Enactivism and Robotic Language Acquisition: A Report from the Frontier
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Underpinnings and Methodological Choices
2.1. Robots Acquiring Language: Why and How?
2.2. Robots Acquiring Linguistic Negation: Why and How?
3. Enactivism
3.1. Autonomy
3.2. Sense-Making
3.3. Embodiment
3.4. Emergence
3.5. Experience
3.6. Affect or Volition
4. Discussion and Future Work
Future Work
5. Conclusions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
NLP | Natural Language Processing |
AI | Artificial Intelligence |
CDS | Child-Directed Speech |
HRI | Human–Robot Interaction |
SDS | Spoken Dialogue Systems |
References
- Förster, F.; Saunders, J.; Nehaniv, C.L. Robots that Say ‘No’. Affective Symbol Grounding and the Case of Intent Interpretations. IEEE Trans. Cogn. Dev. Syst. 2017, 10, 530–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Förster, F.; Saunders, J.; Lehmann, H.; Nehaniv, C.L. Robots Learning to Say ‘No’: Prohibition and Rejective Mechanisms in Acquisition of Linguistic Negation. arXiv, 2018; arXiv:1810.11804. [Google Scholar]
- Nehaniv, C.L.; Förster, F.; Saunders, J.; Broz, F.; Antonova, E.; Köse, H.; Lyon, C.; Lehmann, H.; Sato, Y.; Dautenhahn, K. Interaction and experience in enactive intelligence and humanoid robotics. In Proceedings of the 2013 IEEE Symposium on Artificial Life (ALIFE), Singapore, 16–19 April 2013; pp. 148–155. [Google Scholar]
- de Jong, M.; Zhang, K.; Roth, A.M.; Rhodes, T.; Schmucker, R.; Zhou, C.; Ferreira, S.; Cartucho, J.; Veloso, M. Towards a Robust Interactive and Learning Social Robot. In Proceedings of the 17th International Conference on Autonomous Agents and MultiAgent Systems, Stockholm, Sweden, 10–15 July 2018; pp. 883–891. [Google Scholar]
- Ward, N.G.; DeVault, D. Ten Challenges in Highly-Interactive Dialog System; Turn-Taking and Coordination in Human-Machine Interaction: Papers from the 2015 AAAI Spring Symposium; The AAAI Press: Palo Alto, CA, UAS, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Porcheron, M.; Fischer, J.E.; Reeves, S.; Sharples, S. Voice Interfaces in Everyday Life. In Proceedings of the 2018 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Montreal, QC, Canada, 21–26 April 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Harnad, S. The Symbol Grounding Problem. Physica D 1990, 42, 335–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siskind, J.M. Grounding the Lexical Semantics of Verbs in Visual Perception using Force Dynamics and Event Logic. J. Artif. Intell. Res. 2001, 15, 31–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sugita, Y.; Tani, J. Learning Semantic Combinatoriality from the Interaction between Linguistic and Behavioral Processes. Adapt. Behav. 2005, 13, 33–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Saunders, J.; Nehaniv, C.L.; Lyon, C. The acquisition of word semantics by a humanoid robot via interaction with a human tutor. In New Frontiers in Human-Robot Interaction; Dautenhahn, K., Saunders, J., Eds.; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2011; pp. 211–234. [Google Scholar]
- Saunders, J.; Lehmann, H.; Sato, Y.; Nehaniv, C.L. Towards Using Prosody to Scaffold Lexical Meaning in Robots. In Proceedings of the 2011 IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning (ICDL), Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 24–27 August 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Saunders, J.; Lehmann, H.; Förster, F.; Nehaniv, C.L. Robot Acquisition of Lexical Meaning—Moving Towards the Two-word Stage. In Proceedings of the 2012 IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL), San Diego, CA, USA, 7–9 November 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Kennington, C.; Plane, S. Symbol, Conversational, and Societal Grounding with a Toy Robot. arXiv, 2017; arXiv:1709.10486. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello, M. Constructing a Language; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Broz, F.; Nehaniv, C.L.; Belpaeme, T.; Bisio, A.; Dautenhahn, K.; Fadiga, L.; Ferrauto, T.; Fischer, K.; Förster, F.; Gigliotta, O.; et al. The ITALK Project: A Developmental Robotics Approach to the Study of Individual, Social, and Linguistic Learning. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2014, 6, 534–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fenson, L.; Dale, P.S.; Reznick, J.S.; Bates, E.; Thal, D.J.; Pethick, S.J. Variability in Early Communicative Development. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1994, 59, i-185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pea, R. The Development Of Negation In Early Child Language. In The Social Foundations of Language and Thought: Essays in Honor of Jerome S. Bruner; Olson, D., Ed.; W.W. Norton: New York, NY, USA, 1980; pp. 156–186. [Google Scholar]
- Metta, G.; Sandini, G.; Vernon, D.; Natale, L.; Nori, F. The iCub humanoid robot: An open platform for research in embodied cognition. In Proceedings of the 8th Workshop on Performance Metrics for Intelligent Systems, Gaithersburg, MD, USA, 19–21 August 2008; pp. 50–56. [Google Scholar]
- Förster, F. Robots That Say ‘No’: Acquisition of Linguistic Behaviour in Interaction Games with Humans. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Hertfordshire, Hertfordshire, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Ryan, J. Early language development: Towards a communicational analysis. In The Integration of a Child into a Social World; Richards, M.P.M., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1974. [Google Scholar]
- Spitz, R.A. No and Yes: On the Genesis of Human Communication; International Universities Press: New York, NY, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
- De Jaegher, H.; Di Paolo, E.A. Participatory sense-making. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 2007, 6, 485–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Varela, F.J.; Thompson, E.; Rosch, E. The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, R.A.; Foglia, L. Embodied Cognition. In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy; Zalta, E.N., Ed.; Spring 2017 ed.; Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University: Stanford, CA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Varela, F.J.; Thompson, E.; Rosch, E. The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience—Revised Edition; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Di Paolo, E.A.; Rohde, M.; De Jaegher, H. Horizons for the Enactive Mind: Values, Social Interaction, and Play. In Enaction: Toward a New Paradigm for Cognitive Science; Steward, J., Gapenne, O., Di Paolo, E.A., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- O’Regan, J.K. Why Red Doesn’t Sound Like a Bell: Understanding the Feel of Consciousness; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Hutto, D.D.; Myin, E. Radicalizing Enactivism: Basic Minds without Content; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Dumas, G.; Lefebvre, A.; Zhang, M.; Tognoli, E.; Kelso, J.S. The Human Dynamic Clamp: A Probe for Coordination Across Neural, Behavioral, and Social Scales. In Complexity and Synergetics; Müller, S., Plath, P., Radons, G., Fuchs, A., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Sacks, H.; Schegloff, E.A.; Jefferson, G. A Simplest Systematics for the Organization of Turn-Taking for Conversation. Language 1974, 50, 696–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jefferson, G. Preliminary notes on a possible metric which provides for a “standard maximum” silence of approximately one second in conversation. In Conversation: An Interdisciplinary Perspective; Roger, D., Bull, P., Eds.; Multilingual Matters: Clevedon, UK, 1989; Chapter 8; pp. 166–196. [Google Scholar]
- Aha, D.W. (Ed.) Lazy Learning; Springer-Science+Business Media: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1997. [Google Scholar]
1. | |
2. | There are still many occasions where spoken dialogue systems (SDS) fail dramatically. In robotics, the on-board speech recognition is often times sub-optimal due to the presence of noise or due to a limited speech corpus (cf. [4]). However, even assuming perfect speech recognition, a dialogue system may have too narrow a focus in terms of the application area that the designers envisioned (cf. Ward et al. for a wish-list for future dialogue systems [5]). Moreover, our understanding and modelling of conversational capabilities seem to lag far behind developments in speech recognition (cf. [6] for some revealing transcripts of ‘conversations’ between humans and voice interfaces). |
3. | While some symbol grounding architectures ground linguistic entities directly in sensorimotor data, others may link them to abstractions thereof (‘concepts’). In particular, logical-type architectures similar to the one developed by Siskind [8] involve multiple layers of explicit ‘data abstraction’. In these cases, the established link is not a direct one. The derived concepts, however, are typically causally linked to the agent’s sensorimotor context, such that a link between ‘word’ and ‘world’ can be postulated and just requires the analyst’s willingness to go through several abstraction layers. |
4. | A detailed analysis of potential confusions of coders with respect to negation words that occurred in our studies and the reasons behind these confusions are provided in [19]. |
© 2019 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Förster, F. Enactivism and Robotic Language Acquisition: A Report from the Frontier. Philosophies 2019, 4, 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/philosophies4010011
Förster F. Enactivism and Robotic Language Acquisition: A Report from the Frontier. Philosophies. 2019; 4(1):11. https://doi.org/10.3390/philosophies4010011
Chicago/Turabian StyleFörster, Frank. 2019. "Enactivism and Robotic Language Acquisition: A Report from the Frontier" Philosophies 4, no. 1: 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/philosophies4010011
APA StyleFörster, F. (2019). Enactivism and Robotic Language Acquisition: A Report from the Frontier. Philosophies, 4(1), 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/philosophies4010011