Peck, J.; Childers, T.L. Individual Differences in Haptic Information Processing: The “Need for Touch” Scale. J. Consum. Res. 2003, 30, 430–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peck, J.; Wiggins, J. It just feels good: Consumers’ affective response to touch and its influence on persuasion. J. Mark. 2006, 70, 56–69. [Google Scholar]
- Ramanathan, S.; Menon, G. Don’t Know Why, But I Had This Craving: Goal-Dependent Automaticity in Impulsive Decisions; Working Paper No. 11-01; Stern School of Business New York University: New York, NY, USA, 2001; Available online: https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/Don't-Know-Why%2C-But-I-Had-This-Craving%3A-IN-Ramanathan-Menon/da351b0075038dd5cb1840e7be4b44b782ea09cf (accessed on 7 January 2019).
- Peck, J.; Childers, T.L. If I touch it I have to have it: Individual and environmental influences on impulse purchasing. J. Bus. Res. 2006, 59, 765–769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krishna, A.; Morrin, M. Does touch affect taste? The perceptual transfer of product container haptic cues. J. Consum. Res. 2008, 34, 807–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rogers, S.J.; Ozonoff, S. Annotation: What do we know about sensory dysfunction in autism? A critical review of the empirical evidence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2005, 46, 1255–1268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Riquelme, I.; Hatem, S.M.; Montoya, P. Abnormal Pressure Pain, Touch Sensitivity, Proprioception, and Manual Dexterity in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Neural Plast. 2016, 2016, 1723401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cascio, C.J.; Lorenzi, J.; Baranek, G.T. Self-reported pleasantness ratings and examiner-coded defensiveness in response to touch in children with ASD: Effects of stimulus material and bodily location. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016, 46, 1528–1537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liss, M.; Mailloux, J.; Erchull, M.J. The relationships between sensory processing sensitivity, alexithymia, autism, depression, and anxiety. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2000, 45, 255–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sivik, T. Alexithymia and hypersensitivity to touch and palpation. Integr. Physiol. Behav. Sci. 1993, 28, 130–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nyklíček, I.; Vingerhoets, A.J. Alexithymia is associated with low tolerance to experimental painful stimulation. Pain 2000, 85, 471–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Harrar, V.; Spence, C. The taste of cutlery: How the taste of food is affected by the weight, size, shape, and colour of the cutlery used to eat it. Flavour 2013, 2, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maggioni, E.; Risso, P.; Olivero, N.; Gallace, A. The effect of a container’s weight on the perception of mineral water. J. Sens. Stud. 2015, 30, 395–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Harrar, V.; Alcaide, J.; Spence, C. Does the weight of the dish influence our perception of food? Food Qual. Pref. 2011, 22, 753–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Spence, C. The weight of the container influences expected satiety, perceived density, and subsequent expected fullness. Appetite 2012, 58, 559–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schifferstein, H.N. The drinking experience: Cup or content? Food Qual. Pref. 2009, 20, 268–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tu, Y.; Yang, Z.; Ma, C. Touching tastes: The haptic perception transfer of liquid food packaging materials. Food Qual. Pref. 2015, 39, 124–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheskin, L. How to Predict What People Will Buy; Liveright: New York, NY, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
- Spence, C.; Gallace, A. Multisensory design: Reaching out to touch the consumer. Psychol. Market. 2011, 28, 267–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, C. Crossmodal correspondences: A tutorial review. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2011, 73, 971–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Pan, N. Quantification and evaluation of human tactile sense towards fabrics. J. Des. Nat. 2007, 1, 48–60. [Google Scholar] [Green Version]
- Liao, X.; Hu, J.; Li, Y.; Li, Q.; Wu, X. A Review on Fabric Smoothness-roughness Sensation Studies. J. Fiber Bioeng. Inf. 2011, 4, 105–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Young, K. The Rise of Green Consumerism: What do Brands Need to Know? Available online: https://blog.globalwebindex.com/chart-of-the-week/green-consumerism/ (accessed on 29 May 2019).
- Lanzini, P.; Testa, F.; Iraldo, F. Factors affecting drivers’ willingness to pay for biofuels: The case of Italy. J. Clean. Prod. 2016, 112, 2684–2692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kucher, A.; Heldak, M.; Kucher, L.; Raszka, B. Factors Forming the Consumers’ Willingness to Pay a Price Premium for Ecological Goods in Ukraine. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, C. Multisensory Packaging Design: Color, Shape, Texture, Sound, and Smell. In Integrating the Packaging and Product Experience in Food and Beverages: A Road-Map to Consumer Satisfaction; Burgess, P., Ed.; Woodhead Publishing: Duxford, UK, 2016; pp. 1–22. [Google Scholar]
- Whitaker, T.A.; Simões-Franklin, C.; Newell, F.N. Vision and touch: Independent or integrated systems for the perception of texture? Brain Res. 2008, 1242, 59–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lederman, S.J.; Thorne, G.; Jones, B. Perception of texture by vision and touch: Multidimensionality and intersensory integration. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1986, 12, 169–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amedi, A.; von Kriegstein, K.; van Atteveldt, N.M.; Beauchamp, M.S.; Naumer, M.J. Functional imaging of human crossmodal identification and object recognition. Exp. Brain Res. 2005, 166, 559–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Stilla, R.; Sathian, K. Selective visuo-haptic processing of shape and texture. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2008, 29, 1123–1138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prather, S.C.; Votaw, J.R.; Sathian, K. Task-specific recruitment of dorsal and ventral visual areas during tactile perception. Neuropsychologia 2004, 42, 1079–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sathian, K.; Lacey, S.; Stilla, R.; Gibson, G.O.; Deshpande, G.; Hu, X.; LaConte, S.; Glielmi, C. Dual pathways for haptic and visual perception of spatial and texture information. Neuroimage 2011, 57, 462–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lederman, S.J.; Abbott, S.G. Texture perception: Studies of intersensory organization using a discrepancy paradigm, and visual versus tactual psychophysics. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1981, 7, 902–915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schifferstein, H.N. The perceived importance of sensory modalities in product usage: A study of self-reports. Acta Psychol. 2006, 121, 41–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ward, J.; Banissy, M.J.; Jonas, C.N. Haptic perception and synaesthesia. In Human Haptic Perception; Grunewald, M., Ed.; Birkhaüser: Basel, Switzerland, 2008; pp. 259–265. [Google Scholar]
- Slobodenyuk, N.; Jraissati, Y.; Kanso, A.; Ghanem, L.; Elhajj, I. Cross-Modal Associations between Color and Haptics. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2015, 77, 1379–1395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Martino, G.; Marks, L.E. Cross-modal interaction between vision and touch: The role of synesthetic correspondence. Perception 2000, 29, 745–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Spence, C. The weight of the bottle as a possible extrinsic cue with which to estimate the price (and quality) of the wine? Observed correlations. Food Qual. Pref. 2012, 25, 41–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ludwig, V.U.; Simner, J. What colour does that feel? Tactile–visual mapping and the development of cross-modality. Cortex 2013, 49, 1089–1099. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schurmann, M.; Caetano, G.; Hlushchuk, Y.; Jousmaki, V.; Hari, R. Touch activates human auditory cortex. Neuroimage 2006, 30, 1325–1331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beauchamp, M.S.; Yasar, N.E.; Frye, R.E.; Ro, T. Touch, sound and vision in human superior temporal sulcus. Neuroimage 2008, 41, 1011–1020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kassuba, T.; Klinge, C.; Hölig, C.; Menz, M.M.; Ptito, M.; Röder, B.; Siebner, H.R. The left fusiform gyrus hosts trisensory representations of manipulable objects. Neuroimage 2011, 56, 1566–1577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sapir, E. A study in phonetic symbolism. J. Exp. Psychol. 1929, 12, 225–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newman, S.S. Further experiments in phonetic symbolism. Am. J. Psychol. 1933, 45, 53–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guest, S.; Catmur, C.; Lloyd, D.; Spence, C. Audiotactile interactions in roughness perception. Exp. Brain Res. 2002, 146, 161–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peeva, D.; Baird, B.; Izmirli, O.; Blevins, D. Haptic and sound correlations: Pitch, loudness and texture. In Proceedings of the Eighth International Conference on Information Visualisation (IV’04), London, UK, 14–16 July 2004; The Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2004; pp. 659–664. [Google Scholar]
- Eitan, Z.; Rothschild, I. How music touches: Musical parameters and listeners’ audio-tactile metaphorical mappings. Psychol. Music 2010, 39, 449–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eitan, Z.; Timmers, R. Beethoven’s last piano sonata and those who follow crocodiles: Cross-domain mappings of auditory pitch in a musical context. Cognition 2010, 114, 405–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jousmäki, V.; Hari, R. Parchment-skin illusion: Sound-biased touch. Curr. Biol. 1998, 8, R190–R191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Velasco, C.; Jones, R.; King, S.; Spence, C. “Hot or cold?” On the informative value of auditory cues in the perception of the temperature of a beverage. In ((( ABA ))) Audio Branding Academy Yearbook 2012/2013; Bronner, K., Hirt, R., Ringe, C., Eds.; Nomos: Baden-Baden, Germany, 2013; pp. 177–187. [Google Scholar]
- Spence, C.; Wang, Q. Sensory expectations elicited by the sounds of opening the packaging and pouring a beverage. Flavour 2015, 4, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Slocombe, B.G.; Carmichael, D.A.; Simner, J. Cross-modal tactile-taste interactions in food evaluations. Neuropsychologia 2016, 88, 58–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cliff, M.A. Influence of wine glass shape on perceived aroma and colour intensity in wines. J. Wine Res. 2001, 12, 39–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delwiche, J.F.; Pelchat, M.L. Influence of glass shape on wine aroma. J. Sens. Stud. 2002, 17, 19–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hummel, T.; Delwiche, J.F.; Schmidt, C.; Hüttenbrink, K.B. Effects of the form of glasses on the perception of wine flavors: A study in untrained subjects. Appetite 2003, 41, 197–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Demattè, M.L.; Sanabria, D.; Sugarman, R.; Spence, C. Cross-modal interactions between olfaction and touch. Chem. Sens. 2006, 31, 291–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vilanova, M.; Vidal, P.; Cortés, S. Effect of the glass shape on flavor perception of “toasted wine” from Ribeiro (NW Spain). J. Sens. Stud. 2008, 23, 114–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krishna, A.; Elder, R.S.; Caldara, C. Feminine to smell but masculine to touch? Multisensory congruence and its effect on the aesthetic experience. J. Consum. Psychol. 2010, 20, 410–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gatti, E.; Bordegoni, M.; Spence, C. Investigating the influence of colour, weight, and fragrance intensity on the perception of liquid bath soap: An experimental study. Food Qual. Pref. 2014, 31, 56–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cavazzana, A.; Larsson, M.; Hoffmann, E.; Hummel, T. The vessel’s shape influences the smell and taste of cola. Food Qual. Pref. 2017, 59, 8–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mirabito, A.; Oliphant, M.; Van Doorn, G.; Spence, C. Glass shape affects the perceived taste of beer. Food Qual. Pref. 2017, 62, 257–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spence, C.; Wan, X. Beverage perception and consumption: The influence of the container on the perception of the contents. Food Qual. Pref. 2015, 39, 131–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Russell, K.; Zivanovic, S.; Morris, W.C.; Penfield, M.; Weiss, J. The effect of glass shape on the concentration of polyphenolic compounds and perception of Merlot wine. J. Food Qual. 2005, 28, 377–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raudenbush, B.; Meyer, W.; Eppich, W.; Corley, N.; Petterson, S. Ratings of pleasantness and intensity for beverages served in containers congruent and incongruent with expectancy. Percept. Mot. Skills 2002, 94, 671–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cardello, A.V.; Maller, O. Acceptability of Water, Selected Beverages and Foods as a Function of Serving Temperature. J. Food Sci. 1982, 47, 1549–1552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bult, J.H.; de Wijk, R.A.; Hummel, T. Investigations on multimodal sensory integration: Texture, taste, and ortho- and retronasal olfactory stimuli in concert. Neurosci. Lett. 2007, 411, 6–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roudnitzky, N.; Bult, J.H.; de Wijk, R.A.; Reden, J.; Schuster, B.; Hummel, T. Investigation of interactions between texture and ortho- and retronasal olfactory stimuli using psychophysical and electrophysiological approaches. Behav. Brain Res. 2011, 216, 109–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Iannilli, E.; Bult, J.H.; Roudnitzky, N.; Gerber, J.; de Wijk, R.A.; Hummel, T. Oral texture influences the neural processing of ortho- and retronasal odors in humans. Brain Res. 2014, 1587, 77–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zhou, X.; Wan, X.; Mu, B.; Du, D.; Spence, C. Crossmodal associations and subjective ratings of Asian noodles and the impact of the receptacle. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 41, 141–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barnett, A.; Velasco, C.; Spence, C. Bottled vs. canned beer: Do they really taste different? Beverages 2016, 2, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Rompay, T.J.; Kramer, L.M.; Saakes, D. The sweetest punch: Effects of 3D-printed surface textures and graphic design on ice-cream evaluation. Food Qual. Pref. 2018, 68, 198–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van Rompay, T.J.; Groothedde, S. The taste of touch: Enhancing saltiness impressions through surface texture design. Food Qual. Pref. 2019, 73, 248–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Rompay, T.J.; Finger, F.; Saakes, D.; Fenko, A. “See me, feel me”: Effects of 3D-printed surface patterns on beverage evaluation. Food Qual. Pref. 2017, 62, 332–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Spence, C. Do the material properties of cutlery affect the perception of the food you eat? An exploratory study. J. Sens. Stud. 2011, 26, 358–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Laughlin, Z.; Miodownik, M.; Spence, C. Tasting spoons: Assessing how the material of a spoon affects the taste of the food. Food Qual. Pref. 2012, 24, 24–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aktar, T.; Chen, J.; Ettelaie, R.; Holmes, M. Tactile Sensitivity and Capability of Soft-Solid Texture Discrimination. J. Texture Stud. 2015, 46, 429–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Howes, P.D.; Wongsriruksa, S.; Laughlin, Z.; Witchel, H.J.; Miodownik, M. The Perception of Materials through Oral Sensation. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e105035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bergmann Tiest, W.M. Tactual perception of material properties. Vis. Res. 2010, 50, 2775–2782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Barnett-Cowan, M. An illusion you can sink your teeth into: Haptic cues modulate the perceived freshness and crispness of pretzels. Perception 2010, 39, 1684–1686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Spence, C. The influence of the feel of product packaging on the perception of the oral-somatosensory texture of food. Food Qual. Pref. 2012, 26, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biggs, L.; Juravle, G.; Spence, C. Haptic exploration of plateware alters the perceived texture and taste of food. Food Qual. Pref. 2016, 50, 129–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mielby, L.A.; Wang, Q.J.; Jensen, S.; Bertelsen, A.S.; Kidmose, U.; Spence, C.; Byrne, D.V. See, Feel, Taste: The Influence of Receptacle Colour and Weight on the Evaluation of Flavoured Carbonated Beverages. Foods 2018, 7, 119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Risso, P.; Maggioni, E.; Etzi, R.; Gallace, A. The Effect of the Tactile Attributes of a Container on Mineral Water Perception. Beverages 2019, 5, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zampini, M.; Mawhinney, S.; Spence, C. Tactile perception of the roughness of the end of a tool: What role does tool handle roughness play? Neurosci. Lett. 2006, 400, 235–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McDaniel, C.; Baker, R.C. Convenience food packaging and the perception of product quality: What does “hard-to-open” mean to consumers? J. Market. 1977, 41, 57–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ares, G.; Deliza, R. Studying the influence of package shape and colour on consumer expectations of milk desserts using word association and conjoint analysis. Food Qual. Pref. 2010, 21, 930–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hutmacher, F.; Kuhbandner, C. Long-Term Memory for Haptically Explored Objects: Fidelity, Durability, Incidental Encoding, and Cross-Modal Transfer. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 29, 2031–2038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zajonc, R.B. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1968, 9, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bornstein, R.F. Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychol. Bull. 1989, 106, 265–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ballesteros, S.; Reales, J.M.; Manga, D. Implicit and explicit memory for familiar and novel objects presented to touch. Psicothema 1999, 11, 785–800. [Google Scholar]
- Jakesch, M.; Carbon, C.C. The Mere Exposure Effect in the Domain of Haptics. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e31215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, X.; Barnes, C.J.; Childs, T.H.; Henson, B.; Shao, F. Materials’ tactile testing and characterisation for consumer products’ affective packaging design. Mater. Des. 2009, 30, 4299–4310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klatzky, R.L.; Lederman, S.J. Stages of Manual Exploration in Haptic Object Identification. Percept. Psychophys. 1992, 52, 661–670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Klatzky, R.L.; Lederman, S.J. Toward a Computational Model of Constraint-Driven Exploration and Haptic Object Identification. Perception 1993, 22, 597–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peck, J.; Childers, T.L. Self-report and behavioral measures in product evaluation and haptic information: Is what I say how I feel? Adv. Consum. Res. 2004, 32, 247. [Google Scholar]
- Peck, J.; Childers, T.L. To Have and to Hold: The Influence of Haptic Information on Product Judgments. J. Mark. 2003, 67, 35–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peck, J.; Shu, S.B. The Effect of Mere Touch on Perceived Ownership. J. Consum. Res. 2009, 36, 434–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guinard, J.X.; Uotoni, B.; Schlich, P. Internal and external mapping of preferences for commercial lager beers: Comparison of hedonic ratings by consumers blind versus with knowledge of brand and price. Food Qual. Pref. 2001, 12, 243–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deliza, R.; MacFie, H.J. The generation of sensory expectation by external cues and its effect on sensory perception and hedonic ratings: A review. J. Sens. Stud. 1996, 11, 103–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guest, S.; Spence, C. Tactile dominance in speeded discrimination of textures. Exp. Brain Res. 2003, 150, 201–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kampfer, K.; Leischnig, A.; Ivens, B.S.; Spence, C. Touch-flavor transference: Assessing the effect of packaging weight on gustatory evaluations, desire for food and beverages, and willingness to pay. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e016121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ngo, M.K.; Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Spence, C. On the colour and shape of still and sparkling water: Insights from online and laboratory-based testing. Food Qual. Pref. 2012, 24, 240–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bartoshuk, L.M. Comparing sensory experiences across individuals: Recent psychophysical advances illuminate genetic variation in taste perception. Chem. Sens. 2000, 25, 447–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Types of Visual Cues | Presentation Types of Visual Cues | Types of Touch Cues | Presentation Types of Touch Cues | Key Findings | References |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hue (black/white) | Colored squares (via computer) | Vibrotactile | Computer-controlled shaker | Low-frequency vibrations were associated with a black hue; high-frequency vibrations were associated with a white hue | Martino & Marks [162] |
Hue (red/white wine) | Wine color | Weight | Wine bottles | Red wine bottles were rated heavier compared to white wine | Piqueras-Fiszman & Spence [163] |
Luminance, chroma, hue | Color wheel (via computer) | Temperature, roughness, vibrotactile, pressure | Sandpaper (roughness), solenoid tapper (vibrotactile) | Low color luminance was associated with roughness and high pressure to skin | Ward et al. [160] |
Luminance, chroma, hue | Color wheel (via computer) | Hardness/softness, pointed/roundness, roughness/smoothness | Foam cubes (hard-soft), wooden 3-D shapes (pointed-round), sandpaper-covered flat surfaces (rough-smooth) | High luminance correlated with high softness and roundness; high chroma correlated with smoothness and softness; specific color hues were associated with certain tactile sensations | Ludwig & Simner [164] |
Luminance, chroma, hue | Color wheel (via computer) | Hardness, roughness, heaviness, elasticity, adhesiveness | Programmed haptic device (SensAble PHANTOM OMNI®) | High color luminance was associated with high smoothness, high softness, high elasticity, and low adhesion | Slobodenyuk et al. [161] |
Types of Auditory Cues | Presentation Types of Auditory Cues | Types of Touch Cues | Presentation Types of Touch Cues | Key Findings | References |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Loudness, pitch | (Modified) sounds of participants rubbing their own palms together played back to the participants | Roughness/moistness, dryness/smoothness | Participants’ own skin (participants rubbing their palms together) | Increased sound intensity and high pitch were more associated with higher smoothness/dryness of human palmar skin | Jousmäki & Hari [174] |
Loudness, pitch | (Modified) sounds of participants touching the touch stimuli played back to the participants | Roughness | Abrasive closed-coat silicon carbide papers attached on plastic discs | Decreased sound intensity and lower pitch increased the perception of tactile smoothness | Guest et al. [170] |
Loudness, auditory associations | Recorded sounds | Roughness | Programmed haptic device (SensAble PHANTOM) | Rougher textures were correlated with increased sound intensity; smoother textures were more associated with decreased sound intensity | Peeva et al. [171] |
Loudness, pitch, sound type (violin vs. flute), auditory associations | Recorded sounds | Sharpness/bluntness, roughness/smoothness, hardness/softness, weight, temperature | Touch-related terms (i.e., no physical touch stimuli) | High smoothness and softness can be associated with low sound intensity, low pitch, and flute sound (compared to violin), while high sharpness can be associated with high sound intensity and flute sound (compared to violin) | Eitan & Rothschild [172] |
Pitch, auditory associations | Daniel Barenhoim’s recording of Beethoven’s piano sonata (2nd movement, opus 111) | Temperature, hardness/softness, weight, roughness/softness, sharpness/bluntness, size (small/large), thinness/thickness | Touch-related terms (i.e., no physical touch stimuli) | High pitch was more associated with “small”, “thin”, “sharp”, “smooth”; low pitch was more associated with “large”, “thick”, “heavy”, “blunt”, “rough” | Eitan & Timmers (Experiment 2) [173] |
Types of Olfactory Cues | Presentation Types of Olfactory Cues | Types of Touch Cues | Presentation Types of Touch Cues | Key Findings | References |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Orthonasal odor | Wine (red & white); Overall aroma intensity, fruity aroma intensity | Shape | Wine glasses | Aroma intensities were rated higher when wines were served in bowl-shaped glass than in tulip-shaped glass (in white and red wines) | Cliff [178] |
Retronasal odor | Hot chocolate, beer, & orange juice; Overall flavor intensity, overall pleasantness | Shape | Receptacle (bottles vs. cups vs. glasses) | Hot chocolate, beer, and orange juice were rated to be most pleasant when consumed from bottles (compared to glasses and cups) | Raudenbush et al. [189] |
Orthonasal odor | Wine (red); Overall aroma intensity, fruity aroma intensity, vinegar aroma intensity, oak/woodiness aroma intensity, mustiness aroma intensity | Shape | Wine glasses | Odor intensity of red wine samples were rated as less intense when presented in tapered bulb-shaped glasses than open bulb-shaped and square-shaped glasses | Delwiche & Pelchat [179] |
Retronasal odor | Wine (red & white); Overall aroma intensity, overall pleasantness | Shape | Wine glasses | Odor intensity of red and white wine samples were rated as most intense when presented in bulbous-shaped glass than tulip-shaped and beaker-shaped glasses | Hummel et al. [180] |
Orthonasal odor | Lemon & animal odors | Roughness/softness | Treated fabric squares | Fabrics of varying degrees of softness were rated softer in the presence of a lemon odor (compared to an animal-like odor) | Demattè et al. (Experiment 1) [181] |
Orthonasal odor, retronasal odor | Wine (toasted odor wine); Overall aroma intensity, overall quality | Shape | Wine glasses | Odor intensity of toasted wine samples were rated as most intense when presented in a specific wine glass (Schott Zwiesel type Cask-aged spirits 8432/17 with 209 x 76 mm dimensions) | Vilanova et al. [182] |
Orthonasal odor | Feminine fragrance (Hanae Mori White) & masculine fragrance (Hanae Mori Black) (Experiment 1); Pumpkin cinnamon & eucalyptus-spearmint (Experiment 2); Pleasantness, likeability | Roughness/smoothness (Experiment 1); Temperature (Experiment 2) | Textured paper (Experiment 1); Gel packs (warm & cold) (Experiment 2) | Experiment 1: Smooth-textured paper was rated more positively in the presence of a feminine smell; rough-textured paper was rated more positively in the presence of a masculine smellExperiment 2: A warm gel-pack with a “warm” pumpkin cinnamon smell was rated more positively than with a “cold” eucalyptus-spearmint smell; a cold gel-pack with a “cold” eucalyptus-spearmint smell was rated more positively than a “warm” pumpkin cinnamon smell | Krishna et al. (Experiments 1 & 2) [183] |
Retronasal odor | Lemon yogurt; Overall flavor intensity | Curvature (round/angular) | Yogurt packaging/container | Angular yogurt containers were perceived as more intense in taste (compared to rounded yogurt containers) | Becker et al. [9] |
Orthonasal odor | Liquid soap; Overall fragrance intensity | Weight | Soap bottles | Fragranced liquid soap in heavier bottles were rated as having a higher fragrance intensity than soap in lighter bottles | Gatti et al. [184] |
Retronasal odor | Noodles; Savory flavor intensity | Shape, material | Plates, bowls (ceramic, glass, paper, metal) | No differences with regards to touch stimuli | Zhou et al. (Experiment 2) [194] |
Retronasal odor | Beer; Overall flavor quality, pleasantness | Shape, material | Beer cans vs. bottles | Beers served in bottles were rated higher in taste quality (poor/good) (compared to cans) | Barnett et al. [195] |
Orthonasal odor, retronasal odor | Cola & sparkling water; Overall aroma intensity, pleasantness | Shape | Glasses | The aromas of cola drinks served in cola glass were rated more intense and pleasant than when served in a straight water glass or bulbous bottle | Cavazzana et al. [185] |
Orthonasal odor, retronasal odor | Beer; Overall aroma pleasantness, overall flavor pleasantness, overall flavor intensity; fruitiness aroma intensity | Shape | Glasses | Higher glass curvature was associated with higher overall odor intensity (in beer) | Mirabito et al. [186] |
Retronasal odor | Ice cream; Overall flavor intensity | Sharpness/smoothness | 3D-printed cups | Ice cream served in angular-surfaced bowls were rated higher in intensity | Van Rompay et al. [196] |
Retronasal odor | Potato chips; Overall flavor intensity | Roughness/smoothness | Bowls | Salted chips served in rough and uneven bowls were rated higher in saltiness and taste intensity than when served in smooth and even bowls | Van Rompay & Groothedde [197] |
Types of Gustatory Cues | Presentation Types of Gustatory Cues | Types of Touch Cues | Presentation Types of Touch Cues | Key Findings | References |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sweetness, bitterness, sourness, saltiness | Wine (red & white); Taste intensity | Shape | Wine glasses | Red and white wine samples were rated as more sour in beaker-shaped glasses | Hummel et al. [180] |
Bitterness | Lemon yogurt; Taste intensity | Curvature(round/angular) | Yogurt packaging/container | No differences | Becker et al. [9] |
Sweetness, bitterness, sourness, saltiness | Cream; Taste intensity | Cutlery item material | Spoons | Spoons of different materials could transfer certain tastes and enhance the dominant taste of cream samples; Copper and zinc spoons lent a degree of bitterness and metallic flavor to the cream | Piqueras-Fiszman et al. [200] |
Sweetness (Experiment 1); Saltiness (Experiment 3) | Yogurt (Experiment 1); Cheese (Experiment 3); Taste intensity, pleasantness | Cutlery item weight and size (Experiment 1); Cutlery item type (Experiment 3) | Spoons (Experiment 1); Cutlery items (toothpicks vs. cheese knives vs. spoons) | Experiment 1: Yogurt was rated as sweeter when served with the smallest spoons (compared to larger spoons)Experiment 3: Cheese was rated as saltier when sampled using a knife (compared to spoon, toothpick, and fork) | Harrar & Spence (Experiments 1 & 3) [137] |
Sweetness, bitterness, sourness | Cold tea | Material | Cups (glass, plastic, paper) | No differences with regards to touch stimuli | Tu et al. (Experiment 1) [142] |
Sweetness | Noodles | Shape, material | Plates, bowls(ceramic, glass, paper, metal) | No differences with regards to touch stimuli | Zhou et al. (Experiment 2) [194] |
Sweetness, bitterness, sourness, saltiness | Cola & sparkling water; Taste intensity, pleasantness | Shape | Glasses | Cola drinks served in a cola glass were perceived to be sweeter and more pleasant than when served in a water glass or bulbous bottle | Cavazzana et al. [185] |
Sweetness, bitterness | Beer; Taste intensity | Shape | Glasses | Higher glass curvature was associated with a higher fruitiness (in beer) | Mirabito et al. [186] |
Sweetness, bitterness | Hot chocolate & coffee; Taste intensity, overall liking | Curvature (round/angular) | 3D-printed cups | Drinks served in angular-surfaced cups were rated higher in bitterness and intensity; Drinks served in rounder-surfaced cups were rated higher in sweetness and lower in intensity (in hot chocolate and coffee) | Van Rompay et al. [198] |
Sweetness, sourness | Ice cream; Taste intensity | Sharpness/smoothness | 3D-printed cups | Ice cream served in smoother-surfaced bowls were rated higher in sweetness; No differences on sourness | Van Rompay et al. [196] |
Saltiness | Potato chips; Taste intensity | Roughness/smoothness | Bowls | Salted chips served in rough and uneven bowls were rated higher in saltiness and taste intensity than when served in smooth and even bowls | Van Rompay & Groothedde [197] |
Types of Oral Touch Cues | Presentation Types of Oral Touch Cues | Types of Touch Cues | Presentation Types of Touch Cues | Key Findings | References |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Crispness | Potato chips; Attribute intensity | Material | Packaging bags (polyvinyl vs. wax-coated) | Potato chips in polyvinyl bags were perceived to be crisper | McDaniel & Baker [210] |
Weight, thinness/thickness, softness/hardness, temperature, roughness/smoothness, flexible/stiff | Hot tea & carbonated beverage; Attribute intensity | Weight, thinness/thickness, softness/hardness, temperature, roughness/smoothness, flexible/stiff | Cups (of varying materials); Attribute intensity | Product ratings for certain attributes (e.g., warmness and softness), followed packaging ratings for those attributes | Schifferstein (Experiments 1 & 2) [141] |
Softness/firmness, freshness/staleness | Pretzels; Attribute intensity | Softness/firmness, freshness/staleness | Pretzels; Attribute intensity | Stale pretzels evaluated by hands were associated with a staler and softer perception of fresh pretzels evaluated orally; Fresh pretzels evaluated by hands were associated with a fresher and firmer perception of stale pretzels evaluated orally | Barnett-Cowan [204] |
Density | Yogurt; Attribute intensity | Weight | Bowls | Yogurt served in heavier bowls were rated as denser and liked more than when served in lighter bowls | Piqueras-Fiszman & Spence [140] |
Crunchiness | Biscuits; Attribute intensity | Roughness/smoothness | Containers | Biscuits served in rough-finished containers were rated as crunchier than when served in smooth-coated containers | Piqueras-Fiszman & Spence [205] |
Density | Yogurt; Attribute density | Cutlery item weight | Spoons | Yogurt sampled using lighter spoons was rated as denser and more expensive than when sampled using heavier spoons | Harrar & Spence (Experiment 1) [137] |
Carbonation | Still & carbonated water; Attribute intensity, pleasantness | Weight | Cups (plastic) | Still and carbonated water samples were rated as less pleasant and more carbonated when served in heavy plastic cups (compared to lighter plastic cups) | Maggioni et al. [138] |
Temperature | Tea; Attribute intensity | Material | Cups (glass, plastic, paper) | Tea samples served in glass cups were perceived to be colder (compared to plastic and paper cups) | Tu et al. [142] |
Crunchiness, roughness | Biscuits; Attribute intensity | Roughness/smoothness | Plates | Biscuits served in rougher-surfaced plates were rated as crunchier and rougher than when served in smoother-surfaced plates | Biggs et al. [206] |
Carbonation | Cola & water; Attribute intensity | Shape | Glasses | Cola and water served in a bulbous bottle were perceived to have more carbonation than when served in cola or water glasses | Cavazzana et al. [185] |
Carbonation | Fruit drinks; Attribute intensity | Weight | Cups (plastic) | Highly bitter fruit drinks were perceived to be more carbonated when presented with heavier plastic cups (compared to lighter plastic cups) | Mielby et al. [207] |
Freshness, lightness | Still & carbonated water; Attribute intensity, pleasantness | Roughness/smoothness | Cups (plain, sandpaper-covered, satin-covered) | Still and carbonated water samples were more pleasant, fresher, and more light when served in plastic cups (compared to sandpaper and/or satin-covered cups) | Risso et al. [208] |
Crispness | Potato chips; Attribute intensity | Roughness/smoothness | Bowls | No differences | Van Rompay & Groothedde [197] |
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Pramudya, R.C.; Seo, H.-S. Hand-Feel Touch Cues and Their Influences on Consumer Perception and Behavior with Respect to Food Products: A Review. Foods 2019, 8, 259. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods8070259
Pramudya RC, Seo H-S. Hand-Feel Touch Cues and Their Influences on Consumer Perception and Behavior with Respect to Food Products: A Review. Foods. 2019; 8(7):259. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods8070259
Chicago/Turabian StylePramudya, Ragita C., and Han-Seok Seo. 2019. "Hand-Feel Touch Cues and Their Influences on Consumer Perception and Behavior with Respect to Food Products: A Review" Foods 8, no. 7: 259. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods8070259
APA StylePramudya, R. C., & Seo, H. -S. (2019). Hand-Feel Touch Cues and Their Influences on Consumer Perception and Behavior with Respect to Food Products: A Review. Foods, 8(7), 259. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods8070259