Classroom Psychomotor Education Programme to Enhance Executive Functions: A Cluster Randomised Feasibility Trial
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Rationale for Developing an Intervention for Executive Functions
1.2. Rationale for Conducting a Feasibility Trial
1.3. Study Aims and Objectives
- To investigate the feasibility of POTENTIAL (implementation of the intervention and the study design).
- To test the suitability of the outcome measures completed by teachers, children, and their parents and carers.
- To explore the acceptability of POTENTIAL by teachers and young participants (acceptability of the intervention and the accompanying manual).
- To investigate the extent of the compliance and fidelity of the intervention delivery.
- To locate and identify facilitating and hindering parameters of the implementation of POTENTIAL (recruitment, sampling, consent, and follow-up rates).
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Study Design
2.2. Recruitment and Sampling
2.3. Randomisation and Blinding
2.4. Interventions
2.4.1. Experimental Intervention: POTENTIAL
Systems Theory of Change
The Facilitative Intervention Training [FIT] Method
Task Progression
Dosage
Structure of POTENTIAL Sessions
The Characters and Interactive Stories
2.4.2. No Intervention Control: Education as Usual
2.5. Outcomes
2.5.1. Primary Outcome Measures—Feasibility and Acceptability
2.5.2. Secondary Outcome Measures—Children’s Executive Functions
Working Memory
Inhibition
Attention
Cognitive Flexibility
Self-Regulation
Parents’ and Teachers’ Questionnaires
2.6. Data Analysis
2.7. Ethical Issues
3. Results
3.1. Recruitment and Sampling
3.1.1. Recruitment Rates
3.1.2. Sampling
Participants
3.1.3. Compliance and Fidelity to the Intervention Delivery
It was the first time we incorporated a project that lasted so long in our routine, and it was so intense (daily), and not a single child reported any dissatisfaction. On the contrary, all children requested the programme daily and seemed equally excited and happy in each session from beginning to end.
3.2. The Acceptability of POTENTIAL
We really liked the activities in the programme. They were fun, easy to administer and age-appropriate. Even though we did not manage to work on the more difficult levels, it was nice that they existed as an option. We intend to continue using the manual throughout the rest of the school year as we saw how much our pupils and ourselves enjoyed and benefited from the activities. Unfortunately, nowadays, most children do not have the chance to move their bodies during the day or even play, so having this time during the school day feels necessary for their development and well-being.
The use of the treasure-hunt storyline was very motivating for the children, and the fictional characters were very well designed and matched the various EFs. Even though I was sceptical, at first, regarding using EFs terms while working with children of various abilities in an inclusion unit, I am glad that I gave it a try. The programme is well designed, and all children could participate and even understand (to various extents) the notion of attention and EFs. It needed lots of repetition and practice, but thanks to the characters and the pictograms, this came very naturally to children and me.
3.3. The Appropriateness of the Outcome Measures
4. Discussion
I firmly believe that one of the reasons that all children enjoyed the programme so much (alongside its fun and engaging storyline) was that it gave them daily the opportunity to move and explore their body and its movements in new and exciting ways. As the years pass by, I notice that children in early years have less and less opportunities at school and home to engage in PA which has resulted in more and more difficulties in their overall development. It is refreshing to witness how many things children can achieve through PA and play, and how much they need and enjoy it.
Limitations of the Present Study
5. Conclusions
6. Patents
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Week 1 | ||
---|---|---|
Session | EF | Activity/Game |
1 | Story 2- Mr Brain/Working Memory (A part) | Storybook 2 |
Inhibition of predominant response | Simon says | |
Interference of ongoing response | Butter and honey | |
Working memory- Sustained attention- Self-regulation | Guided imagery (Let’s go to space) | |
2 | A reminder of Mr Brain | Pictogram |
Attention | What does your name say? | |
Working memory | What is in the box | |
Working memory- Sustained attention- Self-regulation | Yoga story (Adventure in space) | |
3 | A reminder of Mr Brain | Pictogram |
Interference of ongoing response | Musical chairs | |
Inhibition of predominant response | Day or night | |
Working memory- Sustained attention- Self-regulation | Guided imagery (The planet of the robots) | |
4 | A reminder of Mr Brain | Pictogram |
Attention | Get the flag | |
Working memory | 5-s challenges | |
Working memory- Sustained attention- Self-regulation | Yoga story (Adventure in space) | |
5 | A reminder of Mr Brain | Pictogram |
Interference of ongoing response | What is the time, Mr Brain? | |
Cognitive flexibility | Fantastical role-play | |
Working memory- Sustained attention- Self-regulation | Breathing exercises (The shapes of relaxation) | |
Story 2- Mr Brain/Working Memory (B part) | Storybook 2 |
Title of Story | Book Cover |
---|---|
POT, the inattentive robot and the pursuit of the lost attention | |
POT, the inattentive robot and Mr Brain | |
POT, the inattentive robot and Dr Flexy | |
POT, the inattentive robot and the Witch | |
POT, the inattentive robot and the Maintenance Man | |
POT, the inattentive robot and the detectives ‘Attention’ | |
POT, the inattentive robot and the Traffic Controller | |
POT, the inattentive robot and the secret recipe of organisation |
References
- Carson, V.; Lee, E.-Y.; Hewitt, L.; Jennings, C.; Hunter, S.; Kuzik, N.; Stearns, J.A.; Unrau, S.P.; Poitras, V.J.; Gray, C.; et al. Systematic review of the relationships between physical activity and health indicators in the early years (0–4 years). BMC Public Health 2017, 17 (Suppl. S5), 33–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gale, C.R.; O’Callaghan, F.J.; Godfrey, K.M.; Law, C.M.; Martyn, C.N. Critical periods of brain growth and cognitive function in children. Brain 2004, 127 Pt 2, 321–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tierney, A.L.; Nelson, C.A., III. Brain development and the role of experience in the early years. Zero Three 2009, 30, 9–13. [Google Scholar]
- Diamond, A. Interrelated and interdependent. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, 152–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lubans, D.; Richards, J.; Hillman, C.; Faulkner, G.; Beauchamp, M.; Nilsson, M.; Kelly, P.; Smith, J.; Raine, L.; Biddle, S. Physical Activity for Cognitive and Mental Health in Youth: A Systematic Review of Mechanisms. Pediatrics 2016, 138, e20161642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stillman, C.M.; Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Brown, B.; Bender, C.M.; Erickson, K.I. Effects of Exercise on Brain and Cognition Across Age Groups and Health States. Trends Neurosci. 2020, 43, 533–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pesce, C.; Ballester, R.; Benzing, V. Giving physical activity and cognition research ‘some soul’: Focus on children and adolescents. Eur. J. Hum. Mov. 2021, 47, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ureña, N.; Fernández, N.; Cárdenas, D.; Madinabeitia, I.; Alarcón, F. Acute Effect of Cognitive Compromise during Physical Exercise on Self-Regulation in Early Childhood Education. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 9325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rouvali, A.; Riga, V. The relationship between motor skills and executive functions in children 1–15 years old with and without special educational needs and/or disabilities. Inq. Sport Phys. Educ. 2021, 19, 17–29. [Google Scholar]
- Donnelly, J.E.; Hillman, C.; Castelli, D.; Etnier, J.L.; Lee, S.; Tomporowski, P.; Lambourne, K.; Szabo-Reed, A. Physical Activity, Fitness, Cognitive Function, and Academic Achievement in Children: A systematic review. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2016, 48, 1197–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Okely, A.D.; Ghersi, D.; Hesketh, K.D.; Santos, R.; Loughran, S.P.; Cliff, D.P.; Shilton, T.; Grant, D.; Jones, R.A.; Stanley, R.M.; et al. A collaborative approach to adopting/adapting guidelines—The Australian 24-Hour Movement Guidelines for the early years (Birth to 5 years): An integration of physical activity, sedentary behavior, and sleep. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 167–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tremblay, M.S.; LeBlanc, A.G.; Carson, V.; Choquette, L.; Gorber, S.C.; Dillman, C.; Duggan, M.; Gordon, M.J.; Hicks, A.; Janssen, I.; et al. Canadian Physical Activity Guidelines for the Early Years (aged 0–4 years). Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 2012, 37, 345–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Carson, V.; Hunter, S.; Kuzik, N.; Wiebe, S.A.; Spence, J.C.; Friedman, A.; Tremblay, M.S.; Slater, L.; Hinkley, T. Systematic review of physical activity and cognitive development in early childhood. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2016, 19, 573–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, D.; Innerd, A.; Giles, E.L.; Azevedo, L.B. Association between fundamental motor skills and physical activity in the early years: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Sport Health Sci. 2020, 9, 542–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, D.; Innerd, A.; Giles, E.L.; Azevedo, L.B. The Association between Physical Activity, Motor Skills and School Readiness in 4–5-Year-Old Children in the Northeast of England. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Corcoran, L.; Steinley, K. Early Childhood Program Participation, Results from the National Household Education Surveys Program of 2016: First Look; National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, US Department of Education: Washington DC, USA, 2019.
- Calella, P.; Mancusi, C.; Pecoraro, P.; Sensi, S.; Sorrentino, C.; Imoletti, M.; Franzese, A.; Gallè, F.; Liguori, G.; Valerio, G. Classroom active breaks: A feasibility study in Southern Italy. Health Promot. Int. 2020, 35, 373–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, A.; Timperio, A.; Brown, H.; Hesketh, K.D. A primary school active break programme (ACTI-BREAK): Study protocol for a pilot cluster randomised controlled trial. Trials 2017, 18, 433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitehead, M. (Ed.) Physical Literacy across the World; Routledge Taylor & Francis Group: New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Mahone, E.M.; Schneider, H.E. Assessment of Attention in Preschoolers. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2012, 22, 361–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Michaëlsson, M.; Yuan, S.; Melhus, H.; Baron, J.A.; Byberg, L.; Larsson, S.C.; Michaëlsson, K. The impact and causal directions for the associations between diagnosis of ADHD, socioeconomic status, and intelligence by use of a bi-directional two-sample Mendelian randomization design. BMC Med. 2022, 20, 106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rapport, M.D.; Scanlan, S.W.; Denney, C.B. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and scholastic achievement: A model of dual developmental pathways. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 1999, 40, 1169–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newborg, J.; Stock, J.R.; Wnek, L. Battelle Developmental Inventory Screening Test; DLM: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Stefanatos, G.A.; Baron, I.S. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Neuropsychological Perspective Towards DSM-V. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2007, 17, 5–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A. Executive Functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Willoughby, M.T.; Piper, B.; Oyanga, A.; King, K.M.; Merseth, K. Measuring executive function skills in young children in Kenya: Associations with school readiness. Dev. Sci. 2019, 22, e12818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N. The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 21, 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Donders, F.C. On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychol. 1969, 30, 412–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friedman, N.P.; Miyake, A. The Relations Among Inhibition and Interference Control Functions: A Latent-Variable Analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2004, 133, 101–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gandolfi, E.; Viterbori, P.; Traverso, L.; Usai, M.C. Inhibitory processes in toddlers: A latent-variable approach. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kane, M.J.; Meier, M.E.; Smeekens, B.A.; Gross, G.M.; Chun, C.A.; Silvia, P.J.; Kwapil, T.R. Individual differences in the executive control of attention, memory, and thought, and their associations with schizotypy. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2016, 145, 1017–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nigg, J.T. On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychol. Bull. 2000, 126, 220–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stahl, C.; Voss, A.; Schmitz, F.; Nuszbaum, M.; Tüscher, O.; Lieb, K.; Klauer, K.C. Behavioral components of impulsivity. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2014, 143, 850–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Memisevic, H.; Bisevic, I. Exploring the link between inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility in preschool children. Cogn. Brain, Behav. Interdiscip. J. 2018, 22, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Posner, M.I. Hierarchical distributed networks in the neuropsychology of selective attention. In Cognitive Neuropsychology and Neurolinguistics: Advances in Models of Cognitive Function and Impairment; Caramazza, A., Ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1990; pp. 187–210. [Google Scholar]
- McClelland, M.M.; Tominey, S.L.; Schmitt, S.A.; Hatfield, B.E.; Purpura, D.J.; Gonzales, C.R.; Tracy, A.N. Red Light, Purple Light! Results of an Intervention to Promote School Readiness for Children From Low-Income Backgrounds. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kapa, L.L.; Plante, E.; Doubleday, K. Applying an Integrative Framework of Executive Function to Preschoolers With Specific Language Impairment. J. Speech, Lang. Hear. Res. 2017, 60, 2170–2184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waller, R.; Hyde, L.W.; Baskin-Sommers, A.R.; Olson, S.L. Interactions between Callous Unemotional Behaviors and Executive Function in Early Childhood Predict later Aggression and Lower Peer-liking in Late-childhood. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2017, 45, 597–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McClelland, M.M.; Acock, A.C.; Piccinin, A.; Rhea, S.A.; Stallings, M.C. Relations between preschool attention span-persistence and age 25 educational outcomes. Early Child. Res. Q. 2013, 28, 314–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Moffitt, T.E.; Arseneault, L.; Belsky, D.; Dickson, N.; Hancox, R.J.; Harrington, H.; Houts, R.; Poulton, R.; Roberts, B.W.; Ross, S.; et al. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 2693–2698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stodden, D.F.; Goodway, J.D.; Langendorfer, S.J.; Roberton, M.A.; Rudisill, M.E.; Garcia, C.; Garcia, L.E. A developmental perspective on the role of motor skill competence in physical activity: An emergent relationship. Quest 2008, 60, 290–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A.; Ling, D.S. Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 18, 34–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Eldridge, S.M.; Chan, C.L.; Campbell, M.J.; Bond, C.M.; Hopewell, S.; Thabane, L.; Lancaster, G.A.; PAFS Consensus Group. CONSORT 2010 statement: Extension to randomised pilot and feasibility trials. BMJ 2016, 355, i5239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Steckler, A.B.; Linnan, L. Process Evaluation for Public Health Interventions and Research; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Arain, M.; Campbell, M.J.; Cooper, C.L.; Lancaster, G.A. What is a pilot or feasibility study? A review of current practice and editorial policy. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2010, 10, 67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rowe, A.; Titterington, J.; Holmes, J.; Henry, L.; Taggart, L. A classroom intervention targeting working memory, attention and language skills: A cluster randomised feasibility trial. Pilot Feasibility Stud. 2021, 7, 45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rapport, M.D.; Orban, S.A.; Kofler, M.J.; Friedman, L.M. Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2013, 33, 1237–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, S.C.L.; Harvey, D.J.; Shields, R.; Shields, G.; Rashedi, R.N.; Tancredi, D.; Angkustsiri, K.; Hansen, R.L.; Schweitzer, J.B. Effects of Yoga on Attention, Impulsivity, and Hyperactivity in Preschool-Aged Children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2018, 39, 200–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Razza, R.A.; Bergen-Cico, D.; Raymond, K. Enhancing Preschoolers’ Self-Regulation Via Mindful Yoga. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2015, 24, 372–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mellenthin, C. Guided imagery. In Play Therapy with Children: Modalities for Change; Kaduson, H.G., Schaefer, C.E., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2021; pp. 125–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Murdock, M. Spinning Inward: Using Guided Imagery with Children for Learning, Creativity, & Relaxation; Shambhala Publications: Boulder, CO, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Thibodeau, R.B.; Gilpin, A.T.; Brown, M.M.; Meyer, B.A. The effects of fantastical pretend-play on the development of executive functions: An intervention study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2016, 145, 120–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Golomb, C.; Kuersten, R. On the transition from pretence play to reality: What are the rules of the game? Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 1996, 14, 203–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McLeroy, K.R.; Bibeau, D.; Steckler, A.; Glanz, K. An Ecological Perspective on Health Promotion Programs. Health Educ. Q. 1988, 15, 351–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowe, A.; Titterington, J.; Taggart, L. A classroom-based intervention targeting working memory, attention and language skills in 4–5 year olds (RECALL): Study protocol for a cluster randomised feasibility trial. Pilot Feasibility Stud. 2019, 5, 82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Klingberg, T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010, 14, 317–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Posner, M.I.; Petersen, S.E. The Attention System of the Human Brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 1990, 13, 25–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rueda, M.R.; Checa, P.; Cómbita, L.M. Enhanced efficiency of the executive attention network after training in preschool children: Immediate changes and effects after two months. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2012, 2 (Suppl. S1), S192–S204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veljković, A.A.; Katanić, B.; Mašanovic, B. Effects of a 12-Weeks Yoga Intervention on Motor and Cognitive Abilities of Preschool Children. Front. Pediatr. 2021, 9, 799226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Volckaert, A.M.S.; Noël, M.-P. Training executive function in preschoolers reduce externalizing behaviors. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2015, 4, 37–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ericsson, K.A. Discovering deliberate practice activities that overcome plateaus and limits on improvement of per-formance. In Proceedings of the International Symposium on Performance Science, Utrecht, The Netherlands, 16–20 June 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Ericsson, K.A.; Nandagopal, K.; Roring, R.W. Toward a Science of Exceptional Achievement: Attaining superior performance through deliberate practice. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009, 1172, 199–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ericsson, K.A.; Towne, T.J. Expertise. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 2010, 1, 404–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gagné, P.P.; Longpré, L.P. Apprendre…Avec Réflecto: Programme D’entraînement et de Développement des Compétences Cognitives. [Learn… with Reflecto: Training and Cognitive Skills Development Program]; Chenelière/McGraw-Hill: Montréal, Canada, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Dorer, M. The Deep Well of Time: The Transformative Power of Storytelling in the Classroom; Parent Child Press: Santa Rosa, CA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Halperin, J.M.; Marks, D.J.; Bedard, A.C.V.; Chacko, A.; Curchack, J.T.; Yoon, C.A.; Healey, D.M. Training ex-ecutive, attention, and motor skills: A proof-of-concept study in preschool children with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. 2013, 17, 711–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Röthlisberger, M.; Neuenschwander, R.; Cimeli, P.; Michel, E.; Roebers, C.M. Improving executive functions in 5- and 6-year-olds: Evaluation of a small group intervention in prekindergarten and kindergarten children. Infant Child Dev. 2012, 21, 411–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bezevegis, G.E.; Oikonomou, A.; Mylonas, K. Εργαλείο ανίχνευσης και διερεύνησης διαταραχών μνήμης στο νηπιαγωγείο και στο δημοτικό ή “τεστ μνήμης”; National and Kapodistrian University of Athens: Athens, Greece, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Gerstadt, C.L.; Hong, Y.J.; Diamond, A. The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 312–7 years old on a stroop- like day-night test. Cognition 1994, 53, 129–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Korkman, M.; Kirk, U.; Kemp, S. NEPSY: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment Manual; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Zelazo, P.D. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nat. Protoc. 2006, 1, 297–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McClelland, M.M.; Cameron, C.; Bowles, R.; Geldhof, G. Developing a Measure of Self-Regulation for at-Risk Children; U.S. Department of Education, Institute for Education Sciences: Washington, DC, USA, 2018.
- Braun, V.; Clarke, V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 2006, 3, 77–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McClelland, M.M.; Gonzales, C.R.; Cameron, C.E.; Geldhof, G.J.; Bowles, R.P.; Nancarrow, A.F.; Merculief, A.; Tracy, A. The Head-Toes-Knees-Shoulders Revised: Links to Academic Outcomes and Measures of EF in Young Children. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 721846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Interactive Story | Alerting Activity | Organising Activity | Relaxation | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Guided relaxation | Breathing exercises | Yoga stories | |||
One per week (Monday part A and Friday Part B- Conclusion) | One per day | One or two per day (Target 15–20 min total duration) | Two times per week (Different each time) | Once a week | Two times per week (Same story twice) |
Objectives | Outcome Measures |
---|---|
To investigate the feasibility of POTENTIAL (implementation of the intervention and the study design). | The rates of actual recruitment compared to the recruitment targets were counted in terms of the following:
|
To test the suitability of the outcome measures completed by teachers, children, and their parents and carers. |
|
To explore the acceptability of POTENTIAL by teachers and young participants (acceptability of the intervention and the accompanying manual). |
|
To investigate the extent of the compliance and fidelity of the intervention delivery. |
|
Participants | Recruitment Targets | Numbers Recruited | Characteristics |
---|---|---|---|
Classes | n = 4 | n = 4 | Early years classes with an Inclusion Unit in a small part of Achaia, the third largest Regional Unit of Greece |
Children | n = 32 | n = 42 | Gender: girls (n = 16, 50%); boys (n = 16, 50%) Age at baseline, 47 to 77 months (mean = 58.9 months) |
n = 16 | n = 20 | Children about whom the teachers had concerns regarding their attention and listening skills | |
n = 12 | n = 16 | Typically developing children about whom the teachers had no concerns regarding their attention and listening skills | |
n = 4 | n = 6 | Children with diagnosed SEN/D |
Theme | Quotes from Teachers |
---|---|
All activities were acceptable. | All the activities were fun and easy to administrate. No instances did anyone report that they did not want to participate in the activities. |
Some stories were lengthy. | The stories helped with the children’s motivation. However, a few stories (two) were a little lengthier than needed. |
The fantastical characters and interactive stories were acceptable and motivating. | Both the children and I enjoyed the stories and the characters. They were age-appropriate, and the illustration was very on point. The main character was likeable, and all the children wanted to help. |
Need for more training before the implementation of the intervention. | Even though the continuous support from the research team was helpful, a more extensive and practical training evening would have allowed me to feel more confident during the first week. |
The supporting material was acceptable and helpful. | The pictograms were very helpful, as they gave us smart tips about EFs in a playful way. I also liked that I could refer to them during the day (e.g., when I was giving instructions about an activity or when the children were hyperactive at lunch time). |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Riga, V.; Rouvali, A. Classroom Psychomotor Education Programme to Enhance Executive Functions: A Cluster Randomised Feasibility Trial. Youth 2023, 3, 502-525. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth3020035
Riga V, Rouvali A. Classroom Psychomotor Education Programme to Enhance Executive Functions: A Cluster Randomised Feasibility Trial. Youth. 2023; 3(2):502-525. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth3020035
Chicago/Turabian StyleRiga, Vassiliki, and Aimilia Rouvali. 2023. "Classroom Psychomotor Education Programme to Enhance Executive Functions: A Cluster Randomised Feasibility Trial" Youth 3, no. 2: 502-525. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth3020035
APA StyleRiga, V., & Rouvali, A. (2023). Classroom Psychomotor Education Programme to Enhance Executive Functions: A Cluster Randomised Feasibility Trial. Youth, 3(2), 502-525. https://doi.org/10.3390/youth3020035