Next Article in Journal
Molecular Research on Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis
Next Article in Special Issue
Epigenetic Regulation by Polycomb Complexes from Drosophila to Human and Its Relation to Communicable Disease Pathogenesis
Previous Article in Journal
Oxygen–Glucose Deprivation Promoted Fibroblast Senescence and Collagen Expression via IL11
Previous Article in Special Issue
Roles of ZnT86D in Neurodevelopment and Pathogenesis of Alzheimer Disease in a Drosophila melanogaster Model
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sleep Modulates Alcohol Toxicity in Drosophila

by
Aliza K. De Nobrega
1,†,
Eric J. Noakes
1,2,†,
Natalie A. Storch
1,
Alana P. Mellers
1 and
Lisa C. Lyons
1,*
1
Program in Neuroscience, Department of Biological Science, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306, USA
2
Department of Biological Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90007, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2022, 23(20), 12091; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms232012091
Submission received: 12 August 2022 / Revised: 3 October 2022 / Accepted: 5 October 2022 / Published: 11 October 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Role of Drosophila in Human Disease Research 3.0)

Abstract

:
Alcohol abuse is a significant public health problem. While considerable research has shown that alcohol use affects sleep, little is known about the role of sleep deprivation in alcohol toxicity. We investigated sleep as a factor modulating alcohol toxicity using Drosophila melanogaster, a model for studies of sleep, alcohol, and aging. Following 24 h of sleep deprivation using a paradigm that similarly affects males and females and induces rebound sleep, flies were given binge-like alcohol exposures. Sleep deprivation increased mortality, with no sex-dependent differences. Sleep deprivation also abolished functional tolerance measured at 24 h after the initial alcohol exposure, although there was no effect on alcohol absorbance or clearance. We investigated the effect of chronic sleep deprivation using mutants with decreased sleep, insomniac and insulin-like peptide 2, finding increased alcohol mortality. Furthermore, we investigated whether pharmacologically inducing sleep prior to alcohol exposure using the GABAA-receptor agonist 4,5,6,7-tetrahydroisoxazolo(5,4-c)pyridin-3-ol (THIP) mitigated the effects of alcohol toxicity on middle-aged flies, flies with environmentally disrupted circadian clocks, and flies with short sleep. Pharmacologically increasing sleep prior to alcohol exposure decreased alcohol-induced mortality. Thus, sleep prior to binge-like alcohol exposure affects alcohol-induced mortality, even in vulnerable groups such as aging flies and those with circadian dysfunction.

1. Introduction

Alcohol abuse and its associated pathologies are pervasive societal problems with serious negative impacts on individual health, family structure, and the economy [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8]. In the United States, alcohol use disorders account for 79% of all diagnoses of substance use disorders [9], and the economic impact of alcohol misuse is estimated at USD 249 billion annually [7,10]. Alcohol abuse and alcohol pathologies appear higher in populations in which sleep deprivation is common, including teenagers, young adults, shift workers, and aged individuals [11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18]. Although considerable behavioral research has demonstrated the effects of alcohol on sleep homeostasis [19,20,21], surprisingly little is known about the role of sleep in modulating alcohol sensitivity and toxicity at the physiological level. Sleep impairments are traditionally viewed as symptoms of alcohol use disorders; however, sleep disorders increase the incidence and risk of relapse in recovering alcoholics [22,23,24,25]. Sleep deprivation represents a significant rising public health problem in the United States and the world [26,27,28,29]. The pervasiveness of factors contributing to sleep disruptions, including artificial light at night, the use of personal electronics, and the increase in shiftwork and extended work days [30,31], combined with the increased risk of substance abuse associated with sleep deprivation makes understanding how sleep deprivation affects alcohol-induced behaviors and toxicity imperative to identify and optimize therapies for the future prevention and treatment of alcohol-induced pathologies.
The high degree of physiological, molecular, and neurological conservation between the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster and mammals makes Drosophila an ideal model for the investigation of sleep and alcohol interactions [32,33,34]. Sleep in Drosophila occurs in stages, varying in intensity during the night with observable sex- and age-dependent differences [35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44]. As in other species, circadian and homeostatic processes regulate sleep in flies, with waking activity affecting sleep need [38,45]. Moreover, alcohol physiology is remarkably conserved from flies to humans with parallels in behaviors, as well as the underlying molecular mechanisms [46,47]. When exposed to alcohol vapor, initially flies exhibit hyperactivity with increased locomotor activity, followed by a loss in motor control and, eventually, sedation [48,49,50,51]. Flies also develop functional alcohol tolerance dependent upon changes in neural plasticity [50,52,53,54,55] and addiction-like behaviors [56,57,58], with a preference for alcohol following previous exposure [59,60].
We investigated the role of sleep deprivation on alcohol responses using a group sleep deprivation protocol. Group sleep deprivation increases rebound sleep in male and female flies. We find that 24 h of acute sleep deprivation increase behavioral sensitivity and mortality following acute and repeated exposure to alcohol. Furthermore, the effects of sleep deprivation eliminate sex-based differences in alcohol responses and alcohol-induced mortality. Approximately 72 h of recovery sleep is necessary to return alcohol responses to baseline following 24 h of acute sleep deprivation. These effects are independent of alcohol metabolism, as no differences are observed in alcohol absorption and clearance between sleep deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies. Sleep deprivation also inhibits the induction of long-term functional alcohol tolerance, observed 24 h following the first alcohol exposure in both male and female flies, although short-term tolerance measured 4 h following the first alcohol exposure is not affected. Chronic sleep restriction using short-sleep phenotype genetic mutants also increases alcohol-induced mortality. Encouragingly, we find that pharmacologically increasing sleep has the opposite effect of sleep deprivation, ameliorating alcohol mortality in middle-aged flies and flies with disrupted circadian clocks. This research highlights the critical role of sleep as a factor in alcohol toxicity.

2. Results

2.1. Sleep Deprivation Increases Sensitivity to Alcohol-Induced Sedation

Sleep deprivation appears to be a contributing factor to the increased use of alcohol, as suggested in studies of shift workers and young adults [61,62,63,64,65,66], although little is known about the effects of sleep loss on alcohol pathologies. In order to study the effect of sleep deprivation on alcohol sensitivity, it was necessary to develop a group-housed method of sleep deprivation. Flies in isolation with single housing exhibited a decreased number of synapses [67] and showed a reduced response to alcohol [68]. Furthermore, when males and females were separated and not allowed to mate, male flies showed increased alcohol preferences and changes in reward-signaling pathways [56]. Male and female sleep-deprived flies showed significant rebound sleep in the 3 h following sleep deprivation, as well as increased sleep during the night and the next day, compared to non-sleep-deprived flies (Figure 1A–C). Sleep-deprived flies using mechanical group sleep deprivation exhibited shorter sleep bouts with an increased number of sleep bouts, demonstrating sleep fragmentation for the first two days following sleep deprivation. The need for additional sleep was reduced by days 4 and 5, although small significant differences remained in the total sleep between sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies (Supplemental Figure S1). Thus, group-housed mechanical sleep deprivation was an effective and gentle method of sleep deprivation.
Drosophila (mixed sex, 10 d old) were sleep-deprived using mechanical sleep deprivation for 24 h (ZT 8–ZT 8) and then exposed to 50% alcohol vapor (1 h exposure; Figure 1D). We used approximately 10-day-old flies to facilitate the investigation of sleep deprivation on alcohol toxicity, as this age group of wild-type flies retains a robust circadian clock with circadian modulation of alcohol sensitivity and demonstrates measurable alcohol-induced mortality following a single binge-like alcohol exposure [69]. Sleep-deprived flies became sedated significantly faster than non-sleep-deprived age-matched controls, indicating that sleep loss increased behavioral sensitivity to alcohol (Figure 1E,F; t(14) = 13.46, p = 0.0002). Potentially, sleep-deprived flies could enter a deep sleep state resembling sedation, so we tested whether 24 h of sleep deprivation in flies exposed only to water vapor for 1 h resulted in sedation. We found virtually no sedation in the absence of exposure to alcohol, with no sedation occurring in three independent experiments (total of 136 flies tested with 0 sedation). In a fourth experiment, 2 flies out of 48 exhibited a sedation-like state 55 min after the experiment started. Thus, sleep deprivation alone did not result in a sedation state. To verify that the effects of mechanical sleep deprivation on sedation occurred through sleep deprivation, we sleep-deprived flies and then let them recover for either 48 or 72 h prior to alcohol exposure. We found that, by 72 h after acute sleep deprivation, there were no differences in sedation between sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies, whereas flies with only 48 h of recovery time still showed a significant reduction in the time necessary for 50% of the flies to become sedated with binge-like alcohol exposure (Figure 1G,H).
Although alcohol has been previously shown to differentially affect the rates of sedation and recovery in males and females, we hypothesized that 24 h of acute sleep deprivation would eliminate sex-based differences in alcohol sensitivity. To test this prediction, after the flies had mated, male and female flies were separated. Males and females were sleep-deprived (9–11 d old) for 24 h and then exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h with sedation assessed. We found that sleep deprivation eliminated the sex-based differences in alcohol sensitivity, with male and female groups exhibiting similar sedation rates (Figure 1I,J).
Given that the majority of Drosophila sleep studies have been performed using younger flies, we wanted to verify whether the effects of sleep deprivation on alcohol responses were similar in younger flies. We found that 24 h of acute sleep deprivation significantly increased alcohol sensitivity in both male and female 5–6 d old flies, as shown by considerably the reduced times for 50% of the flies to reach sedation (Supplemental Figure S2A–C). No sex-specific differences were observed in the sensitivity of the flies to alcohol after sedation. We also performed experiments using 3 d old mixed-sex populations, finding that 24 h of sleep deprivation in young flies also increased alcohol sensitivity, with flies showing significantly reduced times for sedation (Supplemental Figure S2D–F). Thus, these experiments demonstrated that sleep deprivation of group-housed flies was effective for both increasing homeostatic sleep pressure and eliminating sex-based differences in alcohol responses.

2.2. Sleep Deprivation Increases Acute and Chronic Alcohol Toxicity

Excessive binge drinking escalates the incidence of alcohol-poisoning deaths [70,71]. Therefore, it is important to understand the potential confounding effects of sleep loss on alcohol toxicity. We tested the effect of a single exposure to 50% alcohol vapor on mortality. Flies (10 d, mixed sex) were sleep-deprived for 24 h (ZT 8–ZT 8) and exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for one h at ZT 9 (Figure 2A). Nearly 100% of the flies became sedated under this protocol. Mortality was assessed at 24 h and 7 days following alcohol exposure. When exposed to alcohol vapor following sleep deprivation, flies showed significant mortality 24 h after alcohol exposure compared to non-sleep-deprived flies exposed to alcohol vapor or flies that were sleep-deprived and exposed to water vapor (Figure 2B, ANOVA: F3,28 = 22.50, p < 0.0001 and Figure 2C; ANOVA: F3,28 = 14.01, p < 0.0001). Non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived flies exposed to water vapor alone had negligible levels of mortality at either 24 h or 7 days, suggesting that 24 h of sleep deprivation itself did not result in mortal injury to the flies (Figure 2B,C). Exposure to alcohol vapor caused a noticeable but not significant rise in both immediate and delayed mortality compared to water vapor controls (Figure 2B,C). These results suggest that sleep deprivation exacerbated the acute toxicity of alcohol, with primary mortality observed within 24 h of alcohol exposure. To verify that the effects of sleep deprivation also eliminated sex-based differences in alcohol-induced mortality, separated male and female groups of flies were exposed to a single binge-like alcohol exposure (50% alcohol, vapor, 1 h) with mortality assessed. We found that mortality at 24 h after alcohol exposure was similar in sleep-deprived male and female groups (Figure 2E). In younger flies aged 5–6 d old, sleep deprivation also induced significant levels of mortality measured at 24 h after a single alcohol exposure (Supplemental Figure S3). We next investigated the effects sleep deprivation prior to a repeat binge alcohol exposure paradigm. As previously described, flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h (ZT 8–ZT 8) and then exposed to 40% alcohol vapor for 1 h (ZT 9) on three consecutive days (Figure 2F–I). Perhaps not surprisingly, the first alcohol exposure after sleep deprivation induced a significant increase in mortality (Figure 2G, ANOVA: F3,76 = 15.42, p < 0.0001). Alcohol-induced mortality was not significantly higher following the second and third alcohol exposures (Figure 2H), potentially due to the opportunity for recovery sleep following the first exposure to alcohol. The degree of mortality observed at 7 days following the last alcohol exposure was similar between the acute and repeated binge alcohol paradigms (Figure 2I, ANOVA: F3,76 = 19.91, p < 0.0001). Thus, sleep deprivation significantly increased alcohol toxicity in males and females, making acute sleep deprivation a potential risk factor for increased alcohol pathologies.

2.3. Sleep Deprivation Does Not Affect the Rate of Alcohol Clearance

Potentially, the increases in alcohol sensitivity and mortality observed following sleep deprivation were due to increased alcohol absorption or a decline in the rate of alcohol clearance, resulting in greater alcohol exposure and subsequent toxicity. To investigate this possibility, flies were sleep-deprived as previously described and exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 30 min at ZT 9, and alcohol absorbance was measured (Figure 3A). There were no significant differences in alcohol absorbance or clearance between sleep-deprived flies and non-sleep-deprived flies (Figure 3B). These results suggest that potential metabolic changes due to sleep deprivation did not account for the observed increased sensitivity to alcohol, with the more likely possibilities including sleep-deprivation-induced changes in neuroadaptation at the molecular or cellular levels.

2.4. Chronic Sleep Deprivation Induces Increased Alcohol-Induced Mortality

Chronic sleep deprivation comprised of multiple short-sleep nights may be a predisposing factor for increased alcohol consumption and other recreational drug use [18,72]. In the United States, approximately 70 million Americans suffer from chronic sleep loss with serious consequences for health and longevity, as well as economic productivity [29,73,74]. To investigate the effects of chronic sleep restriction on alcohol neurobiology, we employed a genetic approach rather than a mechanical system to induce sleep deprivation to avoid the possibility of stress arising from long-term mechanical stimulation. Numerous mutants with short-sleep phenotypes have been identified in Drosophila. However, the circadian clock also regulates aspects of sleep and sleep timing, and many sleep mutants have circadian phenotypes. Given previous research demonstrating circadian modulation of alcohol sensitivity and increased alcohol-induced mortality with circadian disruption [48,69], we used the mutant insomniac that has normal circadian rhythms but exhibits a short-sleep phenotype [75] to investigate the effects of chronic sleep restriction on alcohol toxicity. Insomniac (inc) is a mutation in a putative adaptor protein for the Cullin-3 ubiquitin ligase complex [75]. inc1 and inc2 mutant lines have a 90% reduction in inc transcript mRNA levels, with no detectable protein produced [75]. Confirming previously published results, we found that that inc1 and inc2 male flies (Figure 4A–C) exhibited considerable reductions in total sleep time, with inc1 flies sleeping a little over 300 min per day and inc2 flies sleeping approximately 600 min per day (Figure 4A, ANOVA: F2,67 = 81.13, p < 0.0001). These mutants exhibited significantly shortened sleep bouts (Figure 4B, ANOVA: F2,67 = 17.52, p < 0.0001), reflecting a decrease in sleep consolidation, although they did have a greater number of sleep bouts (Figure 4C, ANOVA: F2,67 = 8.64, p < 0.0001).
We exposed 10 d old inc1 and inc2 flies to 50% alcohol for 1 h at ZT 9 with sedation assessed at 5 min intervals (Figure 4D). Surprisingly, inc1 and inc2 flies did not exhibit increased sensitivity to alcohol; indeed, these mutants were more resistant to the sedating effects of alcohol, with significantly longer times to reach 50% sedation than the w1118 control flies (Figure 4D,E, ANOVA: F2,34 = 47.28, p < 0.0001, with post hoc analysis identifying significant differences between w1118 vs. inc1 and w1118 vs. inc2). These results suggest that either compensatory mechanisms existed to buffer against increased sensitivity to alcohol in these mutants or chronic sleep loss associated with the disruption in the Cullin-3 ubiquitin ligase complex did not increase alcohol sensitivity.
While the chronic sleep deficit associated with the disruption in the Cullin-3 ubiquitin ligase complex in the inc mutants did not increase alcohol sensitivity, we hypothesized that it would still increase alcohol toxicity, as alcohol affects multiple signaling pathways, both in the central nervous system and in peripheral tissues. To test this, we gave 10 d inc1 and inc2 flies a single exposure to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 and assessed mortality at 24 h and 7 d following the alcohol exposure (Figure 4G). Both inc1 and inc2 flies exhibited significantly higher mortality immediately (24 h), as well as 7 d following the exposure, compared to w1118 background controls (Figure 4H,I, ANOVA: F2,29 = 16.46, p < 0.0001). Given that the inc mutants are postulated to have defects in ubiquitination that may affect many target proteins and signaling pathways, it is possible that the observed mortality was due to other consequences of the mutation and not to the effects of chronic sleep restriction on alcohol toxicity.
As the interference with the ubiquitin ligase complex in the inc mutants could affect other pathways influencing alcohol sensitivity, we wanted to confirm these results with another genetic mutant that exhibits a reduced sleep phenotype but a normal circadian clock. Insulin-like peptide 2 (ilp2) is one of seven insulin-like peptides present in Drosophila, although there is only one insulin-like receptor identified [76,77]. Although there are spatial and temporal differences in expression between the Dilp genes, there appears to be a high degree of compensation and functional redundancy, such that knockdown of an individual Dilp does not significantly affect insulin- or igf-signaling pathways [77]. Dilp2 mutants exhibit reduced sleep, although the function of the circadian system appears to be relatively preserved [76], making Dilp2 a good candidate to test the effects of reduced sleep on alcohol toxicity. We verified that Dilp2 mutant flies (male and female) exhibited a reduced sleep phenotype compared to w1118 background controls with significantly shorter bout lengths, although the total bout numbers were similar (Figure 5A–C). The majority of Dilp2 flies demonstrated circadian locomotor activity, with 69.3% of the mutant flies (n = 36) exhibiting circadian rhythms with a period length of 23.51 ± 0.08 h compared with w1118 control flies (n = 61), in which 93% were rhythmic with a period length of 23.75 ± 0.04 h. We exposed 9-10 d old Dilp2 flies to 50% alcohol for 1 h at ZT 9 with sedation assessed at 5 min intervals (Figure 5D). Similar to inc mutants, Dilp2 flies did not show a greater sensitivity to alcohol, with similar sedation rates observed compared to w1118 control flies (Figure 5E,F). However, Dilp2 mutants had significantly higher levels of mortality at 24 h and 7 days after a single binge-like alcohol exposure compared to age-matched w1118 control flies (Figure 5G–I). Together, the results from these studies indicate that separate mechanisms mediated behavioral sensitivity to alcohol and the alcohol’s toxic effects, whereby chronic sleep loss increased alcohol-induced mortality, but did not affect alcohol-induced sedation, unlike acute sleep deprivation.

2.5. Pharmacologically Increasing Sleep Ameliorates Alcohol-Induced Mortality in Populations with Sleep Phenotypes

Previously we found that circadian arrhythmia and aging significantly increased alcohol-induced mortality [69], mirroring issues found in human populations such as shift-workers and elderly people with sleep disturbances. If accrued sleep loss was the driving force for the observed alcohol-induced mortality in inc flies and Dilp2 mutants, we hypothesized that increasing sleep in these mutants should decrease mortality following exposure to alcohol. To pharmacologically increase sleep, inc1 and inc2 mutant flies were raised on standard Drosophila media for 9 d and then transferred to media containing the GABAA agonist THIP (also known as Gaboxadol), which has previously been shown to pharmacologically increase sleep in Drosophila [78,79,80]. THIP treatment significantly increased sleep in inc1 and inc2 male and female flies (Figure 6A). THIP treatment also resulted in more consolidated sleep, as seen in a smaller number of sleep bouts with an increased length of sleep in each bout (Figure 6B,C). Similarly, THIP treatment significantly increased sleep in w1118 control flies (Figure 7). In w1118 flies, THIP resulted in significantly fewer but longer sleep bouts, indicating more consolidated sleep (Figure 7). Flies with THIP-induced increased sleep demonstrated reduced sensitivity to alcohol compared to non-THIP-treated flies or groups in which THIP-treated flies were simultaneously subjected to mechanical sleep deprivation (Figure 6D–H). THIP pre-treatment significantly reduced mortality at 24 h and 7 d following alcohol exposure in both inc1 and inc2 mutant flies compared to non-THIP-exposed inc1 and inc2 mutants (Figure 6I,J, ANOVA: F3,44 = 13.27, p < 0.0001 and Figure 6K, ANOVA: F2,44 = 12.83, p < 0.0001, respectively). However, THIP has dual effects as an analgesic and anxiolytic and has been tested as a treatment for alcohol use disorders, as well as insomnia [81]. Potentially, as an agonist for GABAA receptors, THIP may affect alcohol–receptor interactions to affect mortality rather than through its pharmacological induction of sleep. To determine whether THIP interactions decreased alcohol-induced mortality by altering alcohol–receptor interactions rather than through increased sleep prior to alcohol exposure, we combined mechanical sleep deprivation with 24 h of THIP exposure and then assessed the response to a single binge-like alcohol exposure. Sleep-deprived inc1 and inc2 mutant flies on THIP media showed significantly increased alcohol-induced mortality when compared to inc1 and inc2 mutant flies on control media, although THIP treatment alone significantly decreased alcohol-induced mortality (Figure 6L,M). Thus, the mitigation of alcohol-induced mortality with THIP observed in the inc mutants appeared to occur through benefits arising from increased sleep.
Although Dilp2 mutants exhibited reduced sleep, the mutation affected different signaling pathways than the inc mutation. We found that, similar to inc mutants and w1118 control flies, THIP increased the total sleep in Dilp2 male and female flies (Figure 7A–C, females; Figure 7D–F, males; non-THIP-treated Dilp2 male and female data from Figure 5). THIP-treated Dilp2 and w1118 control flies exhibited similar rates of alcohol-induced sedation (Figure 7G–I). Flies with 24 h of THIP pre-treatment also showed a significant reduction in alcohol-induced mortality measured at 24 h and 7 d after a single alcohol exposure compared with non-THIP-treated flies (Figure 7J,K). These results were consistent with the hypothesis that increased sleep prior to binge-like alcohol exposure buffered the toxic effects of alcohol.

2.6. Pharmacologically Increasing Sleep Independent of Circadian Rhythmicity Decreases Alcohol-Induced Mortality

In Drosophila, the circadian clock can be rendered non-functional using environmental manipulation by housing the flies in constant light. Constant light (LL) is sufficient to dampen molecular oscillations and abolish circadian rhythms in locomotor activity, memory formation, and the rhythm in alcohol-induced loss-of-righting reflex [51,82,83,84,85,86,87]. We have previously shown that environmental disruption of circadian function exacerbates alcohol sensitivity and mortality [48,69]. Previously, we had hypothesized that the increased alcohol mortality observed in flies in LL was due to the disruption in the circadian clock. However, recent research on the effects of constant light has shown that, when very young flies starting at day 1 are maintained in constant light, flies demonstrate significantly increased sleep with longer sleep bout duration during the day and less sleep at night, presumably due to circadian dysfunction altering the timing of sleep. (Rodrigues et al. Free Radical Biology in the paper the reviewer mentioned). Along with a disrupted circadian clock, we found that 10 d CS flies in LL had significantly lower total sleep and, specifically, less sleep during the subjective night compared to 10 d CS flies in LD (Figure 8A, t(236) = 4.46, p < 0.0001; Figure 8B, ANOVA: F3,442 = 27.31, p < 0.0001), consistent with mis-timed sleep due to circadian dysfunction. Flies housed in LL had a significantly higher number of sleep bouts in both the subjective day and night (Figure 8C, ANOVA: F3,442 = 98.99, p < 0.0001), although the sleep bout length was significantly shorter than flies housed in LD, resulting in the decrease in total sleep (Figure 8D, ANOVA: F3,442 = 78.45, p < 0.0001). Thus, it is possible that alterations in sleep in LL flies, in addition to the disruption of the circadian clock, caused the changes in alcohol toxicity. To test the role of sleep, we first determined the degree to which THIP exposure increased sleep in flies housed in LL. As expected, flies housed with THIP-containing food in constant light slept significantly more than flies on regular Drosophila food in LL (Figure 8E–H, mean sleep time per day: t(233) = 29.43, p < 0.0001; mean sleep time, day vs. night, ANOVA: F3,463 = 437.1, p < 0.0001; number of sleep bouts, ANOVA: F3,463 = 358.1, p < 0.0001; mean sleep bout length, ANOVA: F3,463 = 277.6, p < 0.0001). To separate the role of sleep from circadian regulation in mediating alcohol toxicity following a repeat binge-like alcohol exposure, we increased sleep in flies in LL as they remained under conditions of circadian disruption. We used a repeated binge-like alcohol paradigm to achieve significant levels of mortality so that potential reductions in mortality due to THIP pre-treatment could be measured. LL 10 d flies were maintained on medium containing THIP for 48 h prior to the first of three exposures to 40% alcohol vapor and then switched to normal media (Figure 7I). LL flies with THIP pre-treatment and increased sleep had significantly lower mortality than those exposed to alcohol vapor alone (Figure 8J, ANOVA: F3,36 = 132.6, p < 0.0001). These results suggest that increased sleep was sufficient to ameliorate mortality following repeated binge-like alcohol exposure, even under conditions of circadian disruption.

2.7. Increasing Sleep Buffers Age-Related Susceptibility to Alcohol-Induced Mortality

Aging is accompanied by a breakdown in circadian rhythmicity at the cellular, metabolic, and physiological levels, as well as disruptions in sleep architecture [69,88,89,90,91,92]. In recent years, chronic and binge alcohol consumption in middle-aged and older adults has significantly increased [93,94] with more than 75% of alcohol-induced poisoning deaths occurring in these age groups [11,14]. More than 10% of older adults engage in binge-drinking behavior [93]. Given that the aging population is expected to double by 2050 [95], it is necessary to identify ways to treat or ameliorate alcohol toxicity in middle-aged and older individuals. In a previous study, we showed that aging exacerbated alcohol sensitivity and mortality [69]. Middle-aged flies (20 d) exhibit shorter sleep times compared to younger flies [40] (Figure 9A, t(147) = 6.54, p < 0.0001; Figure 9B, ANOVA: F3,294 = 38.12, p < 0.0001). While we found no differences in total sleep amount during the night between 10 and 20 d flies, 20 d flies had a significantly greater number of sleep bouts with shorter duration, reflecting decreases in sleep consolidation (Figure 9C, ANOVA: F3,294 = 19.21, p < 0.0001; Figure 9D, ANOVA: F3,294 = 38.12, p < 0.0001). We pharmacologically induced sleep in 20 d CS flies by housing them on 0.1 mg/mL THIP for 24 h, after which they were given 1 h of alcohol exposure for three consecutive days. THIP-fed 20 d flies slept significantly more than control 20 d flies (Figure 9E–H: avg total sleep/day: t(89) = 13.05, p < 0.0001; avg sleep time, day vs. night, ANOVA: F3,178 = 91.72, p < 0.0001; number of sleep bouts, ANOVA: F3,178 = 60.24, p < 0.0001; sleep bout length, ANOVA: F3,178 = 113.0, p < 0.0001). Middle-aged flies housed on THIP-containing food prior to repeated binge-like alcohol exposures had significantly lower rates of mortality than those exposed to alcohol alone (Figure 9J, ANOVA: F3,44 = 243.8, p < 0.0001). The decreased mortality observed in THIP-fed flies was not due to increased alcohol tolerance from THIP interactions, as 20 d flies given THIP for 1 h at ZT 7 followed by alcohol exposure at ZT 9 showed mortality rates similar to 20 d flies exposed to alcohol alone (Figure 9K, ANOVA: F3,30 = 23.09, p < 0.0001). These results suggest that increased sleep was sufficient to ameliorate mortality following repeated alcohol exposures in middle-aged flies that had both circadian and sleep disruption.

2.8. Sleep Deprivation Inhibits Long-Term but Not Short-Term Tolerance

Drosophila exhibit drug tolerance of repeated alcohol exposures in which the behavioral response to subsequent exposures to alcohol is lessened, similar to those observed in rodent models and humans. At the behavioral level, functional tolerance results in a decreased sensitivity to alcohol during subsequent exposures, with increased alcohol concentrations or longer alcohol exposures necessary to induce sedation [33,96,97]. In flies, rapid tolerance develops after a single alcohol exposure and can be observed during a second alcohol exposure 4 h or 24 h later [50,98]. The development of functional alcohol tolerance is dependent upon changes in neural plasticity rather than changes in the metabolism or clearance of alcohol [50,53,54,55,98,99]. Changes in neural plasticity associated with drug and alcohol tolerance share features in common with the synaptic plasticity observed in learning and memory [100,101,102]. Potentially, sleep loss affects the development of alcohol tolerance, as sleep deprivation disturbs memory formation, as seen across invertebrate and vertebrate species [103,104,105]. To investigate the effect of sleep deprivation on tolerance formed after a single alcohol exposure, 10 d wild-type flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h (ZT 3.5–ZT 3.5) and given a pre-exposure of 50% alcohol vapor for 30 min (ZT 4.5; Figure 10A). Pre-exposed sleep-deprived flies and sleep-deprived naïve flies were exposed to alcohol 4 h later at ZT 9, during which sedation was measured (Figure 10A). Similarly, non-sleep-deprived flies were pre-exposed to alcohol, with responses compared during a second alcohol exposure to naïve flies. Non-sleep-deprived flies demonstrated a robust 4 h alcohol tolerance, with significant increases observed in the time necessary for 50% of the flies to reach sedation compared to naïve flies (Figure 10B, ANOVA: F3,20 = 49.62, p < 0.0001; Figure 10C). Surprisingly, sleep-deprived flies also demonstrated a robust 4 h alcohol tolerance (Figure 10B,C), suggesting that sleep disruption did not affect the cellular-signaling mechanisms necessary for the formation of 4 h tolerance.
To determine the effect of sleep deprivation on the formation of long-term alcohol tolerance, flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h (ZT 7.5–ZT 7.5), given a pre-exposure of 50% alcohol vapor for 30 min (ZT 8.5), and tested 24 h later at ZT 9 (Figure 10D). Groups of non-sleep-deprived flies were handled concurrently. When flies were tested 24 h after the initial alcohol exposure, sleep-deprived flies demonstrated significantly less tolerance to alcohol with a time to sedation similar to sleep-deprived naïve flies, while non-sleep-deprived flies demonstrated a robust long-term tolerance with response times significantly different than naïve flies (Figure 10E, ANOVA: F3,26 = 125.7, p < 0.0001; Figure 10F). Although 24 h of acute sleep deprivation eliminated the sex-specific differences in alcohol sedation, it is possible that sex-specific differences occurred in the development or magnitude of functional alcohol tolerance. After mating for 72–96 h, we investigated the effects of sleep deprivation on 24 h tolerance in males and females maintained separately. We found that acute sleep deprivation inhibited long-term alcohol tolerance in both male and females, with no differences observed between pre-exposed and naïve flies (Figure 10H,I).
Although our previous research found that tolerance was not modulated by the circadian clock, we verified the effect on long-term tolerance by exposing flies to alcohol at ZT 4.5, the same time that we observed the formation of 4 h tolerance in sleep-deprived flies (Supplemental Figure S5A). Sleep-deprived flies pre-exposed to alcohol at ZT 4.5 and then subsequently exposed to alcohol at ZT 9 the following day also exhibited little or no alcohol tolerance, while non-sleep-deprived flies exhibited significant long-term tolerance (Supplemental Figure S5B,C, ANOVA: F3,20 = 0.92, p = 0.4488). Flies (9–10 d old) were sleep-deprived for 24 h, and thus, acute sleep deprivation prior to alcohol exposure inhibited the expression of alcohol tolerance 24 h following the initial alcohol pre-exposure, while no effect was observed on the development of short-term tolerance expressed 4 h after the initial exposure. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that different molecular mechanisms are behind the development of short-term and long-term rapid alcohol tolerance, similar to differences in the formation of short- and long-term memory.

3. Materials and Methods

3.1. Fly Maintenance

All flies were maintained on standard cornmeal-molasses food at 25 °C and 60–70% relative humidity in 12:12 light:dark (LD) cycles. Canton S (CS) flies were used as the wild-type strain. Insomniac (inc) mutants and the background w1118 line were generously provided by Dr. Nicholas Stavropoulos, New York University. Adult flies (~30–40 per vial) were transferred approximately every 3–4 days to maintain stress-free cultures. All experiments were carried out in an environmentally controlled dark room at 25 °C and 60–70% relative humidity under dim red light. Zeitgeber time (ZT) 0 represented lights on, and ZT 12 corresponded with lights off. For experiments performed in constant light (LL) conditions, flies were transferred to LL on the day of eclosion.

3.2. Alcohol Exposure

Alcohol vapor exposure was performed as previously described [51,69,106]. Four tubes, each containing ~30 flies, received a steady flow of ethanol vapor at a pre-determined percentage. Precise alcohol percentages were achieved by mixing air bubbled through deionized water and 95% ethanol (Koptec, Decon Labs, Inc. King of Prussia, PA, USA). Air flow rates were monitored throughout the experiment to ensure consistency of alcohol concentration. Water vapor controls were run simultaneously with 100% water vapor. Alcohol exposures were performed from ZT 8 to ZT 10 to avoid circadian variation in responses unless otherwise stated for a specific protocol.

3.3. Sleep Deprivation

Consistent sleep deprivation was achieved using gentle mechanical stimulation on a GyroMini Nutating Mixer (Labnet International, Inc. Edison, NJ, USA). Vials containing 30–50 flies were placed at an angled position from the vertical center in a larger beaker with a raised block at a fixed position protruding inside the beaker on the mini gyrator. Mixer rotation caused the vials to rotate within the beaker and then gently jump over the raised block, providing the flies with a startle movement every 2.5 s. The constant motion of the vials combined with the startle ensured consistent sleep deprivation, with no apparent injuries or increased mortality observed after 24 h of sleep deprivation. Sleep deprivation was performed in an incubator under 25 °C, 60–70% relative humidity, and 12:12 LD conditions. Non-sleep-deprived controls were housed in the same incubator.

3.4. Sedation

Alcohol-induced sedation was performed as previously described [48]. Briefly, flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for one hour with observations of behavioral state made every five minutes following a gentle tap of the vial. Flies were scored as sedated when immobile and lacking coordinated leg movements except for spontaneous twitching [52]. The mean time to 50% sedation was calculated using a linear extrapolation.

3.5. Tolerance

Tolerance was determined as previously described [51]. Flies received a pre-exposure of 50% alcohol for 30 min at ZT 4.5 following a one-hour dark room acclimation period. Sedation was assessed during the pre-exposure. Flies were then returned to food vials to allow time for recovery and complete metabolism of the alcohol before testing. Testing occurred 4 h later at ZT 9 for short-term rapid tolerance. For 24 h tolerance, flies were sleep-deprived between ZT 8 and ZT 8 and then exposed to alcohol at ZT 9. Testing occurred 24 h later for long-term rapid tolerance, with all experimental groups within the experiment represented at each test. Tolerance was defined as an increase in average time to reach 50% sedation from the pre-exposure with the difference in sedation time between naïve and pre-exposed flies used for quantification.

3.6. Mortality

Following each alcohol exposure, flies were returned to food vials placed horizontally for approximately 2 h to allow recovery of postural control. Immediate mortality was assessed 24 h following the last alcohol exposure and then daily for 6 days. Delayed mortality referred to the cumulative mortality within seven days of the final alcohol exposure. For some experiments, a repetitive alcohol exposure protocol was used to assess alcohol-induced mortality as described previously [69].

3.7. Gaboxadol Treatment

Sleep was pharmacologically increased with the GABA-A agonist, 4,5,6,7-tetrahydroisoxazolo [5,4-c]pyridin-3-ol (THIP or Gaboxadol). Flies that were 10 d old (Inc mutants, Dilp2 mutants, wild-type in constant light) or 20 d old (in LD) flies were transferred to Gaboxadol-containing food (0.1 mg/mL) for either 24 h or 48 h depending on the experiment prior to alcohol exposure. Flies were transferred back to non-THIP-containing media immediately prior to the 1 h habituation that preceded alcohol exposure.

3.8. Alcohol Absorbance

Following 24 h of sleep deprivation, batches of 20 flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 30 min at ZT 9, after which they were frozen at 0, 0.5, 1, 2, or 4 h following alcohol exposure. Alcohol absorbance was measured using an enzymatic alcohol dehydrogenase assay (ADH-NAD kit; Sigma-Aldrich, Burlington, MA, USA) per the manufacturer’s directions and as described previously [49,51]. Briefly, flies were homogenized in 200 uL refrigerated Tris-HCl (pH 7.5) buffer. Homogenate was spun at 15,000× g for 20 min at 4 °C. An amount of 250 uL NAD-ADH reagent was added to a 5 μL aliquot of supernatant. Absorbance was measured at 340 nm within 20 min using a 96-well plate format and a Versa-Max plate reader (Molecular Devices). Alcohol absorbance was normalized to total protein to eliminate the effect of body size variation between batches of flies.

3.9. Locomotor Activity Rhythms

Sleep activity was monitored using Drosophila activity monitors (Trikinetics, Waltham, MA, USA) as described previously [82]. To test the effectiveness of group sleep deprivation, following 24 h of sleep deprivation ending at ZT 8, flies were transferred to individual Trikinetics activity tubes with control media (1.20% agar and 5% sucrose), and sleep was measured compared to non-sleep-deprived flies transferred from group housing vials in the same manner at the same time. Similar procedures were followed for flies entrained to LL at 25 °C and for flies exposed to THIP-infused media. Sleep activity was recorded for 4 to 5 days, with data analyzed using either ClockLab or Shiny R-DAM.

3.10. Statistics

Statistics were performed using GraphPad Prism Version 6.0. Experimental groups were compared using analysis of variance (ANOVA). Post hoc analyses in multiple comparisons were performed using the Bonferroni correction.

4. Discussion

Research from our lab and others has suggested a bidirectional relationship between circadian clock dysfunction and the onset and severity of alcohol-related pathologies [18,48,51,107,108]. Social jet lag, or large shifts in sleep timing between the weekday and the weekend, is observed in numerous populations, including individuals on shift and rotating schedules [109,110], and is strongly correlated with increased alcohol use [111,112]. Due to long working hours, rotating schedules, and work-associated stress, many individuals report using alcohol as a sleep aid [61,62,113,114], which can eventually lead to an increased number of binge-drinking episodes and other detrimental effects associated with alcohol abuse [61,62,64,115]. Previous studies from our lab found that the circadian clock modulated alcohol sensitivity and toxicity and that circadian dysfunction significantly increased behavioral sensitivity to alcohol and mortality following acute and repeated alcohol exposures [48,51]. In humans, differences in individual chronotype also appear to modulate alcohol use and its associated pathologies. Individuals expressing an “evening chronotype” report significantly increased alcohol use [116,117,118,119,120,121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128]. Interestingly, individuals with an evening chronotype also have lower quality of sleep and increased daytime fatigue [129,130]. However, it is difficult to detangle the effects of circadian dysfunction from the effects of altered sleep on alcohol use.
Sleep disorders and sleep disturbances have become increasingly prevalent in modern society with longer working hours, irregular work schedules, and the prevalence of electronics, affecting more than 35% of adults and 70% of teenagers [27,73,131,132,133,134]. Insufficient sleep exacerbates the risk of developing chronic diseases and health problems, including cancer, diabetes, and neurodegenerative and psychiatric disorders [135,136,137,138,139]. Consequently, we investigated the effects of sleep loss on alcohol sensitivity and toxicity using Drosophila melanogaster to dissect the interactions between sleep deprivation and alcohol sensitivity and mortality. For this research, it was important to adopt a group sleep deprivation approach, as animals across phylogeny in social isolation demonstrate behavioral alterations and changes in synaptic structure and number [140]. In Drosophila, social isolation has been found to decrease synapse number in PDF neurons and reduce alcohol sensitivity [67,68]. Studies using rodent models have shown that social isolation increases alcohol preferences [141,142,143,144]. Therefore, we used a group-housed sleep deprivation method that significantly increased homeostatic sleep pressures in both male and females, as seen by increased sleep in the 36 h following the period of sleep deprivation.
We found that group-housed flies with acute (24 h) sleep deprivation had significantly increased sensitivity and mortality following a single binge-like exposure to alcohol. Moreover, 24 h of sleep deprivation eliminated the differential alcohol responses in sensitivity normally observed between males and females. Increased mortality following alcohol exposure occurred primarily within 24 h following alcohol exposure, with similar results observed between males and females. These effects were independent of stress or injury, as 72 h of recovery sleep prior to alcohol returned alcohol-induced behavioral responses to baseline levels with no differences seen between sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies. The increases in sensitivity and mortality were also independent of changes in metabolic tolerance, as there were no differences between sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies in alcohol absorbance or clearance. It should be noted that these experiments were all performed at approximately the same time of day (ZT 8–ZT 10), as the circadian clock regulates both alcohol-induced sedation and mortality in flies [51,69]. As ZT 9 appeared to be the time at which flies were least sensitive to alcohol, it is possible that the magnitude of the effects of sleep deprivation may differ at other times of the day or night. However, given the robustness of the effects of sleep deprivation, it is likely that sleep deprivation overrides the circadian modulation of alcohol response, as was seen with male and female differences. Thus, sleep deprivation changed both immediate alcohol sensitivity and acute alcohol toxicity after a single binge-like alcohol exposure. Our data highlighted the phylogenetic conservation across species, showing a correlation between sleep loss and alcohol behaviors.
Studies from rodents and humans outline a correlation between sleep loss and increased severity of alcohol behavioral responses, including increased alcohol intake, accelerated development of alcohol abuse, dependence, and relapse following alcohol abstinence [145,146,147,148]. In mice, alcohol dose-dependently increased hyperactive locomotor activity in open-field tests, with acute sleep deprivation for 48 h abolishing these stimulatory effects [149]. Insufficient sleep (<8 h per night) is correlated with an increased number of drinking sessions in adolescents and young adults [150,151,152]. College-aged students are considered a vulnerable population for risk-taking behaviors, and multiple studies have shown a strong correlation between poor sleep quality and excessive alcohol intake and the accompanying consequences for mental health and academic performance, including increased rates of depression, anxiety, and psychological stress, as well as academic issues in these students [63,153,154]. Insufficient and poor-quality sleep also appear to predict the onset of alcohol abuse and its adverse consequences [65,155,156,157,158]. Sleep disturbances observed in children 3–5 years of age predicted the early onset of alcohol use at ages 12–14 [159]. This is particularly harmful because recovering alcoholics who use alcohol as a sleep aid are three times more likely to relapse in 12 months [22,23,160]. Altogether, these studies emphasize disturbed sleep as a potent risk factor for the initiation of alcohol use, the escalation of problems associated with alcohol abuse, and the hindrance of recovery from alcohol-use disorders.
With the genetic tools and mutants available, Drosophila provided a suitable model system to test the relationship between chronic sleep disturbances and alcohol-induced pathologies. Using flies with mutations in the insomniac gene (inc) that provided a model mirroring chronic sleep restriction, we found that inc mutants had significantly increased mortality following alcohol exposure than background controls. Inc mutant flies were surprisingly less sensitive to the sedative effects of alcohol compared to their background controls, supporting previous research that the different physiological consequences of alcohol can be regulated separately. Although the mechanism through which sleep buffers alcohol toxicity is unknown, it is possible that the changes in oxidative stress in the inc mutant flies may contribute to the change in alcohol toxicity. Inc1 and inc2 mutant flies have a reduced lifespan compared to genetic background controls, with inc1 flies having a maximum lifespan of approximately 50 days, inc2 mutants with an approximate 60-day maximum, and wild-type flies with an approximate 70-day maximum lifespan [75]. The inc gene seems necessary for mediating the oxidative stress response, as reducing inc both globally and neuronally significantly increases mortality following a single injection to the paraquat, a common inducer of oxidative stress [75,161]. Support for this hypothesis is found in previous research demonstrating that pharmacologically increasing sleep in inc mutant flies using gaboxadol significantly decreased the sensitivity to paraquat-induced oxidative stress [161]. Sleep loss was also shown to increase reactive oxygen species in the gut [154], raising the possibility that peripheral mechanisms also contribute to increased alcohol toxicity. As changes in sleep potentially impact multiple physiological processes in the central nervous system, as well as in peripheral organs, the precise mechanism through which sleep buffers alcohol toxicity is undoubtedly the focus of future studies.
To independently examine the effects of short sleep on alcohol mortality, we used another short-sleep mutant that maintained a functional circadian clock, Dilp2. Although Dilp2 mutants exhibit slight developmental delays, these fly mutants are long-lived, with between an 8 and 13% increase in lifespan observed in both male and female flies [77]. The insulin-like peptides, Dilp2, 3, and 5 are expressed in the median neurosecretory cells of the adult fly brain [77,162]. In Dilp2 mutant flies, Dilp3 and Dilp5 are upregulated with a large degree of compensation apparent [77]. The role of insulin signaling in alcohol responses was previously investigated, reporting that flies with reduced levels of the insulin receptor demonstrated an increased sensitivity to alcohol [163]. However, we found that Dilp2 mutants had no significant difference in the rate or sensitivity to alcohol-induced sedation, probably due to compensation from other insulin-like peptides in Drosophila. Dilp2 mutants had significantly higher levels of alcohol-induced mortality compared to background controls. Similar to the inc mutants, we found that increasing sleep in Dilp2 flies using gaboxadol significantly mitigated alcohol-induced mortality. Previously, researchers found that overexpression of Dilp5 or Dilp6 ameliorated developmental alcohol toxicity in larvae, whereas overexpression of Dilp2 had no effect on alcohol toxicity unless it was ectopically expressed throughout the animal [164].
Pharmacologically increasing sleep in flies maintained in LL with dysfunctional circadian clocks and middle-aged wild-type flies was sufficient to significantly reduce alcohol-induced mortality. Gaboxadol increased the total sleep duration, as well as significantly increasing sleep bout length, suggesting a greater consolidation of sleep. Both increased total sleep and increased sleep consolidation suggest that improved sleep quality could aid in mitigating alcohol-induced pathologies. Although there have been few studies examining the relationship between sleep health and alcohol toxicity, sleep loss and decreased sleep consolidation have been shown to reduce reproductive output, accelerate aging, and increase the accumulation of reactive oxygen species and death in flies [165,166]. In humans, increasing sleep in adolescents was correlated with decreased risk of emotional and cognitive disruption, as well as lowered risk of obesity [167]. In addition, increasing sleep by 30 min for 3 days over the weekend in healthy industrial workers and individuals susceptible to obesity significantly increased insulin sensitivity and had a restorative effect of sleep on metabolic homeostasis [168,169]. Finally, increasing sleep in older adults significantly improved performance on visual tasks and stabilized memory recall [170]. Although more specific research needs to be conducted assessing the direct effects of increased sleep on alcohol toxicity in vulnerable groups, these data suggest a role for sleep as a buffer to protect against the toxic effects of alcohol in populations vulnerable to chronic sleep loss, such as aged adults and shift workers.
The development of acute tolerance to alcohol is a distinct and critical behavioral metric used to gauge propensity for alcohol dependence and abuse [171], which can be separated from alcohol sensitivity and alcohol toxicity. Similar to mammals, acute exposure to a high concentration of alcohol induces functional tolerance in Drosophila at the behavioral [50,98,172] and the molecular levels [99,173,174,175,176]. Functional alcohol tolerance is dependent on changes in neuronal strength and connectivity or synaptic plasticity [99,173,174]. Consistent with previous findings, we observed tolerance at 4 h and 24 h following a short pre-exposure to alcohol vapor [50,51]. We found that sleep deprivation eliminated or dampened the development of tolerance at 24 h in both male and female flies but had no effect on tolerance observed at 4 h. Potentially, acute sleep deprivation selectively impaired the cellular and molecular processes necessary for encoding long-term rapid tolerance to alcohol without severe disruption of the mechanisms necessary for the development of 4 h tolerance. In fact, previous studies have demonstrated altered expressions of rapid tolerance in flies with mutations in genes necessary for learning and memory [46,98,177]. For example, the gene dunce (dnc) encodes a phosophodiesterase required for cAMP degradation and is necessary for behavioral and synaptic plasticity [178,179]. Originally identified as a learning mutant [180,181], dnc mutant flies exhibit significant sleep deficits [182] and are incapable of forming rapid tolerance [183,184]. Time-dependent differences in the effects of sleep deprivation can also be seen for memory with acute sleep deprivation, affecting the consolidation of long-term but not short-term hippocampal-dependent memory in mice [185,186]. Together with support from existing research, the results from our studies suggest that sleep deprivation selectively impacted processes underlying synaptic plasticity to affect the development of long-term rapid tolerance. In conclusion, the results from our study started to dissociate the role of sleep in modulating alcohol toxicity from the regulation of alcohol neurobiology by the circadian clock. These results lay the groundwork for future studies and treatments considering sleep quality and sleep duration as an important component of alcohol use disorder and alcohol-induced pathologies.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijms232012091/s1.

Author Contributions

Individual authors made the following contributions. Study design and methodology, L.C.L. and E.J.N.; data analysis, A.K.D.N., E.J.N. and L.C.L.; data acquisition, E.J.N., N.A.S., A.P.M. and L.C.L.; manuscript writing and figure preparation, A.K.D.N., N.A.S. and L.C.L.; funding acquisition, L.C.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health, as well as the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (grant R21AA021233).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data underlying this article are available in the article and in its online supplementary material. Detailed data for individual samples will be shared on reasonable request to the corresponding author. An early version of this manuscript (the authors’ original version) prior to peer review may be found in bioRxiv.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Esser, M.B.; Hedden, S.L.; Kanny, D.; Brewer, R.D.; Gfroerer, J.C.; Naimi, T.S. Prevalence of alcohol dependence among US adult drinkers, 2009–2011. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2014, 11, E206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Esser, M.B.; Sherk, A.; Liu, Y.; Naimi, T.S.; Stockwell, T.; Stahre, M.; Kanny, D.; Landen, M.; Saitz, R.; Brewer, R.D. Deaths and Years of Potential Life Lost From Excessive Alcohol Use-United States, 2011–2015. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2020, 69, 981–987. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Grant, B.F.; Dawson, D.A.; Stinson, F.S.; Chou, S.P.; Dufour, M.C.; Pickering, R.P. The 12-month prevalence and trends in DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence: United States, 1991–1992 and 2001–2002. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2004, 74, 223–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Grant, B.F.; Stinson, F.S.; Dawson, D.A.; Chou, S.P.; Ruan, W.J.; Pickering, R.P. Co-occurrence of 12-month alcohol and drug use disorders and personality disorders in the United States: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2004, 61, 361–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Kesmodel, U.; Wisborg, K.; Olsen, S.F.; Henriksen, T.B.; Secher, N.J. Moderate alcohol intake during pregnancy and the risk of stillbirth and death in the first year of life. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2002, 155, 305–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Leonard, K.E.; Rothbard, J.C. Alcohol and the marriage effect. J. Stud. Alcohol Suppl. 1999, 13, 139–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Sacks, J.J.; Gonzales, K.R.; Bouchery, E.E.; Tomedi, L.E.; Brewer, R.D. 2010 National and State Costs of Excessive Alcohol Consumption. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2015, 49, e73–e79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Stahre, M.; Roeber, J.; Kanny, D.; Brewer, R.D.; Zhang, X. Contribution of excessive alcohol consumption to deaths and years of potential life lost in the United States. Prev. Chronic Dis. 2014, 11, E109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. SAMSHA. Results from the 2013 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. Summary of National Findings; NSDUH Series H-48; HHS Publication No. (SMA) 14-4863 (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration): Rockville, MD, USA, 2014.
  10. (HHS) UDoHaHS. Facing addiction in America: The surgeon general’s report on alcohol, drugs, and health. In General OotS; SMA 16-4991 (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration): Rockville, MD, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  11. Blazer, D.G.; Wu, L.T. The epidemiology of at-risk and binge drinking among middle-aged and elderly community adults: National Survey on Drug Use and Health. Am. J. Psychiatry 2009, 166, 1162–1169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Bushnell, P.T.; Colombi, A.; Caruso, C.C.; Tak, S. Work schedules and health behavior outcomes at a large manufacturer. Ind. Health 2010, 48, 395–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Hasler, B.P.; Soehner, A.M.; Clark, D.B. Sleep and circadian contributions to adolescent alcohol use disorder. Alcohol 2015, 49, 377–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Kendler, K.S.; Ohlsson, H.; Sundquist, J.; Sundquist, K. Alcohol Use Disorder and Mortality Across the Lifespan: A Longitudinal Cohort and Co-relative Analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 2016, 73, 575–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Lee, J.; Manousakis, J.; Fielding, J.; Anderson, C. Alcohol and sleep restriction combined reduces vigilant attention, whereas sleep restriction alone enhances distractibility. Sleep 2015, 38, 765–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Novier, A.; Diaz-Granados, J.L.; Matthews, D.B. Alcohol use across the lifespan: An analysis of adolescent and aged rodents and humans. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2015, 133, 65–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Novier, A.; Ornelas, L.C.; Diaz-Granados, J.L.; Matthews, D.B. Differences in Behavioral Responding in Adult and Aged Rats Following Chronic Ethanol Exposure. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2016, 40, 1462–1472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Swanson, G.R.; Gorenz, A.; Shaikh, M.; Desai, V.; Kaminsky, T.; Berg, J.V.D.; Murphy, T.; Raeisi, S.; Fogg, L.F.; Vitaterna, M.H.; et al. Night workers with circadian misalignment are susceptible to alcohol-induced intestinal hyperpermeability with social drinking. Am. J. Physiol. Gastrointest. Liver Physiol. 2016, 311, G192–G201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Chakravorty, S.; Chaudhary, N.S.; Brower, K.J. Alcohol Dependence and Its Relationship With Insomnia and Other Sleep Disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2016, 40, 2271–2282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Colrain, I.M.; Nicholas, C.L.; Baker, F.C. Alcohol and the sleeping brain. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2014, 125, 415–431. [Google Scholar]
  21. Ebrahim, I.O.; Shapiro, C.M.; Williams, A.J.; Fenwick, P.B. Alcohol and sleep I: Effects on normal sleep. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2013, 37, 539–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Brower, K.J.; Aldrich, M.S.; Hall, J.M. Polysomnographic and subjective sleep predictors of alcoholic relapse. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1998, 22, 1864–1871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Brower, K.J.; Hall, J.M. Effects of age and alcoholism on sleep: A controlled study. J. Stud. Alcohol. 2001, 62, 335–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Hartwell, E.E.; Bujarski, S.; Glasner-Edwards, S.; Ray, L.A. The Association of Alcohol Severity and Sleep Quality in Problem Drinkers. Alcohol Alcohol. 2015, 50, 536–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Smith, N.; Hill, R.; Marshall, J.; Keaney, F.; Wanigaratne, S. Sleep related beliefs and their association with alcohol relapse following residential alcohol detoxification treatment. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 2014, 42, 593–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Buxton, O.M.; Marcelli, E. Short and long sleep are positively associated with obesity, diabetes, hypertension, and cardiovascular disease among adults in the United States. Soc. Sci. Med. 2010, 71, 1027–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Garbarino, S.; Lanteri, P.; Durando, P.; Magnavita, N.; Sannita, W.G. Co-Morbidity, Mortality, Quality of Life and the Healthcare/Welfare/Social Costs of Disordered Sleep: A Rapid Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Strine, T.W.; Chapman, D.P. Associations of frequent sleep insufficiency with health-related quality of life and health behaviors. Sleep Med. 2005, 6, 23–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Wheaton, A.G.; Olsen, E.O.; Miller, G.F.; Croft, J.B. Sleep Duration and Injury-Related Risk Behaviors Among High School Students—United States, 2007–2013. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2016, 65, 337–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Knutson, K.L.; Van Cauter, E.; Rathouz, P.J.; DeLeire, T.; Lauderdale, D.S. Trends in the prevalence of short sleepers in the USA: 1975–2006. Sleep 2010, 33, 37–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Roenneberg, T. Chronobiology: The human sleep project. Nature 2013, 498, 427–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Landayan, D.; Wolf, F.W. Shared neurocircuitry underlying feeding and drugs of abuse in Drosophila. Biomed. J. 2015, 38, 496–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Park, A.; Ghezzi, A.; Wijesekera, T.P.; Atkinson, N.S. Genetics and genomics of alcohol responses in Drosophila. Neuropharmacology 2017, 122, 22–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Tomita, J.; Ban, G.; Kume, K. Genes and neural circuits for sleep of the fruit fly. Neurosci. Res. 2017, 118, 82–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Andretic, R.; Shaw, P.J. Essentials of sleep recordings in Drosophila: Moving beyond sleep time. Methods Enzymol. 2005, 393, 759–772. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  36. Donlea, J.M. Neuronal and molecular mechanisms of sleep homeostasis. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 2017, 24, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Hendricks, J.C.; Finn, S.M.; Panckeri, K.A.; Chavkin, J.; Williams, J.A.; Sehgal, A.; Pack, A. Rest in Drosophila is a sleep-like state. Neuron 2000, 25, 129–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Huber, R.; Hill, S.L.; Holladay, C.; Biesiadecki, M.; Tononi, G.; Cirelli, C. Sleep homeostasis in Drosophila melanogaster. Sleep 2004, 27, 628–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Isaac, R.E.; Li, C.; Leedale, A.E.; Shirras, A.D. Drosophila male sex peptide inhibits siesta sleep and promotes locomotor activity in the post-mated female. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2010, 277, 65–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Koh, K.; Evans, J.M.; Hendricks, J.C.; Sehgal, A. A Drosophila model for age-associated changes in sleep:wake cycles. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 13843–13847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Robertson, M.; Keene, A.C. Molecular mechanisms of age-related sleep loss in the fruit fly—A mini-review. Gerontology 2013, 59, 334–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Shaw, P.J.; Cirelli, C.; Greenspan, R.J.; Tononi, G. Correlates of sleep and waking in Drosophila melanogaster. Science 2000, 287, 1834–1837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Van Alphen, B.; Yap, M.H.; Kirszenblat, L.; Kottler, B.; van Swinderen, B. A dynamic deep sleep stage in Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 6917–6927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Vienne, J.; Spann, R.; Guo, F.; Rosbash, M. Age-Related Reduction of Recovery Sleep and Arousal Threshold in Drosophila. Sleep 2016, 39, 1613–1624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Ganguly-Fitzgerald, I.; Donlea, J.; Shaw, P.J. Waking experience affects sleep need in Drosophila. Science 2006, 313, 1775–1781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Devineni, A.V.; McClure, K.; Guarnieri, D.; Corl, A.; Wolf, F.; Eddison, M.; Heberlein, U. The genetic relationships between ethanol preference, acute ethanol sensitivity, and ethanol tolerance in Drosophila melanogaster. Fly 2011, 5, 191–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Rodan, A.R.; Rothenfluh, A. The genetics of behavioral alcohol responses in Drosophila. Int. Rev. Neurobiol. 2010, 91, 25–51. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. De Nobrega, A.K.; Lyons, L.C. Circadian Modulation of Alcohol-Induced Sedation and Recovery in Male and Female Drosophila. J. Biol. Rhythm. 2016, 31, 142–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Moore, M.S.; DeZazzo, J.; Luk, A.Y.; Tully, T.; Singh, C.M.; Heberlein, U. Ethanol intoxication in Drosophila: Genetic and pharmacological evidence for regulation by the cAMP signaling pathway. Cell 1998, 93, 997–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Scholz, H.; Ramond, J.; Singh, C.M.; Heberlein, U. Functional ethanol tolerance in Drosophila. Neuron 2000, 28, 261–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Van der Linde, K.; Lyons, L.C. Circadian modulation of acute alcohol sensitivity but not acute tolerance in Drosophila. Chronobiol. Int. 2011, 28, 397–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Cowmeadow, R.B.; Krishnan, H.R.; Atkinson, N.S. The slowpoke gene is necessary for rapid ethanol tolerance in Drosophila. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2005, 29, 1777–1786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ghezzi, A.; Al-Hasan, Y.M.; Krishnan, H.R.; Wang, Y.; Atkinson, N.S. Functional mapping of the neuronal substrates for drug tolerance in Drosophila. Behav Genet. 2013, 43, 227–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Ghezzi, A.; Li, X.; Lew, L.K.; Wijesekera, T.P.; Atkinson, N.S. Alcohol-Induced Neuroadaptation Is Orchestrated by the Histone Acetyltransferase CBP. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 2017, 10, 103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Krishnan, H.R.; Li, X.; Ghezzi, A.; Atkinson, N.S. A DNA element in the slo gene modulates ethanol tolerance. Alcohol 2016, 51, 37–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Devineni, A.V.; Heberlein, U. Preferential ethanol consumption in Drosophila models features of addiction. Curr. Biol. 2009, 19, 2126–2132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Kaun, K.R.; Azanchi, R.; Maung, Z.; Hirsh, J.; Heberlein, U. A Drosophila model for alcohol reward. Nat. Neurosci. 2011, 14, 612–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Kaun, K.R.; Devineni, A.V.; Heberlein, U. Drosophila melanogaster as a model to study drug addiction. Hum. Genet. 2012, 131, 959–975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Xu, S.; Chan, T.; Shah, V.; Zhang, S.; Pletcher, S.D.; Roman, G. The propensity for consuming ethanol in Drosophila requires rutabaga adenylyl cyclase expression within mushroom body neurons. Genes Brain Behav. 2012, 11, 727–739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Peru Y Colón de Portugal, R.L.; Ojelade, S.A.; Penninti, P.S.; Dove, R.J.; Nye, M.J.; Acevedo, S.F.; Lopez, A.; Rodan, A.R.; Rothenfluh, A. Long-lasting, experience-dependent alcohol preference in Drosophila. Addict. Biol. 2014, 19, 392–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Dorrian, J.; Heath, G.; Sargent, C.; Banks, S.; Coates, A. Alcohol use in shiftworkers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 99 Pt B, 395–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Dorrian, J.; Skinner, N. Alcohol consumption patterns of shiftworkers compared with dayworkers. Chronobiol. Int. 2012, 29, 610–618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Kenney, S.R.; LaBrie, J.W.; Hummer, J.F.; Pham, A.T. Global sleep quality as a moderator of alcohol consumption and consequences in college students. Addict. Behav. 2012, 37, 507–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Morikawa, Y.; Sakurai, M.; Nakamura, K.; Nagasawa, S.-Y.; Ishizaki, M.; Kido, T.; Naruse, Y.; Nakagawa, H. Correlation between shift-work-related sleep problems and heavy drinking in Japanese male factory workers. Alcohol Alcohol. 2013, 48, 202–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Wong, M.M.; Brower, K.J.; Nigg, J.T.; Zucker, R.A. Childhood sleep problems, response inhibition, and alcohol and drug outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2010, 34, 1033–1044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Wong, M.M.; Robertson, G.C.; Dyson, R.B. Prospective relationship between poor sleep and substance-related problems in a national sample of adolescents. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2015, 39, 355–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Donlea, J.M.; Shaw, P.J. Sleeping together using social interactions to understand the role of sleep in plasticity. Adv. Genet. 2009, 68, 57–81. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  68. Eddison, M.; Guarnieri, D.J.; Cheng, L.; Liu, C.-H.; Moffat, K.G.; Davis, G.; Heberlein, U. Arouser reveals a role for synapse number in the regulation of ethanol sensitivity. Neuron 2011, 70, 979–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. De Nobrega, A.K.; Mellers, A.P.; Lyons, L.C. Aging and circadian dysfunction increase alcohol sensitivity and exacerbate mortality in Drosophila melanogaster. Exp. Gerontol. 2017, 97, 49–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Kanny, D.; Brewer, R.D.; Mesnick, J.B.; Paulozzi, L.J.; Naimi, T.S.; Lu, H. Vital signs: Alcohol poisoning deaths-United States, 2010–2012. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2015, 63, 1238–1242. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  71. White, A.M.; Slater, M.E.; Ng, G.; Hingson, R.; Breslow, R. Trends in Alcohol-Related Emergency Department Visits in the United States: Results from the Nationwide Emergency Department Sample, 2006 to 2014. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2018, 42, 352–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Pasch, K.E.; Latimer, L.A.; Cance, J.D.; Moe, S.G.; Lytle, L.A. Longitudinal bi-directional relationships between sleep and youth substance use. J. Youth Adolesc. 2012, 41, 1184–1196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  73. Consensus Conference Panel; Watson, N.F.; Badr, M.S.; Belenky, G.; Bliwise, D.L.; Buxton, O.M.; Buysse, D.; Dinges, D.F.; Gangwisch, J.; Grandner, M.A.; et al. Recommended Amount of Sleep for a Healthy Adult: A Joint Consensus Statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine and Sleep Research Society. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2015, 11, 591–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Wheaton, A.G.; Chapman, D.P.; Croft, J.B. School Start Times, Sleep, Behavioral, Health, and Academic Outcomes: A Review of the Literature. J. Sch. Health 2016, 86, 363–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  75. Stavropoulos, N.; Young, M.W. Insomniac and Cullin-3 regulate sleep and wakefulness in Drosophila. Neuron 2011, 72, 964–976. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Cong, X.; Wang, H.; Liu, Z.; He, C.; An, C.; Zhao, Z. Regulation of Sleep by Insulin-like Peptide System in Drosophila melanogaster. Sleep 2015, 38, 1075–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  77. Grönke, S.; Clarke, D.F.; Broughton, S.; Andrews, T.D.; Partridge, L. Molecular evolution and functional characterization of Drosophila insulin-like peptides. PLoS Genet. 2010, 6, e1000857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  78. Dissel, S.; Angadi, V.; Kirszenblat, L.; Suzuki, Y.; Donlea, J.; Klose, M.; Koch, Z.; English, D.; Winsky-Sommerer, R.; van Swinderen, B.; et al. Sleep restores behavioral plasticity to Drosophila mutants. Curr. Biol. 2015, 25, 1270–1281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  79. Berry, J.A.; Cervantes-Sandoval, I.; Chakraborty, M.; Davis, R.L. Sleep Facilitates Memory by Blocking Dopamine Neuron-Mediated Forgetting. Cell 2015, 161, 1656–1667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  80. Tainton-Heap, L.A.; Kirszenblat, L.C.; Notaras, E.T.; Grabowska, M.J.; Jeans, R.; Feng, K.; Shaw, P.J.; van Swinderen, B. A Paradoxical Kind of Sleep in Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 2021, 31, 578–590.e576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Vashchinkina, E.; Panhelainen, A.; Vekovischeva, O.Y.; Aitta-Aho, T.; Ebert, B.; Ator, N.A.; Korpi, E.R. GABA site agonist gaboxadol induces addiction-predicting persistent changes in ventral tegmental area dopamine neurons but is not rewarding in mice or baboons. J. Neurosci. 2012, 32, 5310–5320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  82. Lyons, L.C.; Roman, G. Circadian modulation of short-term memory in Drosophila. Learn. Mem. 2009, 16, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  83. Ewer, J.; Frisch, B.; Hamblen-Coyle, M.J.; Rosbash, M.; Hall, J.C. Expression of the period clock gene within different cell types in the brain of Drosophila adults and mosaic analysis of these cells’ influence on circadian behavioral rhythms. J. Neurosci. 1992, 12, 3321–3349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Konopka, R.J.; Pittendrigh, C.; Orr, D. Reciprocal behaviour associated with altered homeostasis and photosensitivity of Drosophila clock mutants. J. Neurogenet. 1989, 6, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Price, J.L.; Dembinska, M.E.; Young, M.W.; Rosbash, M. Suppression of PERIOD protein abundance and circadian cycling by the Drosophila clock mutation timeless. EMBO J. 1995, 14, 4044–4049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Yoshii, T.; Heshiki, Y.; Ibuki-Ishibashi, T.; Matsumoto, A.; Tanimura, T.; Tomioka, K. Temperature cycles drive Drosophila circadian oscillation in constant light that otherwise induces behavioural arrhythmicity. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005, 22, 1176–1184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Power, J.; Ringo, J.; Dowse, H. The role of light in the initiation of circadian activity rhythms of adult Drosophila melanogaster. J. Neurogenet. 1995, 9, 227–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Arellanes-Licea, E.; Caldelas, I.; De Ita-Pérez, D.; Díaz-Muñoz, M. The circadian timing system: A recent addition in the physiological mechanisms underlying pathological and aging processes. Aging Dis. 2014, 5, 406–418. [Google Scholar]
  89. Bah, T.M.; Goodman, J.; Iliff, J.J. Sleep as a Therapeutic Target in the Aging Brain. Neurotherapeutics 2019, 16, 554–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Mander, B.A.; Winer, J.R.; Walker, M.P. Sleep and Human Aging. Neuron 2017, 94, 19–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Tevy, M.F.; Giebultowicz, J.; Pincus, Z.; Mazzoccoli, G.; Vinciguerra, M. Aging signaling pathways and circadian clock-dependent metabolic derangements. Trends Endocrinol. Metab. 2013, 24, 229–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Zhong, H.; Yu, B.; Luo, D.; Yang, L.-Y.; Zhang, J.; Jiang, S.-S.; Hu, S.-J.; Luo, Y.-Y.; Yang, M.-W.; Hong, F.-F.; et al. Roles of aging in sleep. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2019, 98, 177–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Han, B.H.; Moore, A.A.; Ferris, R.; Palamar, J.J. Binge Drinking Among Older Adults in the United States, 2015 to 2017. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2019, 67, 2139–2144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Han, B.H.; Moore, A.A.; Sherman, S.E.; Palamar, J.J. Prevalence and correlates of binge drinking among older adults with multimorbidity. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2018, 187, 48–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Wan, H.; Goodkind, D.; Kowal, P. An Aging World: 2015. US Census Bureau, International Population Reports; US Census Bureau: Suitland, MD, USA, 2015; p. 95.
  96. Abrahao, K.P.; Salinas, A.G.; Lovinger, D.M. Alcohol and the Brain: Neuronal Molecular Targets, Synapses, and Circuits. Neuron 2017, 96, 1223–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  97. Roberto, M.; Varodayan, F.P. Synaptic targets: Chronic alcohol actions. Neuropharmacology 2017, 122, 85–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Berger, K.H.; Heberlein, U.; Moore, M.S. Rapid and chronic: Two distinct forms of ethanol tolerance in Drosophila. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2004, 28, 1469–1480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Cowmeadow, R.B.; Krishnan, H.R.; Ghezzi, A.; Al’Hasan, Y.M.; Wang, Y.Z.; Atkinson, N.S. Ethanol tolerance caused by slowpoke induction in Drosophila. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2006, 30, 745–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Engel, G.L.; Taber, K.; Vinton, E.; Crocker, A.J. Studying alcohol use disorder using Drosophila melanogaster in the era of ‘Big Data’. Behav. Brain Funct. 2019, 15, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Heyne, A.; May, T.; Goll, P.; Wolffgramm, J. Persisting consequences of drug intake: Towards a memory of addiction. J. Neural Transm. 2000, 107, 613–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Van Skike, C.E.; Goodlett, C.; Matthews, D.B. Acute alcohol and cognition: Remembering what it causes us to forget. Alcohol 2019, 79, 105–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Havekes, R.; Park, A.J.; Tudor, J.C.; Luczak, V.G.; Hansen, R.T.; Ferri, S.L.; Bruinenberg, V.M.; Poplawski, S.G.; Day, J.P.; Aton, S.J.; et al. Sleep deprivation causes memory deficits by negatively impacting neuronal connectivity in hippocampal area CA1. Elife 2016, 5, e13424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Krishnan, H.C.; Gandour, C.E.; Ramos, J.L.; Wrinkle, M.C.; Sanchez-Pacheco, J.J.; Lyons, L.C. Acute Sleep Deprivation Blocks Short- and Long-Term Operant Memory in. Sleep 2016, 39, 2161–2171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  105. Saygin, M.; Ozguner, M.F.; Onder, O.; Doguc, D.K.; Ilhan, I.; Peker, Y. The impact of sleep deprivation on hippocampal-mediated learning and memory in rats. Bratisl. Lek. Listy 2017, 118, 408–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Van der Linde, K.; Fumagalli, E.; Roman, G.; Lyons, L.C. The FlyBar: Administering alcohol to flies. J. Vis. Exp. 2014, 87, e50442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  107. Bishehsari, F.; Saadalla, A.; Khazaie, K.; Engen, P.A.; Voigt, R.M.; Shetuni, B.B.; Forsyth, C.; Shaikh, M.; Vitaterna, M.H.; Turek, F.; et al. Light/Dark Shifting Promotes Alcohol-Induced Colon Carcinogenesis: Possible Role of Intestinal Inflammatory Milieu and Microbiota. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2016, 17, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Voigt, R.M.; Forsyth, C.B.; Shaikh, M.; Zhang, L.; Raeisi, S.; Aloman, C.; Preite, N.Z.; Donohue, T.M.; Fogg, L.; Keshavarzian, A. Diurnal variations in intestinal barrier integrity and liver pathology in mice: Implications for alcohol binge. Am. J. Physiol. Gastrointest. Liver Physiol. 2018, 314, G131–G141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Reid, K.J.; Abbott, S.M. Jet Lag and Shift Work Disorder. Sleep Med. Clin. 2015, 10, 523–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Haynie, D.L.; Lewin, D.; Luk, J.; Lipsky, L.; O’Brien, F.; Iannotti, R.J.; Liu, D.; Simons-Morton, B. Beyond Sleep Duration: Bidirectional Associations Among Chronotype, Social Jetlag, and Drinking Behaviors in a Longitudinal Sample of US High School Students. Sleep 2018, 41, zsx202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. Hasler, B.P.; Wallace, M.L.; White, S.J.; Molina, B.S.G.; Pedersen, S.L. Preliminary Evidence That Real World Sleep Timing and Duration are Associated With Laboratory-Assessed Alcohol Response. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2019, 43, 1575–1584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. O’Brien, E.M.; Mindell, J.A. Sleep and risk-taking behavior in adolescents. Behav. Sleep Med. 2005, 3, 113–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  113. Schweizer, C.A.; Hoggatt, K.J.; Washington, D.L.; Bean-Mayberry, B.; Yano, E.M.; Mitchell, M.N.; Alessi, C.A.; Martin, J.L. Use of alcohol as a sleep aid, unhealthy drinking behaviors, and sleeping pill use among women veterans. Sleep Health 2019, 5, 495–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Dorrian, J.; Baulk, S.D.; Dawson, D. Work hours, workload, sleep and fatigue in Australian Rail Industry employees. Appl. Ergon. 2011, 42, 202–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Buchvold, H.V.; Pallesen, S.; Øyane, N.M.; Bjorvatn, B. Associations between night work and BMI, alcohol, smoking, caffeine and exercise—A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 1112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  116. Giannotti, F.; Cortesi, F.; Sebastiani, T.; Ottaviano, S. Circadian preference, sleep and daytime behaviour in adolescence. J. Sleep Res. 2002, 11, 191–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  117. Pieters, S.; Van Der Vorst, H.; Burk, W.J.; Wiers, R.W.; Engels, R.C. Puberty-dependent sleep regulation and alcohol use in early adolescents. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2010, 34, 1512–1518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  118. Pieters, S.; van der Vorst, H.; Engels, R.C.; Wiers, R.W. Implicit and explicit cognitions related to alcohol use in children. Addict. Behav. 2010, 35, 471–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Robinson, D.; Gelaye, B.; Tadesse, M.G.; Williams, M.A.; Lemma, S.; Berhane, Y. Daytime Sleepiness, Circadian Preference, Caffeine Consumption and Khat Use among College Students in Ethiopia. J. Sleep Disord. Treat. Care. 2013, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Saxvig, I.W.; Pallesen, S.; Wilhelmsen-Langeland, A.; Molde, H.; Bjorvatn, B. Prevalence and correlates of delayed sleep phase in high school students. Sleep Med. 2012, 13, 193–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  121. Tavernier, R.; Willoughby, T. Bidirectional associations between sleep (quality and duration) and psychosocial functioning across the university years. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 50, 674–682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Urbán, R.; Magyaródi, T.; Rigó, A. Morningness-eveningness, chronotypes and health-impairing behaviors in adolescents. Chronobiol. Int. 2011, 28, 238–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Díaz-Morales, J.F.; Escribano, C.; Jankowski, K.S. Chronotype and time-of-day effects on mood during school day. Chronobiol. Int. 2015, 32, 37–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  124. Gruber, R.; Michaelsen, S.; Bergmame, L.; Frenette, S.; Bruni, O.; Fontil, L.; Carrier, J. Short sleep duration is associated with teacher-reported inattention and cognitive problems in healthy school-aged children. Nat. Sci. Sleep 2012, 4, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  125. Hasler, B.P.; Sitnick, S.L.; Shaw, D.S.; Forbes, E.E. An altered neural response to reward may contribute to alcohol problems among late adolescents with an evening chronotype. Psychiatry Res. 2013, 214, 357–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  126. Sadeh, A.; Gruber, R.; Raviv, A. The effects of sleep restriction and extension on school-age children: What a difference an hour makes. Child Dev. 2003, 74, 444–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Short, M.A.; Gradisar, M.; Lack, L.C.; Wright, H.R. The impact of sleep on adolescent depressed mood, alertness and academic performance. J. Adolesc. 2013, 36, 1025–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Digdon, N.; Landry, K. University students’ motives for drinking alcohol are related to evening preference, poor sleep, and ways of coping with stress. Biol. Rhythm. Res. 2013, 44, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Rique, G.L.; Fernandes Filho, G.M.; Ferreira, A.D.; de Sousa-Muñoz, R.L. Relationship between chronotype and quality of sleep in medical students at the Federal University of Paraiba, Brazil. Sleep Sci. 2014, 7, 96–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  130. Martin, J.S.; Hébert, M.; Ledoux, E.; Gaudreault, M.; Laberge, L. Relationship of chronotype to sleep, light exposure, and work-related fatigue in student workers. Chronobiol. Int. 2012, 29, 295–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Bixler, E.O.; Vgontzas, A.N.; Lin, H.M.; Liao, D.; Calhoun, S.; Vela-Bueno, A.; Fedok, F.; Vlasic, V.; Graff, G. Sleep disordered breathing in children in a general population sample: Prevalence and risk factors. Sleep 2009, 32, 731–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  132. Owens, J.; Adolescent Sleep Working Group; Committee on Adolescence; Au, R.; Carskadon, M.; Millman, R.; Wolfson, A.; Braverman, P.K.; Adelman, W.P.; Breuner, C.C.; et al. Insufficient sleep in adolescents and young adults: An update on causes and consequences. Pediatrics 2014, 134, e921–e932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  133. Paruthi, S.; Brooks, L.J.; D’Ambrosio, C.; Hall, W.; Kotagal, S.; Lloyd, R.M.; Malow, B.A.; Maski, K.; Nichols, C.; Quan, S.F.; et al. Recommended Amount of Sleep for Pediatric Populations: A Consensus Statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2016, 12, 785–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  134. Wheaton, A.G.; Ferro, G.A.; Croft, J.B. School Start Times for Middle School and High School Students-United States, 2011–2012 School Year. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2015, 64, 809–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  135. Knutson, K.L.; Van Cauter, E. Associations between sleep loss and increased risk of obesity and diabetes. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008, 1129, 287–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Stone, C.R.; Haig, T.R.; Fiest, K.M.; McNeil, J.; Brenner, D.R.; Friedenreich, C.M. The association between sleep duration and cancer-specific mortality: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Cancer Causes Control 2019, 30, 501–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  137. Van Egroo, M.; Narbutas, J.; Chylinski, D.; Villar González, P.; Maquet, P.; Salmon, E.; Vandewalle, G.; Bastin, C.; Collette, F. Sleep-wake regulation and the hallmarks of the pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease. Sleep 2019, 42, zsz017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  138. Videnovic, A.; Lazar, A.S.; Barker, R.A.; Overeem, S. ‘The clocks that time us’—Circadian rhythms in neurodegenerative disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2014, 10, 683–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  139. Videnovic, A.; Noble, C.; Reid, K.J.; Peng, J.; Turek, F.W.; Marconi, A.; Rademaker, A.W.; Simuni, T.; Zadikoff, C.; Zee, P.C. Circadian melatonin rhythm and excessive daytime sleepiness in Parkinson disease. JAMA Neurol. 2014, 71, 463–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  140. Mumtaz, F.; Khan, M.I.; Zubair, M.; Dehpour, A.R. Neurobiology and consequences of social isolation stress in animal model-A comprehensive review. Biomed. Pharmacother. 2018, 105, 1205–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Evans, O.; Rodríguez-Borillo, O.; Font, L.; Currie, P.J.; Pastor, R. Alcohol Binge Drinking and Anxiety-Like Behavior in Socialized Versus Isolated C57BL/6J Mice. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2020, 44, 244–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Lopez, M.F.; Doremus-Fitzwater, T.L.; Becker, H.C. Chronic social isolation and chronic variable stress during early development induce later elevated ethanol intake in adult C57BL/6J mice. Alcohol 2011, 45, 355–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  143. Lopez, M.F.; Laber, K. Impact of social isolation and enriched environment during adolescence on voluntary ethanol intake and anxiety in C57BL/6J mice. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 148, 151–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  144. Sanna, E.; Talani, G.; Obili, N.; Mascia, M.P.; Mostallino, M.C.; Secci, P.P.; Follesa, P.; Pisu, M.G.; Biggio, F.; Utzeri, C.; et al. Voluntary Ethanol Consumption Induced by Social Isolation Reverses the Increase of α(4)/δ GABA(A) Receptor Gene Expression and Function in the Hippocampus of C57BL/6J Mice. Front. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  145. Clark, C.P.; Gillin, J.; Golshan, S.; Demodena, A.; Smith, T.L.; Danowski, S.; Irwin, M.; Schuckit, M. Increased REM sleep density at admission predicts relapse by three months in primary alcoholics with a lifetime diagnosis of secondary depression. Biol. Psychiatry 1998, 43, 601–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. García-García, F.; Priego-Fernández, S.; López-Muciño, L.A.; Acosta-Hernández, M.E.; Peña-Escudero, C. Increased alcohol consumption in sleep-restricted rats is mediated by delta FosB induction. Alcohol 2021, 93, 63–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  147. He, S.; Hasler, B.P.; Chakravorty, S. Alcohol and sleep-related problems. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2019, 30, 117–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  148. Roehrs, T.; Papineau, K.; Rosenthal, L.; Roth, T. Ethanol as a hypnotic in insomniacs: Self administration and effects on sleep and mood. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999, 20, 279–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  149. Araujo, N.P.; Andersen, M.L.; Abilio, V.; Gomes, D.C.; Carvalho, R.C.; Silva, R.; Ribeiro, R.D.A.; Tufik, S.; Frussa-Filho, R. Sleep deprivation abolishes the locomotor stimulant effect of ethanol in mice. Brain Res. Bull. 2006, 69, 332–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  150. McKnight-Eily, L.R.; Eaton, D.K.; Lowry, R.; Croft, J.B.; Presley-Cantrell, L.; Perry, G.S. Relationships between hours of sleep and health-risk behaviors in US adolescent students. Prev. Med. 2011, 53, 271–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Sivertsen, B.; Skogen, J.C.; Jakobsen, R.; Hysing, M. Sleep and use of alcohol and drug in adolescence. A large population-based study of Norwegian adolescents aged 16 to 19 years. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015, 149, 180–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. Tynjälä, J.; Kannas, L.; Levälahti, E. Perceived tiredness among adolescents and its association with sleep habits and use of psychoactive substances. J. Sleep Res. 1997, 6, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Du, C.; Zan, M.C.H.; Cho, M.J.; Fenton, J.I.; Hsiao, P.Y.; Hsiao, R.; Keaver, L.; Lai, C.-C.; Lee, H.; Ludy, M.-J.; et al. The Effects of Sleep Quality and Resilience on Perceived Stress, Dietary Behaviors, and Alcohol Misuse: A Mediation-Moderation Analysis of Higher Education Students from Asia, Europe, and North America during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Nutrients 2021, 13, 442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Kenney, S.R.; Lac, A.; Labrie, J.W.; Hummer, J.F.; Pham, A. Mental health, sleep quality, drinking motives, and alcohol-related consequences: A path-analytic model. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 2013, 74, 841–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  155. Hasler, B.P.; Bootzin, R.R.; Cousins, J.C.; Fridel, K.; Wenk, G.L. Circadian phase in sleep-disturbed adolescents with a history of substance abuse: A pilot study. Behav. Sleep Med. 2008, 6, 55–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  156. Hasler, B.P.; Kirisci, L.; Clark, D.B. Restless Sleep and Variable Sleep Timing During Late Childhood Accelerate the Onset of Alcohol and Other Drug Involvement. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 2016, 77, 649–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  157. Hasler, B.P.; Martin, C.S.; Wood, D.S.; Rosario, B.; Clark, D.B. A longitudinal study of insomnia and other sleep complaints in adolescents with and without alcohol use disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2014, 38, 2225–2233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  158. Mike, T.B.; Shaw, D.S.; Forbes, E.E.; Sitnick, S.L.; Hasler, B.P. The hazards of bad sleep-Sleep duration and quality as predictors of adolescent alcohol and cannabis use. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2016, 168, 335–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  159. Wong, M.M.; Brower, K.J.; Fitzgerald, H.E.; Zucker, R.A. Sleep problems in early childhood and early onset of alcohol and other drug use in adolescence. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2004, 28, 578–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  160. Johnson, E.O.; Breslau, N. Sleep problems and substance use in adolescence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2001, 64, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  161. Hill, V.M.; O’Connor, R.M.; Sissoko, G.B.; Irobunda, I.S.; Leong, S.; Canman, J.C.; Stavropoulos, N.; Shirasu-Hiza, M. A bidirectional relationship between sleep and oxidative stress in Drosophila. PLoS Biol. 2018, 16, e2005206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  162. Broughton, S.J.; Piper, M.D.W.; Ikeya, T.; Bass, T.M.; Jacobson, J.; Driege, Y.; Martinez, P.; Hafen, E.; Withers, D.J.; Leevers, S.J.; et al. Longer lifespan, altered metabolism, and stress resistance in Drosophila from ablation of cells making insulin-like ligands. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2005, 102, 3105–3110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  163. Corl, A.B.; Rodan, A.R.; Heberlein, U. Insulin signaling in the nervous system regulates ethanol intoxication in Drosophila melanogaster. Nat. Neurosci. 2005, 8, 18–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  164. McClure, K.D.; French, R.L.; Heberlein, U. A Drosophila model for fetal alcohol syndrome disorders: Role for the insulin pathway. Dis. Model Mech. 2011, 4, 335–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  165. Vaccaro, A.; Dor, Y.K.; Nambara, K.; Pollina, E.A.; Lin, C.; Greenberg, M.E.; Rogulja, D. Sleep Loss Can Cause Death through Accumulation of Reactive Oxygen Species in the Gut. Cell 2020, 181, 1307–1328.e1315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  166. Potdar, S.; Daniel, D.K.; Thomas, F.A.; Lall, S.; Sheeba, V. Sleep deprivation negatively impacts reproductive output in. J. Exp. Biol. 2018, 221 Pt 6, jeb.174771. [Google Scholar]
  167. Dong, L.; Martinez, A.J.; Buysse, D.J.; Harvey, A.G. A composite measure of sleep health predicts concurrent mental and physical health outcomes in adolescents prone to eveningness. Sleep Health 2019, 5, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  168. Killick, R.; Hoyos, C.M.; Melehan, K.L.; Dungan, G.C.; Poh, J.; Liu, P.Y. Metabolic and hormonal effects of ‘catch-up’ sleep in men with chronic, repetitive, lifestyle-driven sleep restriction. Clin. Endocrinol. 2015, 83, 498–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  169. Leproult, R.; Deliens, G.; Gilson, M.; Peigneux, P. Beneficial impact of sleep extension on fasting insulin sensitivity in adults with habitual sleep restriction. Sleep 2015, 38, 707–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  170. Sonni, A.; Spencer, R.M.C. Sleep protects memories from interference in older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 2015, 36, 2272–2281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  171. Fillmore, M.T.; Weafer, J. Acute tolerance to alcohol in at-risk binge drinkers. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 2012, 26, 693–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  172. Kong, E.C.; Allouche, L.; Chapot, P.A.; Vranizan, K.; Moore, M.S.; Heberlein, U.; Wolf, F.W. Ethanol-regulated genes that contribute to ethanol sensitivity and rapid tolerance in Drosophila. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2010, 34, 302–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  173. Lasek, A.W.; Giorgetti, F.; Berger, K.H.; Tayor, S.; Heberlein, U. Lmo genes regulate behavioral responses to ethanol in Drosophila melanogaster and the mouse. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2011, 35, 1600–1606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  174. Lasek, A.W.; Lim, J.; Kliethermes, C.L.; Berger, K.H.; Joslyn, G.; Brush, G.; Xue, L.; Robertson, M.; Moore, M.S.; Vranizan, K.; et al. An evolutionary conserved role for anaplastic lymphoma kinase in behavioral responses to ethanol. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e22636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  175. Pietrzykowski, A.Z.; Friesen, R.M.; Martin, G.E.; Puig, S.I.; Nowak, C.L.; Wynne, P.M.; Siegelmann, H.T.; Treistman, S.N. Posttranscriptional regulation of BK channel splice variant stability by miR-9 underlies neuroadaptation to alcohol. Neuron 2008, 59, 274–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  176. Savarese, A.; Zou, M.E.; Kharazia, V.; Maiya, R.; Lasek, A.W. Increased behavioral responses to ethanol in Lmo3 knockout mice. Genes Brain Behav. 2014, 13, 777–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  177. LaFerriere, H.; Guarnieri, D.J.; Sitaraman, D.; Diegelmann, S.; Heberlein, U.; Zars, T. Genetic dissociation of ethanol sensitivity and memory formation in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 2008, 178, 1895–1902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  178. Chen, C.N.; Denome, S.; Davis, R.L. Molecular analysis of cDNA clones and the corresponding genomic coding sequences of the Drosophila dunce+ gene, the structural gene for cAMP phosphodiesterase. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1986, 83, 9313–9317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  179. Qiu, Y.H.; Chen, C.N.; Malone, T.; Richter, L.; Beckendorf, S.K.; Davis, R.L. Characterization of the memory gene dunce of Drosophila melanogaster. J. Mol. Biol. 1991, 222, 553–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  180. Dudai, Y.; Jan, Y.N.; Byers, D.; Quinn, W.G.; Benzer, S. Dunce, a mutant of Drosophila deficient in learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1976, 73, 1684–1688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  181. Nighorn, A.; Healy, M.J.; Davis, R.L. The cyclic AMP phosphodiesterase encoded by the Drosophila dunce gene is concentrated in the mushroom body neuropil. Neuron 1991, 6, 455–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  182. Kirszenblat, L.; Ertekin, D.; Goodsell, J.; Zhou, Y.; Shaw, P.J.; van Swinderen, B. Sleep regulates visual selective attention in. J. Exp. Biol. 2018, 221 Pt 24, jeb191429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  183. Ruppert, M.; Franz, M.; Saratsis, A.; Escarcena, L.V.; Hendrich, O.; Gooi, L.M.; Scholz, H.; Schwenkert, I.; Klebes, A. Hangover Links Nuclear RNA Signaling to cAMP Regulation via the Phosphodiesterase 4d Ortholog dunce. Cell Rep. 2017, 18, 533–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  184. Scholz, H.; Franz, M.; Heberlein, U. The hangover gene defines a stress pathway required for ethanol tolerance development. Nature 2005, 436, 845–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  185. Graves, L.A.; Heller, E.A.; Pack, A.I.; Abel, T. Sleep deprivation selectively impairs memory consolidation for contextual fear conditioning. Learn. Mem. 2003, 10, 168–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  186. Prince, T.M.; Wimmer, M.; Choi, J.; Havekes, R.; Aton, S.; Abel, T. Sleep deprivation during a specific 3-hour time window post-training impairs hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2014, 109, 122–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Acute sleep deprivation increases sensitivity to alcohol-induced sedation (AC). Comparison of sleep profiles between 10 d wild-type sleep-deprived (SLD) and non-sleep-deprived (NSD) CS flies housed in LD cycles. Compared to NSD flies, both male and female SLD flies exhibited significantly increased quiescence immediately following the end of the sleep deprivation protocol ((A), ZT 9-12 [ANOVA: F3,108 = 52.55, p < 0.0001]; * indicates significant differences between NSD and SLD male and female flies as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001), as well as during the 12 h dark cycle ((B), D1 ZT 12-24 ANOVA: F3,108 = 21.16, p < 0.0001) and the second day ((C), D2 ZT 1-24 [ANOVA: F3,108 = 76.85, p < 0.0001) after sleep deprivation, reflecting rebound sleep in SLD flies. (D) Wild-type CS 10 d old flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h and then exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h. Sensitivity to sedation was measured by counting the number of flies sedated every 5 min. (E) Sleep deprivation significantly exacerbated alcohol-induced sedation (t(14) = 13.4558, p = 0.0002). Mean time necessary for 50% of the flies to become sedated during alcohol exposure and standard error of the mean are plotted. (F) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation for 10 d old sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies. (G,H) Effect of recovery sleep on alcohol-induced sedation. Wild-type CS flies were allowed to recover from sleep deprivation for either 48 h or 72 h prior to alcohol exposure, with behavioral sensitivity assessed. An amount of 48 h recovery sleep (RS) was insufficient to completely restore normal responses to alcohol-induced sedation in 10 d old SLD wild-type flies. (G) [ANOVA: F3,8 = 24.6, p < 0.05]; * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis. An amount of 72 h recovery sleep restored normal behavioral sensitivity to alcohol sedation. (H) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation for 10 d old NSD flies and SLD flies with recovery sleep. (I) Separate groups of 10 d male and female flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h and then exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h (ANOVA F3,28 = 29.24, p < 0.0001). N shown on bars for each group is the number of vials tested for each group, with 25–30 flies per vial. (J) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation for 10 d sleep-deprived and non-sleep deprived male and female flies.
Figure 1. Acute sleep deprivation increases sensitivity to alcohol-induced sedation (AC). Comparison of sleep profiles between 10 d wild-type sleep-deprived (SLD) and non-sleep-deprived (NSD) CS flies housed in LD cycles. Compared to NSD flies, both male and female SLD flies exhibited significantly increased quiescence immediately following the end of the sleep deprivation protocol ((A), ZT 9-12 [ANOVA: F3,108 = 52.55, p < 0.0001]; * indicates significant differences between NSD and SLD male and female flies as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001), as well as during the 12 h dark cycle ((B), D1 ZT 12-24 ANOVA: F3,108 = 21.16, p < 0.0001) and the second day ((C), D2 ZT 1-24 [ANOVA: F3,108 = 76.85, p < 0.0001) after sleep deprivation, reflecting rebound sleep in SLD flies. (D) Wild-type CS 10 d old flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h and then exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h. Sensitivity to sedation was measured by counting the number of flies sedated every 5 min. (E) Sleep deprivation significantly exacerbated alcohol-induced sedation (t(14) = 13.4558, p = 0.0002). Mean time necessary for 50% of the flies to become sedated during alcohol exposure and standard error of the mean are plotted. (F) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation for 10 d old sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies. (G,H) Effect of recovery sleep on alcohol-induced sedation. Wild-type CS flies were allowed to recover from sleep deprivation for either 48 h or 72 h prior to alcohol exposure, with behavioral sensitivity assessed. An amount of 48 h recovery sleep (RS) was insufficient to completely restore normal responses to alcohol-induced sedation in 10 d old SLD wild-type flies. (G) [ANOVA: F3,8 = 24.6, p < 0.05]; * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis. An amount of 72 h recovery sleep restored normal behavioral sensitivity to alcohol sedation. (H) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation for 10 d old NSD flies and SLD flies with recovery sleep. (I) Separate groups of 10 d male and female flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h and then exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h (ANOVA F3,28 = 29.24, p < 0.0001). N shown on bars for each group is the number of vials tested for each group, with 25–30 flies per vial. (J) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation for 10 d sleep-deprived and non-sleep deprived male and female flies.
Ijms 23 12091 g001
Figure 2. Acute sleep deprivation exacerbates alcohol-induced mortality following single or repeated exposure to alcohol. (A) Wild-type CS 10 d old flies were sleep deprived for 24 h and then exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h with mortality assessed every 24 h. (B) Sleep-deprived flies exhibited a significant increase in mortality within 24 h of exposure to alcohol compared to non-sleep-deprived flies (ANOVA F3,28 = 22.50, p < 0.0001). (C) Mortality differences remained apparent at 7 d after exposure to alcohol between non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived flies (ANOVA: F3,28 = 14.01, p < 0.0001). (D,E) Separate groups of 10 d male and female flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h and then exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h. Sleep-deprived males and females exhibited significantly increased mortality 24 h following alcohol exposure (ANOVA F5,32 = 29.77, p < 0.0001), indicating no sex differences in the effects of acute sleep deprivation on alcohol-induced toxicity. (F) Wild-type CS 10 d old flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h, followed by 3 consecutive exposures to 1 h alcohol (50% alcohol vapor) at ZT 9 with each exposure separated by 24 h. (G) Sleep-deprived flies exhibited a drastic increase in mortality within 24 h of first exposure to alcohol compared to non-sleep-deprived flies (ANOVA: F3,76 = 15.42, p < 0.0001). (H) Mortality continued to increase 24 h following the third exposure to alcohol vapor in both non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived flies (ANOVA: F3,76 = 14.42, p < 0.0001). (I) Mortality measured at 7 d following the third alcohol exposure was significantly higher than mortality following the first alcohol exposure (ANOVA: F3,76 = 19.91, p < 0.0001). * indicates significant differences between NSD and SLD groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Figure 2. Acute sleep deprivation exacerbates alcohol-induced mortality following single or repeated exposure to alcohol. (A) Wild-type CS 10 d old flies were sleep deprived for 24 h and then exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h with mortality assessed every 24 h. (B) Sleep-deprived flies exhibited a significant increase in mortality within 24 h of exposure to alcohol compared to non-sleep-deprived flies (ANOVA F3,28 = 22.50, p < 0.0001). (C) Mortality differences remained apparent at 7 d after exposure to alcohol between non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived flies (ANOVA: F3,28 = 14.01, p < 0.0001). (D,E) Separate groups of 10 d male and female flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h and then exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h. Sleep-deprived males and females exhibited significantly increased mortality 24 h following alcohol exposure (ANOVA F5,32 = 29.77, p < 0.0001), indicating no sex differences in the effects of acute sleep deprivation on alcohol-induced toxicity. (F) Wild-type CS 10 d old flies were sleep-deprived for 24 h, followed by 3 consecutive exposures to 1 h alcohol (50% alcohol vapor) at ZT 9 with each exposure separated by 24 h. (G) Sleep-deprived flies exhibited a drastic increase in mortality within 24 h of first exposure to alcohol compared to non-sleep-deprived flies (ANOVA: F3,76 = 15.42, p < 0.0001). (H) Mortality continued to increase 24 h following the third exposure to alcohol vapor in both non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived flies (ANOVA: F3,76 = 14.42, p < 0.0001). (I) Mortality measured at 7 d following the third alcohol exposure was significantly higher than mortality following the first alcohol exposure (ANOVA: F3,76 = 19.91, p < 0.0001). * indicates significant differences between NSD and SLD groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Ijms 23 12091 g002
Figure 3. Sleep deprivation does not affect alcohol accumulation or rate of alcohol clearance. (A) Flies were aged in 12:12 LD cycle for 10 days and then sleep-deprived for 24 h. CS 10 d old flies were exposed to 30 min of 50% alcohol vapor on day 11 with alcohol absorbance and rate of alcohol clearance assessed. (B) No significant differences existed in alcohol absorbance or rate of alcohol clearance between sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies (n = 4 per group).
Figure 3. Sleep deprivation does not affect alcohol accumulation or rate of alcohol clearance. (A) Flies were aged in 12:12 LD cycle for 10 days and then sleep-deprived for 24 h. CS 10 d old flies were exposed to 30 min of 50% alcohol vapor on day 11 with alcohol absorbance and rate of alcohol clearance assessed. (B) No significant differences existed in alcohol absorbance or rate of alcohol clearance between sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies (n = 4 per group).
Ijms 23 12091 g003
Figure 4. Mutations in insomniac significantly increase mortality following a single exposure to alcohol without increasing sensitivity to alcohol-induced sedation. The impact of sleep loss due to mutations in the inc1 and inc2 genes on alcohol sensitivity was assessed. (AC) Sleep profiles of w1118, inc1, and inc2 flies. inc1 and inc2 flies had significantly shorter daily sleep times ((A), ANOVA: F2,67 = 81.13, p < 0.0001), increased number of bouts ((B), ANOVA: F2,67 = 8.64, p < 0.0001), and shorter bout lengths ((C), ANOVA: F2,67 = 17.52, p < 0.0001) compared to w1118 flies. (D) inc1, inc2, and w1118 control flies 10 d of age were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with sedation assessed every 5 min during alcohol exposure. (E) inc1 and inc2 flies demonstrated increased resistance to alcohol vapor compared to w1118 controls (ANOVA: F2,34 = 47.28, p < 0.0001). (F) The complete time course for w1118, inc1, and inc2 flies. (GI) The impact of sleep loss due to mutations in inc1 and inc2 on alcohol-induced mortality was assessed. (G) w1118, inc1, and inc2 10 d old flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with mortality assessed every 24 h for 7 d following exposure to alcohol. (H) inc1 and inc2 mutant flies showed significantly increased mortality 24 h following exposure to alcohol compared to w1118 control flies (ANOVA: F2,29 = 16.46, p < 0.0001). (I) Mortality in inc1 and inc2 flies continued to rise 7 d following the initial exposure to alcohol (ANOVA: F2,29 = 20.87, p < 0.0001). * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Figure 4. Mutations in insomniac significantly increase mortality following a single exposure to alcohol without increasing sensitivity to alcohol-induced sedation. The impact of sleep loss due to mutations in the inc1 and inc2 genes on alcohol sensitivity was assessed. (AC) Sleep profiles of w1118, inc1, and inc2 flies. inc1 and inc2 flies had significantly shorter daily sleep times ((A), ANOVA: F2,67 = 81.13, p < 0.0001), increased number of bouts ((B), ANOVA: F2,67 = 8.64, p < 0.0001), and shorter bout lengths ((C), ANOVA: F2,67 = 17.52, p < 0.0001) compared to w1118 flies. (D) inc1, inc2, and w1118 control flies 10 d of age were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with sedation assessed every 5 min during alcohol exposure. (E) inc1 and inc2 flies demonstrated increased resistance to alcohol vapor compared to w1118 controls (ANOVA: F2,34 = 47.28, p < 0.0001). (F) The complete time course for w1118, inc1, and inc2 flies. (GI) The impact of sleep loss due to mutations in inc1 and inc2 on alcohol-induced mortality was assessed. (G) w1118, inc1, and inc2 10 d old flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with mortality assessed every 24 h for 7 d following exposure to alcohol. (H) inc1 and inc2 mutant flies showed significantly increased mortality 24 h following exposure to alcohol compared to w1118 control flies (ANOVA: F2,29 = 16.46, p < 0.0001). (I) Mortality in inc1 and inc2 flies continued to rise 7 d following the initial exposure to alcohol (ANOVA: F2,29 = 20.87, p < 0.0001). * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Ijms 23 12091 g004
Figure 5. Short-sleeping Dilp2 mutants have significantly increased mortality following a single exposure to alcohol with no effect on alcohol-induced sedation. (AC) Sleep profiles of w1118 and Dilp2 flies. Dilp2 male and female flies had significantly shorter daily sleep times ((A), ANOVA: F3,82 = 3.646, p = 0.0160) and shorter bout lengths compared to w1118 flies ((C), [ANOVA: F3,82 = 25.57, p < 0.001], * indicates significant differences as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; (B) [ANOVA: F3,82 = 13.81, p < 0.001], * indicates significant differences as identified by Bonferroni post hoc analysis). (D) Dilp2 and w1118 control flies 10 days of age were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with sedation assessed every 5 min during alcohol exposure. (E) No significant differences in behavioral sensitivity for sedation were observed between Dilp2 and w1118 controls (t(12) = 0.035, p = 0.9726). (F) The complete time course for w1118 and Dilp2 flies. (GI) The impact of sleep loss due to mutations in Dilp2 on alcohol-induced mortality was assessed. w1118 and Dilp2 10 d flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with mortality assessed every 24 h for 7 d following exposure to alcohol (G). Dilp2 mutant flies showed significantly increased mortality 24 h following exposure to alcohol compared to w1118 control flies (t(10) = 4.567, p = 0.0010). (H) Mortality in Dilp2 flies continued to rise 7 d following the initial exposure to alcohol (t(6) = 3.974, p = 0.0073). * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Figure 5. Short-sleeping Dilp2 mutants have significantly increased mortality following a single exposure to alcohol with no effect on alcohol-induced sedation. (AC) Sleep profiles of w1118 and Dilp2 flies. Dilp2 male and female flies had significantly shorter daily sleep times ((A), ANOVA: F3,82 = 3.646, p = 0.0160) and shorter bout lengths compared to w1118 flies ((C), [ANOVA: F3,82 = 25.57, p < 0.001], * indicates significant differences as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; (B) [ANOVA: F3,82 = 13.81, p < 0.001], * indicates significant differences as identified by Bonferroni post hoc analysis). (D) Dilp2 and w1118 control flies 10 days of age were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with sedation assessed every 5 min during alcohol exposure. (E) No significant differences in behavioral sensitivity for sedation were observed between Dilp2 and w1118 controls (t(12) = 0.035, p = 0.9726). (F) The complete time course for w1118 and Dilp2 flies. (GI) The impact of sleep loss due to mutations in Dilp2 on alcohol-induced mortality was assessed. w1118 and Dilp2 10 d flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with mortality assessed every 24 h for 7 d following exposure to alcohol (G). Dilp2 mutant flies showed significantly increased mortality 24 h following exposure to alcohol compared to w1118 control flies (t(10) = 4.567, p = 0.0010). (H) Mortality in Dilp2 flies continued to rise 7 d following the initial exposure to alcohol (t(6) = 3.974, p = 0.0073). * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Ijms 23 12091 g005
Figure 6. Pharmacologically increasing sleep in insomniac mutants significantly increases behavioral resistance to alcohol sedation and ameliorates alcohol-induced mortality. (AC) Comparison of sleep profiles between w1118 and THIP-fed inc1 and inc2 flies housed in LD cycles. Compared to control inc1 and inc2 flies, THIP-fed flies demonstrated significantly increased and consolidated sleep ((A), ANOVA: F7,158= 94.36, p < 0.001; * specifies significant differences between groups as indicated by Bonferroni post hoc analyses) with a decreased number of sleep bouts ((B), ANOVA: F7,158 = 45.10, p < 0.001), as well as increased bout duration for most groups ((C), ANOVA: F F7,158 = 42.62, p < 0.0001). (DH) To determine whether THIP treatment and increased sleep buffered alcohol sensitivity, inc1 and inc2 flies were aged in LD cycles for 9 d and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP or received THIP treatment while being sleep-deprived for 24 h. On day 10, inc1 and inc2 flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with sedation assessed every 5 min during alcohol exposure (D). Pharmacologically increasing sleep (NSD/THIP) in inc1 and inc2 flies significantly increased the resistance to alcohol-induced sedation compared to non-sleep-deprived (NSD) inc1 and inc2 flies ((E), ANOVA: F3,18 = 12.22, p < 0.001; and (G), ANOVA: F3, 16 = 44.38, p < 0.001, respectively; * specifies significant differences between groups). THIP treatment had no effect on alcohol sensitivity in sleep-deprived (SLD) inc1 and inc2 flies, indicating that the alcohol resistance observed in NSD THIP-treated flies was not due to buildup of GABA-A receptor tolerance. (F,H) The complete alcohol sedation time course. (IK) To determine whether increased sleep buffered against the toxic effects of alcohol, inc1, inc2, and w1118 flies were aged in LD cycles for 9 d and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP. On day 10, flies were exposed to 1 h of 50% alcohol vapor at ZT 9 with mortality assessed every 24 h for 7 d following exposure. Significantly reduced mortality was observed in THIP-fed inc1 and inc2 flies 24 h following alcohol exposure compared to inc1 and inc2 flies given alcohol alone ((I), ANOVA: F3,44 = 13.27, p < 0.0001); * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis). (J) Although mortality rose in THIP-fed inc1 and inc2 flies 7 d following alcohol exposure, the percent of THIP-fed inc1 and inc2 flies was still significantly lower than non-THIP fed inc1 and inc2 flies at 7 d after exposure to alcohol (ANOVA: F2,44 = 12.83, p < 0.0001). (L,M) To determine whether the decreased mortality observed in THIP-fed flies was due to GABA-A receptor activation or increased sleep, inc1 and inc2 flies were aged in LD cycles for 10 d and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP while being sleep-deprived for 24 h. At ZT 9 on day 10, the flies were exposed to 1 h of 50% alcohol vapor with mortality assessed every 24 h for 7 d following exposure. inc1 and inc2 flies sleep-deprived while on THIP media were significantly more susceptible to alcohol-induced mortality at 24 h (L) and 7 days (M) compared to non-sleep-deprived THIP-fed flies ((L), ANOVA: F5,54 = 7.743, p < 0.001; (M), ANOVA: F5,40 = 5.140, p < 0.001) * specifies significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Figure 6. Pharmacologically increasing sleep in insomniac mutants significantly increases behavioral resistance to alcohol sedation and ameliorates alcohol-induced mortality. (AC) Comparison of sleep profiles between w1118 and THIP-fed inc1 and inc2 flies housed in LD cycles. Compared to control inc1 and inc2 flies, THIP-fed flies demonstrated significantly increased and consolidated sleep ((A), ANOVA: F7,158= 94.36, p < 0.001; * specifies significant differences between groups as indicated by Bonferroni post hoc analyses) with a decreased number of sleep bouts ((B), ANOVA: F7,158 = 45.10, p < 0.001), as well as increased bout duration for most groups ((C), ANOVA: F F7,158 = 42.62, p < 0.0001). (DH) To determine whether THIP treatment and increased sleep buffered alcohol sensitivity, inc1 and inc2 flies were aged in LD cycles for 9 d and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP or received THIP treatment while being sleep-deprived for 24 h. On day 10, inc1 and inc2 flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with sedation assessed every 5 min during alcohol exposure (D). Pharmacologically increasing sleep (NSD/THIP) in inc1 and inc2 flies significantly increased the resistance to alcohol-induced sedation compared to non-sleep-deprived (NSD) inc1 and inc2 flies ((E), ANOVA: F3,18 = 12.22, p < 0.001; and (G), ANOVA: F3, 16 = 44.38, p < 0.001, respectively; * specifies significant differences between groups). THIP treatment had no effect on alcohol sensitivity in sleep-deprived (SLD) inc1 and inc2 flies, indicating that the alcohol resistance observed in NSD THIP-treated flies was not due to buildup of GABA-A receptor tolerance. (F,H) The complete alcohol sedation time course. (IK) To determine whether increased sleep buffered against the toxic effects of alcohol, inc1, inc2, and w1118 flies were aged in LD cycles for 9 d and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP. On day 10, flies were exposed to 1 h of 50% alcohol vapor at ZT 9 with mortality assessed every 24 h for 7 d following exposure. Significantly reduced mortality was observed in THIP-fed inc1 and inc2 flies 24 h following alcohol exposure compared to inc1 and inc2 flies given alcohol alone ((I), ANOVA: F3,44 = 13.27, p < 0.0001); * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis). (J) Although mortality rose in THIP-fed inc1 and inc2 flies 7 d following alcohol exposure, the percent of THIP-fed inc1 and inc2 flies was still significantly lower than non-THIP fed inc1 and inc2 flies at 7 d after exposure to alcohol (ANOVA: F2,44 = 12.83, p < 0.0001). (L,M) To determine whether the decreased mortality observed in THIP-fed flies was due to GABA-A receptor activation or increased sleep, inc1 and inc2 flies were aged in LD cycles for 10 d and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP while being sleep-deprived for 24 h. At ZT 9 on day 10, the flies were exposed to 1 h of 50% alcohol vapor with mortality assessed every 24 h for 7 d following exposure. inc1 and inc2 flies sleep-deprived while on THIP media were significantly more susceptible to alcohol-induced mortality at 24 h (L) and 7 days (M) compared to non-sleep-deprived THIP-fed flies ((L), ANOVA: F5,54 = 7.743, p < 0.001; (M), ANOVA: F5,40 = 5.140, p < 0.001) * specifies significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Ijms 23 12091 g006
Figure 7. Pharmacologically increasing sleep in short-sleeping Dilp2 mutants significantly reduces alcohol-induced mortality. (AF) Comparison of sleep profiles between w1118 and THIP-fed w1118 and Dilp2 flies housed in LD cycles. Compared to non-THIP-fed female w1118 and Dilp2 flies, THIP-fed flies demonstrated significantly increased total sleep ((A) [ANOVA: F3,98 = 10.95, p < 0.001]; * specifies significant differences between groups as indicated by Bonferroni post hoc analyses). Dilp2 THIP-fed females had significantly higher number of sleep bouts compared to non-THIP-fed controls ((B) ANOVA: F3,98 = 98.62, p < 0.001), while THIP treatment had no effect on bout duration ((C) ANOVA: F3,98 = 66.15, p < 0.001). w1118 female flies treated with THIP showed increased bout length. Compared to non-THIP-fed male Dilp2 flies, male THIP-treated flies had significantly increased sleep ((D) ANOVA: F3,91 = 7.815, p < 0.001), with an increased number of sleep bouts ((E) ANOVA: F3,91 = 55.54, p < 0.001) and slightly increased sleep duration ((F) ANOVA: F3,91 = 46.36, p < 0.001). Male THIP-fed w1118 flies exhibited significantly more consolidated sleep, as seen by decreased sleep bouts (E) and increased sleep bout duration (F), with no changes in total sleep amount (D). (GI) To determine whether increased sleep buffered against the toxic effects of alcohol, flies were aged in LD cycles for 9 d and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP. Pharmacologically increasing sleep (NSD/THIP) in Dilp2 mutants showed similar rates of sedation compared to w1118 THIP-treated flies (t(13) = 0.499, p = 0.6264). (J,K) Significantly reduced mortality was observed in THIP-treated Dilp2 flies 24 h following alcohol exposure compared to non-THIP-treated flies alone ((J) ANOVA: F3,21 = 8.106, p < 0.001) and 7 days later ((K) ANOVA: F3,13 = 10.03, p < 0.01). * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 7. Pharmacologically increasing sleep in short-sleeping Dilp2 mutants significantly reduces alcohol-induced mortality. (AF) Comparison of sleep profiles between w1118 and THIP-fed w1118 and Dilp2 flies housed in LD cycles. Compared to non-THIP-fed female w1118 and Dilp2 flies, THIP-fed flies demonstrated significantly increased total sleep ((A) [ANOVA: F3,98 = 10.95, p < 0.001]; * specifies significant differences between groups as indicated by Bonferroni post hoc analyses). Dilp2 THIP-fed females had significantly higher number of sleep bouts compared to non-THIP-fed controls ((B) ANOVA: F3,98 = 98.62, p < 0.001), while THIP treatment had no effect on bout duration ((C) ANOVA: F3,98 = 66.15, p < 0.001). w1118 female flies treated with THIP showed increased bout length. Compared to non-THIP-fed male Dilp2 flies, male THIP-treated flies had significantly increased sleep ((D) ANOVA: F3,91 = 7.815, p < 0.001), with an increased number of sleep bouts ((E) ANOVA: F3,91 = 55.54, p < 0.001) and slightly increased sleep duration ((F) ANOVA: F3,91 = 46.36, p < 0.001). Male THIP-fed w1118 flies exhibited significantly more consolidated sleep, as seen by decreased sleep bouts (E) and increased sleep bout duration (F), with no changes in total sleep amount (D). (GI) To determine whether increased sleep buffered against the toxic effects of alcohol, flies were aged in LD cycles for 9 d and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP. Pharmacologically increasing sleep (NSD/THIP) in Dilp2 mutants showed similar rates of sedation compared to w1118 THIP-treated flies (t(13) = 0.499, p = 0.6264). (J,K) Significantly reduced mortality was observed in THIP-treated Dilp2 flies 24 h following alcohol exposure compared to non-THIP-treated flies alone ((J) ANOVA: F3,21 = 8.106, p < 0.001) and 7 days later ((K) ANOVA: F3,13 = 10.03, p < 0.01). * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Ijms 23 12091 g007
Figure 8. Pharmacologically increasing sleep in flies that are circadianly disrupted reduces mortality following repeated exposures to alcohol. (AD) Comparison of sleep profiles between male 10 d CS flies housed in LD cycles and 10 d flies housed in constant light conditions (LL). Compared to control flies grown in a 12:12 h LD cycle, 10 d CS flies grown in LL exhibited significantly decreased total sleep time per day ((A) [t(236) = 4.46, p < 0.0001)]) with decreased total sleep during the subjective night ((B) ANOVA: F3,442 = 27.31, p < 0.0001). More fragmented sleep was observed in LL during the day and subjective night with an increased number of sleep bouts ((C) ANOVA: F3,442 = 98.99, p < 0.0001) and decreased bout duration compared to flies maintained in LD ((D) ANOVA: F3,442 = 78.45, p < 0.0001). LL 10 day flies fed 0.1 mg/mL THIP exhibited significantly more sleep compared to 10 d LL flies on control media ((E) t(233) = 29.43, p < 0.0001), increased sleep time during both the day and subjective night ((F) ANOVA: F3,463 = 437.1, p < 0.0001), decreased number of sleep bouts ((G) ANOVA: F3,463 = 358.1, p < 0.0001), and increased bout duration ((H) ANOVA: F3,463 = 277.6, p < 0.0001). (I) To determine whether increasing sleep was sufficient to ameliorate alcohol-induced mortality under conditions of circadian disruption, CS flies were housed in LL upon eclosion and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP on day 8 for 48 h. On days 10, 11, and 12, flies were subjected to a three-exposure repeated binge-like alcohol paradigm with 1 h alcohol exposure (50% alcohol vapor) occurring at ZT 9, and mortality was assessed. (J) Alcohol-induced mortality in THIP-fed LL flies was drastically reduced compared to control LL flies (ANOVA: F3,36 = 132.6, p < 0.0001). * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; **** p < 0.0001.
Figure 8. Pharmacologically increasing sleep in flies that are circadianly disrupted reduces mortality following repeated exposures to alcohol. (AD) Comparison of sleep profiles between male 10 d CS flies housed in LD cycles and 10 d flies housed in constant light conditions (LL). Compared to control flies grown in a 12:12 h LD cycle, 10 d CS flies grown in LL exhibited significantly decreased total sleep time per day ((A) [t(236) = 4.46, p < 0.0001)]) with decreased total sleep during the subjective night ((B) ANOVA: F3,442 = 27.31, p < 0.0001). More fragmented sleep was observed in LL during the day and subjective night with an increased number of sleep bouts ((C) ANOVA: F3,442 = 98.99, p < 0.0001) and decreased bout duration compared to flies maintained in LD ((D) ANOVA: F3,442 = 78.45, p < 0.0001). LL 10 day flies fed 0.1 mg/mL THIP exhibited significantly more sleep compared to 10 d LL flies on control media ((E) t(233) = 29.43, p < 0.0001), increased sleep time during both the day and subjective night ((F) ANOVA: F3,463 = 437.1, p < 0.0001), decreased number of sleep bouts ((G) ANOVA: F3,463 = 358.1, p < 0.0001), and increased bout duration ((H) ANOVA: F3,463 = 277.6, p < 0.0001). (I) To determine whether increasing sleep was sufficient to ameliorate alcohol-induced mortality under conditions of circadian disruption, CS flies were housed in LL upon eclosion and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP on day 8 for 48 h. On days 10, 11, and 12, flies were subjected to a three-exposure repeated binge-like alcohol paradigm with 1 h alcohol exposure (50% alcohol vapor) occurring at ZT 9, and mortality was assessed. (J) Alcohol-induced mortality in THIP-fed LL flies was drastically reduced compared to control LL flies (ANOVA: F3,36 = 132.6, p < 0.0001). * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; **** p < 0.0001.
Ijms 23 12091 g008
Figure 9. Pharmacologically increasing sleep in aging ameliorates alcohol-induced mortality. Compared to young 10 d flies, male 20 d flies grown in LD cycles exhibited significantly decreased average sleep time per day (A), decreased sleep during both the light and dark cycles (B), an increased number of sleep bouts (C), and decreased bout duration (D). (EH) Flies 20 days of age fed 0.1 mg/mL THIP exhibited significantly greater total sleep (E) compared to 20 d flies on control media, (F) increased daytime and nighttime sleep, (G) a decreased number of sleep bouts, and (H) increased bout length. (I) Flies were grown in 12 h light: 12 h dark cycles and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP on day 19 for 24 h. On days 20, 21, and 22, flies were subjected to a 3-exposure repeated binge-like alcohol paradigm with 1 h alcohol exposure (40% alcohol vapor) occurring at ZT 9, and mortality was assessed. (J) Increased rest in 20 d THIP-fed flies significantly ameliorated alcohol-induced mortality compared to 20 d flies fed control media (ANOVA F3,44 = 243.8, p < 0.0001). * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Figure 9. Pharmacologically increasing sleep in aging ameliorates alcohol-induced mortality. Compared to young 10 d flies, male 20 d flies grown in LD cycles exhibited significantly decreased average sleep time per day (A), decreased sleep during both the light and dark cycles (B), an increased number of sleep bouts (C), and decreased bout duration (D). (EH) Flies 20 days of age fed 0.1 mg/mL THIP exhibited significantly greater total sleep (E) compared to 20 d flies on control media, (F) increased daytime and nighttime sleep, (G) a decreased number of sleep bouts, and (H) increased bout length. (I) Flies were grown in 12 h light: 12 h dark cycles and transferred to media containing 0.1 mg/mL THIP on day 19 for 24 h. On days 20, 21, and 22, flies were subjected to a 3-exposure repeated binge-like alcohol paradigm with 1 h alcohol exposure (40% alcohol vapor) occurring at ZT 9, and mortality was assessed. (J) Increased rest in 20 d THIP-fed flies significantly ameliorated alcohol-induced mortality compared to 20 d flies fed control media (ANOVA F3,44 = 243.8, p < 0.0001). * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Ijms 23 12091 g009
Figure 10. Sleep deprivation differentially affects short-term and long-term functional tolerance. (AC) Effect of 24 h of sleep deprivation on short-term functional alcohol tolerance. (A) Wild-type CS flies were aged in 12:12 h LD cycle and sleep-deprived for 24 h on day 10. On day 11, flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 30 min and tested 4 h later by exposure to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h with sedation measured. (B) Sleep deprivation for 24 h had no significant effect on the development of short-term acute alcohol tolerance. Both sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies exhibited alcohol tolerance (ANOVA: F3,20 = 49.62, p < 0.0001). (C) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation for 10 d old sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies. (DF) Effect of sleep deprivation on development of long-term functional alcohol tolerance. CS flies were aged in 12:12 h LD cycle and sleep-deprived for 24 h on day 10. On day 11, flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 30 min and tested 24 h later by exposure to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with sedation measured. (E) Non-sleep-deprived flies exhibited robust alcohol tolerance (ANOVA: F3,26 = 125.7, p < 0.0001) with 24 h sleep deprivation significantly dampening development of long-term functional alcohol tolerance. (F) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation for 10 d old sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies. (G,H) The effect of sleep deprivation on the development of long-term alcohol tolerance in males and females was tested. (G) CS flies were aged in 12:12 h LD cycle with males and females in separate groups and sleep-deprived for 24 h. On day 11, flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 30 min and tested 24 h later with exposure to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h. (H) Sleep deprivation abolished the development of long-term tolerance in both males and females (ANOVA: F3,24 = 0.2390, p = 0.8682). (I) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation in each group. * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Figure 10. Sleep deprivation differentially affects short-term and long-term functional tolerance. (AC) Effect of 24 h of sleep deprivation on short-term functional alcohol tolerance. (A) Wild-type CS flies were aged in 12:12 h LD cycle and sleep-deprived for 24 h on day 10. On day 11, flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 30 min and tested 4 h later by exposure to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h with sedation measured. (B) Sleep deprivation for 24 h had no significant effect on the development of short-term acute alcohol tolerance. Both sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies exhibited alcohol tolerance (ANOVA: F3,20 = 49.62, p < 0.0001). (C) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation for 10 d old sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies. (DF) Effect of sleep deprivation on development of long-term functional alcohol tolerance. CS flies were aged in 12:12 h LD cycle and sleep-deprived for 24 h on day 10. On day 11, flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 30 min and tested 24 h later by exposure to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h at ZT 9 with sedation measured. (E) Non-sleep-deprived flies exhibited robust alcohol tolerance (ANOVA: F3,26 = 125.7, p < 0.0001) with 24 h sleep deprivation significantly dampening development of long-term functional alcohol tolerance. (F) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation for 10 d old sleep-deprived and non-sleep-deprived flies. (G,H) The effect of sleep deprivation on the development of long-term alcohol tolerance in males and females was tested. (G) CS flies were aged in 12:12 h LD cycle with males and females in separate groups and sleep-deprived for 24 h. On day 11, flies were exposed to 50% alcohol vapor for 30 min and tested 24 h later with exposure to 50% alcohol vapor for 1 h. (H) Sleep deprivation abolished the development of long-term tolerance in both males and females (ANOVA: F3,24 = 0.2390, p = 0.8682). (I) Complete time course of alcohol exposure showing percent of flies exhibiting sedation in each group. * indicates significant differences between groups as calculated by Bonferroni post hoc analysis; ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001, **** p < 0.0001.
Ijms 23 12091 g010
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

De Nobrega, A.K.; Noakes, E.J.; Storch, N.A.; Mellers, A.P.; Lyons, L.C. Sleep Modulates Alcohol Toxicity in Drosophila. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2022, 23, 12091. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms232012091

AMA Style

De Nobrega AK, Noakes EJ, Storch NA, Mellers AP, Lyons LC. Sleep Modulates Alcohol Toxicity in Drosophila. International Journal of Molecular Sciences. 2022; 23(20):12091. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms232012091

Chicago/Turabian Style

De Nobrega, Aliza K., Eric J. Noakes, Natalie A. Storch, Alana P. Mellers, and Lisa C. Lyons. 2022. "Sleep Modulates Alcohol Toxicity in Drosophila" International Journal of Molecular Sciences 23, no. 20: 12091. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms232012091

APA Style

De Nobrega, A. K., Noakes, E. J., Storch, N. A., Mellers, A. P., & Lyons, L. C. (2022). Sleep Modulates Alcohol Toxicity in Drosophila. International Journal of Molecular Sciences, 23(20), 12091. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms232012091

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop