Theory of Mind Ability and Socioeconomic Status, a Study of Street-Connected Children and Adolescents in Ecuador
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Results
2.1. Comparison of Street-Connected Youth with Their Control Sample for Verbal Comprehension
2.2. Paired-Sample Analysis of 25 Street-Connected and 25 Control Matched Pairs
2.3. Psychometric Properties of the Assessments Used
2.4. Paired-Sample Comparison of Theory of Mind Performance
2.5. Correlations between Theory of Mind Performance and PTSD Severity Scores
3. Discussion
4. Materials and Methods
4.1. Design
4.2. Sample and Participants
4.2.1. Former Street-Connected Youth
4.2.2. Age-Matched School Attending Children and Adolescents
4.2.3. Children and Adolescents Recruited in a Study of Socioeconomic Status (SES)
4.3. Assessments
4.4. Procedure
4.5. Statistical Analysis
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Avants, B.B.; Hackman, D.A.; Betancourt, L.M.; Lawson, G.M.; Hurt, H.; Farah, M.J. Relation of childhood home environment to cortical thickness in late adolescence: Specificity of experience and timing. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0138217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hackman, D.A.; Farah, M.J. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009, 13, 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lawson, G.M.; Duda, J.T.; Avants, B.B.; Wu, J.; Farah, M.J. Associations between children’s socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness. Dev. Sci. 2013, 16, 641–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Farah, M.J.; Shera, D.M.; Savage, J.H.; Betancourt, L.; Giannetta, J.M.; Brodsky, N.L.; Malmud, E.K.; Hurt, H. Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Res. 2006, 1110, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Noble, K.G.; McCandliss, B.D.; Farah, M.J. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, 464–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Noble, K.G.; Norman, M.F.; Farah, M.J. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Dev. Sci. 2005, 8, 74–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pluck, G.; Banda-Cruz, D.R.; Andrade-Guimaraes, M.V.; Trueba, A.F. Socioeconomic deprivation and the development of neuropsychological functions: A study with “street children” in Ecuador. Child Neuropsychol. 2018, 24, 510–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, H.; Furnham, A. Childhood cognitive ability, education, and personality traits predict attainment in adult occupational prestige over 17 years. J. Vocat. Behav. 2012, 81, 218–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hatch, S.L.; Jones, P.B.; Kuh, D.; Hardy, R.; Wadsworth, M.E.; Richards, M. Childhood cognitive ability and adult mental health in the British 1946 birth cohort. Soc. Sci. Med. 2007, 64, 2285–2296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Arnett, J.J. The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. Am. Psychol. 2008, 63, 602–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Henrich, J.; Heine, S.J.; Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 2010, 33, 61–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pluck, G. The ‘street children’ of Latin America. Psychologist 2015, 28, 20–23. [Google Scholar]
- Naterer, A.; Lavrič, M. Using social indicators in assessing factors and numbers of street children in the world. Child Indic. Res. 2016, 9, 21–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zagorsky, J.L. Do you have to be smart to be rich? The impact of IQ on wealth, income and financial distress. Intelligence 2007, 35, 489–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sauce, B.; Matzel, L.D. The paradox of intelligence: Heritability and malleability coexist in hidden gene-environment interplay. Psychol. Bull. 2018, 144, 26–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fry, C.E.; Langley, K.; Shelton, K.H. A systematic review of cognitive functioning among young people who have experienced homelessness, foster care, or poverty. Child Neuropsychol. 2016, 23, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- O’Neil-Pirozzi, T.M. Language functioning of residents in family homeless shelters. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2003, 12, 229–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pluck, G. Cognitive abilities of ‘street children’: A systematic review. Chuo J. Policy Sci. Cult. Stud. 2013, 21, 121–133. [Google Scholar]
- Shein-Szydlo, J.; Sukhodolsky, D.G.; Kon, D.S.; Tejeda, M.M.; Ramirez, E.; Ruchkin, V. A randomized controlled study of cognitive-behavioral therapy for posttraumatic stress in street children in Mexico City. J. Trauma. Stress 2016, 29, 406–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Delis, D.C.; Kaplan, E.; Kramer, J.H. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Pluck, G.; Amraoui, D.; Fornell-Villalobos, I. Brief communication: Reliability of the D-KEFS Tower Test in samples of children and adolescents in Ecuador. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 2021, 10, 158–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dahlman, S.; Backstrom, P.; Bohlin, G.; Frans, O. Cognitive abilities of street children: Low-SES Bolivian boys with and without experience of living in the street. Child Neuropsychol. 2013, 19, 540–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Howard, S.J.; Cook, C.J.; Everts, L.; Melhuish, E.; Scerif, G.; Norris, S.; Twine, R.; Kahn, K.; Draper, C.E. Challenging socioeconomic status: A cross-cultural comparison of early executive function. Dev. Sci. 2020, 23, e12854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Saxe, G.B. The mathematics of child street vendors. Child Dev. 1988, 59, 1415–1425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alaraudanjoki, E.; Regmi, M.P.; Ahonen, T.; Nurmi, J.E.; Ruoppila, I. Cognitive skills among Nepalese child labourers. Int. J. Psychol. 2001, 36, 242–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dvash, J.; Shamay-Tsoory, S.G. Theory of mind and empathy as multidimensional constructs: Neurological foundations. Top. Lang. Disord. 2014, 34, 282–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wade, M.; Prime, H.; Jenkins, J.M.; Yeates, K.O.; Williams, T.; Lee, K. On the relation between theory of mind and executive functioning: A developmental cognitive neuroscience perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 2119–2140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chakrabarty, S.; Widing, R.E.; Brown, G. Selling behaviours and sales performance: The moderating and mediating effects of interpersonal mentalizing. J. Pers. Sell. Sales Manag. 2014, 34, 112–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pinzon-Rondon, A.M.; Botero, J.C.; Benson, L.; Briceno-Ayala, L.; Kanamori, M. Workplace abuse and economic exploitation of children working in the streets of Latin American cities. Int. J. Occup. Environ. Health 2010, 16, 162–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pinzon-Rondon, A.M.; Koblinsky, S.A.; Hofferth, S.L.; Pinzon-Florez, C.E.; Briceno, L. Work-related injuries among child street-laborers in Latin America: Prevalence and predictors. Rev. Panam. Salud Publica 2009, 26, 235–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cummins, D.D. How the social environment shaped the evolution of mind. Synthese 2000, 122, 3–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heleniak, C.; McLaughlin, K.A. Social-cognitive mechanisms in the cycle of violence: Cognitive and affective theory of mind, and externalizing psychopathology in children and adolescents. Dev. Psychopathol. 2020, 32, 735–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nazarov, A.; Frewen, P.; Parlar, M.; Oremus, C.; MacQueen, G.; McKinnon, M.; Lanius, R. Theory of mind performance in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood abuse. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2014, 129, 193–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Atwoli, L.; Ayuku, D.; Hogan, J.; Koech, J.; Vreeman, R.C.; Ayaya, S.; Braitstein, P. Impact of domestic care environment on trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder among orphans in western Kenya. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e89937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Crombach, A.; Elbert, T. The benefits of aggressive traits: A study with current and former street children in Burundi. Child Abus. Negl. 2014, 38, 1041–1050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Pluck, G.; Banda-Cruz, D.R.; Andrade-Guimaraes, M.V.; Ricaurte-Diaz, S.; Borja-Alvarez, T. Post-traumatic stress disorder and intellectual function of socioeconomically deprived ‘street children’ in Quito, Ecuador. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2015, 13, 215–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pluck, G.; Córdova, M.A.; Bock, C.; Chalen, I.; Trueba, A.F. Socioeconomic status, executive functions, and theory of mind ability in adolescents: Relationships with language ability and cortisol. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2021, 39, 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bernardi, R.A. Validating research results when Cronbach’s alpha is below. 70: A methodological procedure. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1994, 54, 766–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pike, C.K.; Hudson, W.W. Reliability and measurement error in the presence of homogeneity. J. Soc. Serv. Res. 1998, 24, 149–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- DeVellis, R.F. Scale Development: Theory and Applications, 2nd ed.; Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Stone, V.E.; Baron-Cohen, S.; Calder, A.; Keane, J.; Young, A. Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia 2003, 41, 209–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shatz, M.; Diesendruck, G.; Martinez-Beck, I.; Akar, D. The influence of language and socioeconomic status on children’s understanding of false belief. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 39, 717–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Akiki, T.J.; Averill, C.L.; Wrocklage, K.M.; Schweinsburg, B.; Scott, J.C.; Martini, B.; Averill, L.A.; Southwick, S.M.; Krystal, J.H.; Abdallah, C.G. The association of PTSD symptom severity with localized hippocampus and amygdala abnormalities. Chronic Stress 2017, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Dang, J.; King, K.M.; Inzlicht, M. Why are self-report and behavioral measures weakly correlated? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2020, 24, 267–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hedge, C.; Powell, G.; Sumner, P. The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behav. Res. Methods 2018, 50, 1166–1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pluck, G.; Barajas, B.M.; Hernandez-Rodriguez, J.L.; Martinez, M.A. Language ability and adult homelessness. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2020, 55, 332–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gignac, G.E. Evaluating subtest ‘g’ saturation levels via the single trait-correlated uniqueness (STCU) SEM approach: Evidence in favor of crystallized subtests as the best indicators of ‘g’. Intelligence 2006, 34, 29–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barwick, M.A.; Siegel, L.S. Learning difficulties in adolescent clients of a shelter for runaway and homeless street youths. J. Res. Adolesc. 1996, 6, 649–670. [Google Scholar]
- Kidd, D.C.; Castano, E. Reading literary fiction improves theory of mind. Science 2013, 342, 377–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cohen, J. A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 112, 155–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treanor, T.J. Relief for Mandela’s children: Street children and the law in the new South Africa. Fordham Law Rev. 1994, 63, 883–925. [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen, S.; Wheelwright, S.; Spong, A.; Scahill, V.; Lawson, J. Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? J. Dev. Learn. Disord. 2001, 5, 47–78. [Google Scholar]
- Preti, A.; Vellante, M.; Petretto, D.R. The psychometric properties of the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test: An item response theory (IRT) analysis. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 2017, 22, 233–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baron-Cohen, S.; O’Riordan, M.; Jones, R.; Stone, V.; Plaisted, K. A new test of social sensitivity: Detection of faux pas in normal children and children with Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1999, 29, 407–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Steinberg, A.M.; Brymer, M.J.; Kim, S.; Briggs, E.C.; Ippen, C.G.; Ostrowski, S.A.; Pynoos, R.S. Psychometric properties of the UCLA PTSD Reaction Index: Part I. J. Trauma. Stress 2013, 26, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, H.Y. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: Assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restor. Dent. Endod. 2013, 38, 52–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Statistic | RMET | Faux Pas | Comprehension |
---|---|---|---|
Street-connected (n = 25) | |||
%CofV | 24 | 80 | 19 |
α | 0.61 | 0.65 | 0.40 |
Z Skew | 0.96 | 1.2 | 2.9 1 |
Z Kurtosis | 0.26 | 0.64 | 1.5 |
Control (n = 25) | |||
%CofV | 21 | 46 | 18 |
α | 0.64 | 0.75 | 0.43 |
Z Skew | 1.4 | 0.54 | 2.9 1 |
Z Kurtosis | 0.87 | 1.1 | 1.5 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Pluck, G. Theory of Mind Ability and Socioeconomic Status, a Study of Street-Connected Children and Adolescents in Ecuador. Psych 2021, 3, 72-84. https://doi.org/10.3390/psych3020008
Pluck G. Theory of Mind Ability and Socioeconomic Status, a Study of Street-Connected Children and Adolescents in Ecuador. Psych. 2021; 3(2):72-84. https://doi.org/10.3390/psych3020008
Chicago/Turabian StylePluck, Graham. 2021. "Theory of Mind Ability and Socioeconomic Status, a Study of Street-Connected Children and Adolescents in Ecuador" Psych 3, no. 2: 72-84. https://doi.org/10.3390/psych3020008
APA StylePluck, G. (2021). Theory of Mind Ability and Socioeconomic Status, a Study of Street-Connected Children and Adolescents in Ecuador. Psych, 3(2), 72-84. https://doi.org/10.3390/psych3020008