Next Article in Journal
Disposable Fluorescence Optical pH Sensor for Near Neutral Solutions
Next Article in Special Issue
Recovery of Odorants from an Olfactometer Measured by Proton-Transfer-Reaction Mass Spectrometry
Previous Article in Journal
Deposition of Bacteriorhodopsin Protein in a Purple Membrane Form on Nitrocellulose Membranes for Enhanced Photoelectric Response
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

What is a Fresh Scent in Perfumery? Perceptual Freshness is Correlated with Substantivity

Department of Applied Statistics, Operations Research and Quality, Universidad Politécnica de Valencia, Cmno. Vera s/n, 46022 Valencia, Spain
Sensors 2013, 13(1), 463-483; https://doi.org/10.3390/s130100463
Submission received: 26 November 2012 / Revised: 19 December 2012 / Accepted: 20 December 2012 / Published: 28 December 2012

Abstract

:
Perfumes are manufactured by mixing odorous materials with different volatilities. The parameter that measures the lasting property of a material when applied on the skin is called substantivity or tenacity. It is well known by perfumers that citrus and green notes are perceived as fresh and they tend to evaporate quickly, while odors most dissimilar to ‘fresh’ (e.g., oriental, powdery, erogenic and animalic scents) are tenacious. However, studies aimed at quantifying the relationship between fresh odor quality and substantivity have not received much attention. In this work, perceptual olfactory ratings on a fresh scale, estimated in a previous study, were compared with substantivity parameters and antierogenic ratings from the literature. It was found that the correlation between fresh odor character and odorant substantivity is quite strong (r = −0.85). ‘Fresh’ is sometimes interpreted in perfumery as ‘cool’ and the opposite of ‘warm’. This association suggests that odor freshness might be somehow related to temperature. Assuming that odor perception space was shaped throughout evolution in temperate climates, results reported here are consistent with the hypothesis that ‘fresh’ evokes scents typically encountered in the cool season, while ‘warm’ would be evoked by odors found in nature during summer. This hypothesis is rather simplistic but it may provide a new insight to better understand the perceptual space of scents.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Fresh is an odor character descriptor commonly used in perfumery. Actually, it is the one most frequently encountered in a semantic olfactory database of 119 perfume materials reported by Thiboud [1]. In a recent study of this database, it was found that warm, oriental and powdery were the descriptors most dissimilar to fresh (see Figure 6 of [2]). Consistent with this result, fresh and oriental are regarded as opposite families of perfumes in the Fragrance Wheel proposed by Edwards [3]. The fresh category comprises citrus, green, water, and fruity subfamilies.
The Odor Effects Diagram is an olfactory representation of perfumery notes based on two basic polarities: (i) erogenous vs. antierogenous (refreshing) and (ii) narcotic vs. stimulating [4]. Citrus, green, watery, and aldehydic are regarded as refreshing scents, while erogenous, animal, musk, vanilla and powdery appear at the opposite pole (see Figure 5 of [2]). Fruity is located in this diagram between floral and fresh, and the same criterion was considered by Edwards [3].
Perfume is a complex mixture of odorants with different volatilities. The parameter that measures the lasting property of a material when applied on the skin is called substantivity or tenacity. It is well known by perfumers that olfactory notes perceived as fresh tend to evaporate quickly, while the opposite applies to those most dissimilar to fresh. Actually, fresh and green are attributes commonly encountered in the description of top notes (i.e., the ones that are perceived firstly when smelling a fragrance) [2]. Light refers to scents with high volatility, while heavy, rich or tenacious is applied to materials with high substantivity. Light fragrances are those perceived as non-sweet with a predominant fresh note that is often associated with citrus, greens or aldehydes [5]. Conversely, the least volatile ingredients such as mosses and animal scents dominate in heavy perfumes [1]. Vapor pressure is the basic factor that determines the volatility of a specific compound [6], but the vapor composition in equilibrium with the liquid is difficult to predict in mixtures due to the complex molecular interactions that occur [710].
Sensory ratings on a scale of freshness are difficult to obtain because the fresh dimension of olfactory perception is not well understood yet. Probably for this reason, psychophysical studies aimed at quantifying the relationship between this odor quality and tenacity have not received much attention yet. Several sensory maps of scents reported in the literature are investigated in the present work attempting to further understand the psychological aspects involved in the perception of refreshing odor character. The main target of the present work is to study the correlation between this odor quality and odorant substantivity. This relationship is well established in perfumery, but only at descriptive level.

2. Methods

In order to characterize the connection between perceived fresh odor character and substantivity, three stages have been carried out: (i) to estimate the freshness of odorant materials and descriptors on a numeric scale, (ii) to obtain substantivity values from the literature, and (iii) to study the correlation between them by means of simple linear regression. Each one of these stages is detailed below in a different section. In the present work, ‘fresh’ and ‘refreshing’ are used as synonyms. The different connotations of both terms are discussed in Section 4.4.

2.1. Classification of Odor Descriptors on a Scale of Perceptual Freshness

Hedonic tones of odor descriptors are available from the literature, and they can be used to interpret underlying dimensions of odor profile databases [11]. Similarly, it is possible to assign a refreshing tone to odor descriptors as described ahead. Boelens and Haring [12] asked a panel of six perfumers to smell 309 aroma chemicals and to rate on a 0–9 scale the odor similarity to 30 reference materials. Each one was selected as a standard for a certain odor character descriptor. The resulting sensory dataset, which will be referred to hereafter as B-H database, was analyzed in previous studies using principal components analysis (PCA) [2,11]. The first principal component (PC1) is the linear combination of variables (reference materials) that explains the maximum amount of data variability (17.5% in this case). The contributions of variables in the formation of a given PC are called loadings, being p1 and p2 the loadings corresponding to PC1 and PC2, respectively. PC1 was interpreted as a dimension of freshness because fresh was the descriptor with highest p1 loading. The p1 values can be regarded as (i) indirect assessments of the 30 reference materials on a scale of odor freshness, and (ii) refreshing tones of the odor descriptors associated to the reference materials.
Thiboud [1] reproduces a two-dimensional projection of a similarity matrix developed from interviews with consumers in England, USA, Spain, Brazil, and Japan relating to defined perfume bases and to verbal descriptions. The horizontal axis of this fragrance mapping discriminates masculine vs. feminine scents, and fresh is one of the descriptors that determine the vertical axis. The same interpretation was proposed for PC2 and PC1 of the B-H database [2]. Thus, a scatterplot of p2vs. p1, which is usually referred to as loading plot, was superimposed with Thiboud's sensory map, after being properly scaled and rotated in order to achieve the best matching between both odor representations.
Chastrette et al.[13] analyzed semantic odor profiles of 628 pure odorous substances commonly encountered in perfumery. The database was obtained by Firmenich SA from a team of seven perfumers who assessed each compound and assigned two to four notes chosen among 32 possible descriptors. The three most frequent ones were considered as the odor profile. A transformed matrix derived from this database was analyzed with PCA, and the PC1/PC2 loading plot was proposed as a sensory map of scents. I obtained the projections over the first factorial axis from this plot (Figure 1 of [13]) and compared these projections with p1 loadings of equivalent descriptors in the B-H database.
Different sensory maps of scents have been developed by chemical companies that supply perfume raw materials. One of them is the Rosace of Firmenich [14], which is comprised by 13 categories of scents properly arranged according to the opinion of professional perfumers. Citrus and balsamic are located at opposite positions, which suggests that this direction can be interpreted as a dimension of freshness. I obtained the orthogonal projections of all odor classes over this direction. Next, the correlation between these projections and p1 loadings from the B-H database was studied.
Another olfactory representation is the Field of Odors, which displays in a semicircle different descriptors and odor categories [15]. It was derived from an odor profile database of perfume materials. Attempting to study if freshness is a salient dimension of this odor map, I obtained the polar coordinates for the different odor classes (i.e., angular position in the semicircle and distance to the center). The angular coordinates were compared with p1 loadings of equivalent attributes in the B-H database.

2.2. Fresh Odor Quality and Antierogenic Character of Odorant Materials

Thiboud [1] compiled semantic odor profiles for 44 natural odorants and 75 synthetic chemicals. Each material was labeled with three or four main odor descriptors and with a set of ancillary attributes ranging from 0 to 14 (average = 6.6). This database was analyzed in a previous study [2] and it was found that freshness was an underlying dimension. Based on a few representative descriptors of this dimension, I obtained estimated scores on a fresh scale for the 44 natural odorants as described below.
Jellinek [4] conducted a sensory study attempting to classify perfume materials as erotic or not erotic. Three experienced perfumers assessed the erogenic character of 61 essential oils, 34 extracts and 105 aroma chemicals. Each material was added to four perfume compositions at a level that was just sufficient to create a clearly noticeable difference, and the panel had to decide whether the added material increased or decreased the erotic character of the perfume. From these sensory ratings, an antierogenic index (AI) was derived ranging from −100 to 100. The maximum value 100 indicates that the material was regarded by the panel as clearly antierogenic, while −100 corresponds to a noticeably erogenic scent. Jellinek [4] considered that antierogenic could also be interpreted as refreshing in odor description. To study this issue, I checked the correlation between p1 and AI of reference materials in the B-H database.

2.3. Substantivity Parameters of Perfume Materials

After conducting an extensive literature review, I found four substantivity parameters of perfume materials as described next. These parameters were not obtained in the same way and may not be accurate, but they are correlated and can be compared with perceptual freshness.
Perfumes are described according to their top, middle and base notes, which are also called head, heart and bottom notes, respectively. The top note is the first odor perceived when smelling a fragrance product, and it usually consists of the most volatile portion of the composition [1]. Middle notes, which represent the main body of a blend, are perceived after the top notes fade away [5]. Base notes are basically determined by fixative materials with a very low volatility and great substantivity that yield the characteristic lasting note of any fragrance. The three stages of evaporation are often illustrated with a pyramid [16]. The H&R Fragrance Guide [17] contains the semantic odor description of 820 commercial perfumes (367 men's and 453 women's). I counted the total number of times that a given attribute was applied to describe top (Ntop), middle (Nmid) and base notes (Nbase). A substantivity index was calculated according to Equation (1), which takes the value 0, 50 and 100 for attributes that are only applied to describe top, middle and base notes, respectively. Thus, it provides an estimation of the substantivity associated to a given descriptor on a 0-100 scale:
Substantivity index = 50 N mid + 100 N base N top + N mid + N base
Poucher [18] obtained the duration of evaporation by olfaction for 332 odorous materials. A certain quantity of each odorant was placed on a paper strip, and he determined the time that the smelling strip retained the typical odor note. Based on the results, a coefficient of substantivity (CS) from 1 to 100 was assigned to each material.
Another substantivity parameter was reported by Appell [7], who calculated the volatility of 81 essential oils by obtaining the amount of oil evaporated in various time intervals. The quantity (mg) evaporated in one hour from one gram of oil was called evaporation index.
The website of The Good Scents Company ( www.thegoodscentscompany.com) provides useful information about suppliers, safety, organoleptic properties, and physical parameters of aroma chemicals and natural perfume materials. Substantivity values, which will be referred as SGS, are available for most odorants. This parameter is measured in hours and ranges from 1 to 400 h. The maximum SGS in fact indicates a censored value higher than 400 (i.e., ≥16.7 days). Actually, certain materials can smell for months [8].

2.4. Relationship between Fresh Odor Character and Substantivity

The p1 loadings derived from the B-H database can be regarded as assessments of the 30 reference materials on a perceptual scale of odor freshness. These values can also be interpreted as refreshing tones of the odor descriptors associated to reference materials (i.e., the degree of perceptual similarity or dissimilarity of a given odor quality with respect to the fresh odor character). The correlation between p1 loadings and the substantivity index of equivalent descriptors was studied by means of linear regression. The evaporation index and SGS are available for most reference materials in the B-H database. Thus, the correlation between p1, EI and SGS was also checked.
CS values are available for the 44 odorants used by Thiboud [1]. I studied the correlation of CS with the scores of freshness derived from odor profiles, as well as with AI and other substantivity parameters. Additional materials commonly used in perfumery were also taken into consideration.

3. Results

The comparison of refreshing tones derived from odor profiles and sensory maps yields consistent results, as indicated in the next section. This issue is of interest to further understand the psychological issues involved in the perception of fresh odor character. Section 3.2 describes the correlation between the fresh odor character and substantivity.

3.1. Freshness as an Underlying Dimension in Odor Maps

Figure 1 shows the PC1/PC2 loading plot of the B-H database, superimposed with a rotated fragrance map [1]. Although the position of comparable descriptors is not exactly coincident, both sensory maps are strikingly similar. An odor map based on equivalent dimensions has also been reported [19]. Certain disparity exists in Figure 1 for some attributes (e.g., spicy, aldehydic, honey, or animal) probably due to the lack of consensus in their interpretation. For example, eugenol and cinnamon are frequently chosen by perfumers as a reference for spicy[20], but the latter smells sweeter. Decanal was the reference for aldehydic in the B-H database. It smells antierogenic, but most aldehydes are perceived as erogenic [4], which would explain the disparity of aldehydic. Honey scents are often described in perfumery as sweet-medicinal, and Abe et al.[21] found a similarity between honey and animal, which would clarify the discrepancy of honey in Figure 1.
Regarding the vertical axis of Figure 1, men-husbands appears at the bottom and feminine is found at the opposite side. This result further supports the hypothesis of a dimension influenced by psychological and cultural aspects that discriminates feminine vs. masculine cosmetic odors, as discussed elsewhere [2,19].
Another sensory map of scents was obtained by Chastrette et al.[13]. Most descriptors in this odor map have a direct correspondence with attributes in the B-H database. Herbaceous was paired with vegetable because the reference for the latter smells like herbs [2]. Medicinal was matched with phenolic because a significant similarity between both descriptors was found in a previous study [22]. Amber was paired with erogenic because a mixture of ambergris and costus oil was the reference material for erogenic in the B-H database. Figure 2 compares the projection of olfactory notes over the first factorial axis obtained by Chastrette et al.[13] with p1 loadings of equivalent descriptors in the B-H database. The correlation is statistically significant (r = 0.86, p < 10−4), which reveals that freshness is the most salient dimension.
Figure 3 shows the 13 odor classes contained in the Rosace of Firmenich [14]. Citrus and aldehydic are located in opposite positions with respect to balsamic and powdery, which suggests that this direction (solid line in Figure 3 left) can be regarded as the fresh dimension. Scores of freshness (Sfreshness) were obtained as the distance between the orthogonal projection of odor classes (dots in the figure) over the solid line and the right end of this line, measured in a metric arbitrary scale. Most of these odor classes can be directly matched with attributes in the B-H database. Herbaceous and pyrogeneous were paired with vegetable and smoky, respectively, based on their odor similarity. If Sfreshness is compared with p1 loadings from the B-H database, it turns out that the correlation is statistically significant (r = 0.84, p = 0.0003), which indicates that freshness is an underlying dimension of this odor map. Interestingly, the orthogonal direction to the solid line in Figure 3 is defined by floral vs. earthy, which are also located at opposite positions in Figure 1. However, the position of fruity is arguable taking into account that this descriptor is usually regarded as fresh [3].
The Field of Odors [15] displays different odor classes from citrus in one end to animal at the other, which suggests that categories are arranged according to perceptual freshness. In order to study this issue, I compared the angular position (α) of each class with the p1 loading of equivalent attributes in the B-H database. The descriptors vegetable, earthy, powdery, erogenic, aromatic, coniferous, watery, and sweet from the B-H database were paired with herbaceous, mossy, musk, amber, vanilla, terpenic, marine and caramel, respectively, from the Field of Odors, which refer to similar smells. The correlation between α and p1 is statistically significant (r = 0.82, p < 10−4), which confirms that freshness is also the most salient dimension in the Field of Odors. Curiously, a similar correlation coefficient is obtained in Figure 2 (r = 0.86) and Figure 3 (r = 0.84). It is important to keep in mind that odor descriptors are not always interpreted in the same way, which would partly explain the residual variability observed in Figures 24. For example, eugenol is usually regarded as the reference material for spicy, but some perfumers may choose cinnamon [20], which smells sweeter.

3.2. Relationship between Fresh Odor Character and Substantivity

Most reference materials and attributes in the B-H database have a direct correspondence with descriptors used by the H&R guide (Table 1). Some attributes were paired taking into account their similarity in perfumery: erogenic—sensual; animalcastoreum[23]; earthymossy[21]; smokyleathery[14,21,24]; powderywarm[2]. The p1 loadings and substantivity index (SI) of these descriptors are shown in Table 1. Table 2 displays all reference materials in the B-H database (except buttery) and indicates p1, AI, SI, SGS, and evaporation index (EI). Strikingly, p1 is tightly correlated with SI (Table 1: r = −0.81, p < 10−4) and with EI0.5 (Table 2: r = 0.70, p = 0.005). The square root transformation (i.e., EI0.5) is required to normalize the data. Moreover, AI is also significantly correlated with p1 (r = 0.75, p < 10−4), SI (r = −0.63, p = 0.001), SGS (r = −0.68, p = 0.0003) and with EI0.5 (r = 0.72, p = 0.006). Taking into account that (i) antierogenic and refreshing are equivalent concepts [4], and (ii) p1 loadings can be interpreted as sensory ratings on a scale of freshness, the observed correlation among p1, AI, SI, SGS, and EI reveals that materials with a low substantivity tend to be perceived as fresh or refreshing.
In Thiboud's database, certain material was described with a given attribute as main or ancillary descriptor if that odor character was clearly recognizable or just noticeable, respectively. Because of this, odor descriptors can be coded numerically as 0 if that term is not applied, 1 if it is applied as ancillary descriptor, and 2 for the main attributes. These coded descriptions are shown in Table 3 for 6 relevant attributes: (i) fresh and citrus, which account for the fresh odor character, (ii) balsamic and oriental, which are dissimilar to fresh, and (iii) floral and agrestic, with an intermediate refreshing tone (Figure 1). By adding the values in columns fresh, citrus, floral and agrestic, and subtracting the columns balsamic and oriental, it results a numeric variable from −3 to 6 that can be interpreted as sensory scores on a fresh scale (Sfresh in Table 3). Interestingly, Sfresh is significantly correlated with SI (r = −0.85, p < 10−4), CS (r = −0.77, p < 10−4) and SGS (r = −0.61, p < 10−4). The tight correlation once again evidences that fresh odor character reflects odorant tenacity.
Substantivity and AI values of additional materials not included in Table 3 are shown in Table 4. If both tables are merged, it turns out that SI is strongly correlated with CS (r = 0.88, p < 10−4) and SGS (r = 0.83, p < 10−4), but SGS yields a weaker correlation with EI0.5 (r = −0.61, p = 0.0001) probably because many EI values are missing for the least volatile materials.
Poucher [18] classified odorants as top notes (CS < 15), middle notes (15 ≤ CS ≤ 60) and base notes (CS >60). Interestingly, most materials listed in Table 3 up to elemi oil are basically encountered in top notes (SI ≪ 50) and their CS is below 15. Moreover, materials from vetiver oil to tolu can be regarded as base notes (SI ≈ 100) and they exhibit a high substantivity (CS ≫ 60). Thus, Poucher's classification based on CS values seems adequate. Some materials with a low CS (top notes) like mimosa absolute, narcissus absolute or copaiba present a high substantivity according to The Good Scents Company (SGS = 400), which suggests that such particular values might not be reliable. Actually, the website does not indicate how this parameter was obtained. Moreover, 5 out of the 8 outliers highlighted in Figure 5 correspond to SGS values.
Most materials in Table 2 with p1 > 0 smell antierogenic (AI > 0) and, as a result, the correlation between p1 and AI is statistically significant (r = 0.75, p < 10−4). In Table 3, AI is positively correlated with Sfresh (r = 0.42, p = 0.01). By merging Tables 3 and 4, it turns out that AI is also correlated with SI (r = −0.60, p < 10−4), SGS (r = −0.50, p = 0.0004) and CS (r = −0.45, p = 0.0006). Given the relationship between substantivity and freshness, the observed correlations support Jellinek's interpretation of antierogenous and refreshing as equivalent concepts in odor description. Nonetheless, most floral odors were regarded by Jellinek [4] as erogenic (AI ≤ −50), which is somewhat unexpected because floral scents present an intermediate refreshing tone (Figure 1) and substantivity (SI ≈ 50 in Table 4). The reason might be that the three perfumers who obtained AI values were probably men. Floral scents, which are perceived as feminine, may be regarded by men as erogenic (i.e., arousing sexual desire for the opposite sex). Thus, a larger panel should have been used with the same number of male and female individuals. Actually, Jellinek [4] recognized that his results were preliminary because of the difficulties in reaching an agreement among observers.

4. Discussion

4.1. Freshness as a Salient Dimension of Odor Perception

Smell is a complex multidimensional perception difficult to measure and describe [26]. The underlying constructs of this multivariate space are still poorly understood. A relatively easy procedure is proposed here to study if freshness is a salient dimension of odor maps, by assigning a refreshing tone to odor character descriptors. This issue is of interest for developing standard sensory maps of cosmetic odors.
Different works have reported that freshness is a latent dimension in the perceptual space of fragrances (for review, see [2]). However, this idea is not well established yet in olfactory research. Actually, neither fresh nor refreshing are included in the comprehensive Dravnieks' list of 146 terms most commonly used in odor description [27]. Moreover, the fresh dimension was not considered by some studies that have discussed several odor maps of perfumery scents [13,28]. Edwards classifies 12% of fragrances in the citrus, green, watery or fruity categories, which are grouped in the fresh family [2]. But fresh is not considered by most fragrance companies as a main olfactory family of perfumes (see Table 1 of [29]). This category is also missing in the classification of fragrances proposed by the French Society of Perfumers [30], Fragrantica, or Osmoz by Firmenich, among many others. The reason could be the lack of consensus in the interpretation of fresh in odor description, as discussed below.
Many psychophysical studies have reported that pleasantness is the most salient dimension when a wide range of odors are assessed (for review, see [31]). This dimension did not clearly show up in a previous study of the B-H database by checking the correlation between loadings and hedonic tones from the literature [11]. Although the same methodology could have been applied here, there is enough evidence to support that freshness is the basic construct underlying Figures 24. Actually, animal, mossy or earthy are often described as disagreeable odors, but these descriptors appear close to sweet or spicy that are regarded as pleasant.

4.2. Proposed Hypothesis to Explain the High Substantivity of Erogenic Odors

According to Jellinek [4], erogenous scents are those arousing sexual desire. They are basically reminiscent of the human body smell (i.e., odors released by our skin) and somewhat resemble the smell of materials obtained from animals (e.g., civet, castoreum, ambergris or Tonkin musk). This similarity between erogenic and animal is apparent in the B-H database because both descriptors are correlated (r = 0.46) and determine an independent dimension of odor character (see Figure 3 of [2]). Body odorants are obviously easily retained by the skin and, hence, they present a high substantivity. The same applies to animalic materials. This reasoning is consistent with the observed negative correlation between antierogenic odor character and substantivity parameters (Tables 24).
Taking into account that substantivity depends on physicochemical properties, the tight correlation of substantivity with freshness implies that, in the case of aroma chemicals, their refreshing odor character could be estimated by means of electronic noses [32] and also based on physical properties. In the B-H database, the projections of odorants over PC1 can be interpreted as estimated scores of freshness on a numeric scale. Work in progress suggests that these scores can be predicted from a set of 20 physicochemical parameters and molecular descriptors using multiple linear regression, with a coefficient of determination R2 ≈ 0.52.
This preliminary result further suggests that the correlation of fresh odor character with substantivity is primarily innate and evolutionarily hardwired. Nonetheless, olfactory perception of fragrance raw materials can drastically change when applied in a mixture (e.g., a perfume), in a paper blotter or on the skin, thus possibly changing its substantivity. Further research will be necessary to study this issue. The development of standard psychophysical procedures to obtain accurate values of substantivity and scores of freshness for perfumery materials is strongly encouraged.

4.3. Conditioned and Unconditioned Factors Influencing Odor Quality Perception

It is well established that odor quality perception is strongly influenced by memory [33], learning [34] and culture [35,36]. Social effects affecting the perception of fragrances have also been discussed [37]. The human fetus starts the learning process of smelling in the prenatal environment [38]. Flavors from the mother's diet during pregnancy are transmitted to amniotic fluid and swallowed by the fetus [39], which may provide the foundation for cultural and ethnic differences in odor preference.
Despite the general belief that odor perception is basically shaped by learned associations, some studies reveal that certain aspects of olfactory perception might be innate and hardwired. For example, rats show an unconditioned defensive response to the odor of predators [40]. Moreover, recent works have revealed that the hedonic odor character is partly determined by molecular structure [31]. Interestingly, freshness is the most salient perceptual dimension of cosmetic scents and it is also somewhat encoded by molecular structure given the correlation with substantivity. Although many efforts have been carried out to relate molecular structure to the perceived smell [41], odor-structure relationships focused on the fresh olfactory quality have not been reported yet as far as I know. Ferdenzi et al.[42] found that the refreshing/energizing dimension of smell was common to three tested cultures, but other constructs were different. The neurological basis of innate and learned responses to odors has been investigated in mice [43].

4.4. Interpretation of ‘Fresh’ vs. ‘Refreshing’ in Odor Description

The Collins Dictionary & Thesaurus[44] considers the following terms as synonyms of refreshing: new, original, novel, unusual, stimulating, innovative, cooling, invigorating, etc.. Probably based on the semantic associations, different studies have found that refreshing and invigorating are similar odor descriptors [19]. Actually, a fresh fragrance is usually considered invigorating, nature inspired, reminiscent of early morning air or sea breeze and it is typified by green, citrus notes [1]. Chrea et al.[45] found that refreshed and revitalized were interpreted in the same way by French speakers, and both terms were classified in an emotional factor of odor perception called energizing/refreshing. The same association was found in a further study carried out in Liverpool (UK) and Singapore, and the underlying factor was named energy[42]. Refreshing and stimulating are regarded as orthogonal dimensions of the Odor Effects Diagram [4], but this issue is arguable. In the odor map proposed by Tisserand [46], vivacity and stimulant appear next to erogenic, which is also debatable based on the reported empirical evidence.
Given the multiple meanings of refreshing, this term might be interpreted differently according to culture, language, context, age, and experience. This issue may explain why Jellinek [4] regarded refreshing and fresh as different odor descriptors. The former was associated with antierogenous and the latter with green–herbaceous odors, which were supposed to produce antierogenous and stimulating effects. However, this criterion is arguable given the semantic association between refreshing and stimulating. Moreover, odors considered by Jellinek [4] as stimulating are better described as masculine [2]. Although fresh and refreshing may have different connotations in odor description according to language, their different meaning is not clearly established in perfumery and, hence, in my opinion both terms can be used as synonyms for clarity purposes.

4.5. Controversies in the Interpretation of ‘Fresh’ in Odor Description

The association between refreshing and energizing might correspond to the activation dimension of emotion (arousal) [45], but another interpretation is possible. According to Jellinek [29], the refreshing dimension underlies consumers' perceptions as to whether a fragrance seems to be more suited for formal evening wear or for informal daytime wear. People feel more comfortable wearing during the day fragrances that do not transmit an erotic message (i.e., smelling antierogenic), which seems quite obvious. Thus, refreshing scents are preferred for daytime wear because they are not perceived as erotic, and daytime is associated with stimulating activities, energy, vigorous actions, etc.
Although fresh was long ago proposed as an independent category of odors [47], this particular odor quality is rather subjective and it can be interpreted differently. According to Müller [24], fresh is generally associated in European regions with lemon, lavender, green notes and light floral components. Different studies support this interpretation [2]. For example, a so-called light dimension determined by the descriptors fresh and lemon was found by analyzing numeric odor profiles of 40 compounds rated by French individuals [48]. However, Müller states that sweet and powdery perfumes are also considered as fresh in North America [24]. Taking into account that sweet and powdery are perceived as dissimilar to fresh[2], it is of interest to discuss this controversy.
Fresh can be defined in English as (i) newly made or obtained, novel, original, additional, (ii) not stale or deteriorated, (iii) not canned, frozen or preserved, etc.[49]. Consistent with this definition, Schiffman et al.[50] regarded fresh and rotten as two polarities of the same semantic differential scale. By contrast, the first meaning of fresh in French (frais) and Spanish (fresco) is ‘moderately cool’ [51,52], which is antonym of warm. The semantic association between fresh and cool also exists in English, but only when applied to weather, wind or breeze. Thus, fresh air means clean and cool, found outside buildings rather than in a room [49]. In French, the similarity between refreshed, clean and cooling in odor perception has also been reported [45].
I believe that fresh was used originally in the sense of cool, probably long ago by French perfumers. Unfortunately, fresh and cool cannot be regarded nowadays as synonyms because cool has acquired many connotations in perfumery [1]. Cool is often applied to the perception of freshness associated with a trigeminal effect, as it is the case of minty scents. Actually, minty yields the highest correlation with ‘cool, cooling’ (r = 0.82) in the database of Dravnieks [27]. Moreover, Harper [53] selected two odorants sharing a minty–fresh odor character (menthol and camphor) as references for ‘cool, cooling’. In order to avoid confusion, I would recommend using fresh, refreshing and antierogenic as synonyms, referred to scents perceived as cooling and not associated with a trigeminal effect.

4.6. Fresh and Warm as Opposite Polarities of a Perceptual Odor Dimension

Based on the semantic meaning, French and Spanish people would easily agree that fresh and warm are two opposite poles of the same perceptual odor dimension, because they will readily associate fresh with cool. The same link probably exists in Japanese, because a fresh dimension that discriminated cold and sour with respect to warm, sweet and sexy was found by Japanese researchers who analyzed with PCA numeric odor profiles of 37 aroma chemicals according to 55 descriptors [32]. Conversely, in English, not everybody would understand the dissimilarity fresh vs. warm unless it is indicated that fresh is used as a synonym of cool. The lack of consensus in the semantic interpretation of freshness might explain why this perception is not easy to define in perfumery [1,24].
Refreshing and erogenous were regarded by Jellinek [4] as opposite polarities of the same dimension. Consistent with this criterion, AI values are positively correlated with p1 (Table 2) and Sfresh (Table 3). In the B-H database, the negative correlation between fresh and erogenic (r = −0.24, p < 10−4) partly supports Jellineks' interpretation, but fresh yields the most negative correlation with powdery (r = −0.58). The reference material for this descriptor smells warm [2], which suggests that the perceptual dimension of freshness is better interpreted as a fresh vs. warm polarity rather than fresh vs. erogenous.
This interpretation is supported by further evidence from the literature. Warm perfumes have a high proportion of animalic ingredients [24] and are often described as rich and tenacious [1]. Harper [53] selected costus and amber as standards for warm. Both materials were regarded by Boelens & Haring [12] as references for erogenic, which implies that warm, erogenic and animal are similar descriptors in perfumery. Actually, in a two-dimensional odor map recently reported, warm and sexy appeared close to each other [45]. The origin of this similarity from an evolutionary perspective is not well understood yet, but some hypotheses have been proposed [54].
In a reported analysis of the language of French perfume advertising, it was found that the dimension warm vs. fresh was the most important axis of the semantic field of fragrances [55]. Chrea et al.[45] obtained a two-dimensional odor map from numeric ratings of 24 odorant samples according to 73 affective terms in French, and it was found that refreshed and warm appeared in opposite positions within the pleasant cluster of terms. Jellinek [29] developed a two-dimensional map of fragrances and one of the dimensions was interpreted as warm vs. cool. In a previous study, 140 commercial perfumes were also classified according to a cool vs. warm dimension [56]. Given that cool and warm are terms referring to temperature, these studies support the interpretation of fresh and warm as opposite polarities of a perceptual odor dimension somehow associated with temperature. Interestingly, colors can also be described on a warm vs. cool scale [57].

4.7. Proposed Hypotheses to Explain the Fresh/Warm Dimension of Odor Character

Taking into account the alternation of seasons in temperate climates, with cool temperatures in winter and warmer conditions in summer, the ability of human olfaction to perceive scents as fresh vs. warm might be explained from an evolutionary perspective. In summer, as a result of the hot weather, only those odorants with higher substantivity would be found in the natural environment, while light scents could be encountered at cooler temperatures. This hypothesis is consistent with the fact that fresh scents are characterized by a lower substantivity, while the opposite applies to warm odors.
The idea that olfactory perceptual space in humans might reflect chronobiological annual rhythms was put forward by Tisserand [46]. Starting from the four elements (air, fire, earth and water) long ago proposed by Greek philosophers like Hippocrates, he discussed their associations with the four seasons and with moods evoked by essential oils. It was found that the resulting structure had certain resemblance to the Odor Effects Diagram [4]. This diagram has been recently discussed in detail [2]. Tisserand suggested that erogenic scents evoked odors typically encountered in summer, while those associated with winter such as watery were regarded as antierogenic. Autumn was associated with earthy, which is intuitively appealing because this descriptor has a neutral refreshing tone (p1 ≈ 0). Moreover, earthy is the attribute most dissimilar to floral (Figure 1), and the latter would correspond to the typical smells of spring.
The olfactory map shown in Figure 1 might reflect an underlying structure of odor perception that discriminates those scents most typically encountered in the different seasons in temperate climates: fresh (winter), floral (spring), warm (summer), and earthy (autumn). The position of summer next to fruity in Figure 1 might suggest that summer scents are perceived as fresh, which is misleading. The correct interpretation is that fresh scents are preferred in summer, probably because they evoke cool temperatures. Conversely, warm and animalic fragrances are preferred in winter [29].
The work of Tisserand [46] leads to the hypothesis that human olfactory perception was shaped throughout evolution in temperate climates to recognize the characteristic odors of each season. This theory is debatable because our sense of smell did not evolve under a constant climate. Moreover, it is uncertain if the fresh odor character is perceived differently by human races that developed in climates with slight weather variations along the year, or by people living in such climates. The proposed theory seems very simplistic given the complexity of factors affecting olfactory perception, but it deserves to be further investigated because it might provide the fundamental basis to better understand certain psychological aspects of olfactory perception. For example, it is still poorly understood to what extent the perception of freshness depends on culture, language, context, age, etc. It would also be of interest to study why sweet fragrances are preferred by women, or why cosmetic odors are perceived as masculine vs. feminine.

5. Conclusions

Odor freshness is an underlying dimension in the perceptual space of perfumery scents, as further studied here. Fresh odors are typical in top notes, while those most dissimilar to fresh are encountered in base notes. This relationship between freshness and substantivity is well known by perfumers, but it has not received much attention yet by the scientific community. The present work reports that the correlation between perceptual freshness and odorant substantivity is quite strong (p1vs. SI in Table 1: r = −0.82; Sfreshvs. SI in Table 3: r = −0.85). This result is important because substantivity depends on physicochemical properties, which provides the fundamental basis to predict odor freshness based on molecular structure.
The interpretation of fresh in perfumery as synonym of cool and antonym of warm suggests that this psychological dimension might be associated with temperature and may reflect a structure of odor perception shaped throughout evolution in temperate climates. Thus, fresh would correspond to scents typically encountered in the cool season, while warm would be evoked by odors found in nature during summer.

References

  1. Thiboud, M. Empirical Classification of Odors. In Perfumes: Art, Science, and Technology; Müller, P.M., Lamparsky, D., Eds.; Elsevier: New York, NY, USA, 1991; pp. 253–286. [Google Scholar]
  2. Zarzo, M.; Stanton, D.T. Understanding the underlying dimensions in perfumers' odor perception space as a basis for developing meaningful odor maps. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2009, 71, 225–247. [Google Scholar]
  3. Edwards, M. Fragrances of the World: Parfums du Monde 2010, 26th ed.; Fragrances of the World: Sidney, BC, Canada, 2010; pp. 6–8. [Google Scholar]
  4. Jellinek, P. The Psychological Basis of Perfumery. In The Psychological Basis of Perfumery, 4th ed.; Jellinek, J.S., Ed.; Chapman & Hall: London, UK, 1997; pp. 1–162. [Google Scholar]
  5. Green, M. Natural Perfumes: Simple Aromatherapy Recipes; Interweave Press: Loveland, CO, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  6. Appell, L. Physical foundations in perfumery. Part III. Vapor pressure. Am. Perf. Cosmet. 1964, 79, 29–41. [Google Scholar]
  7. Appell, L. Physical foundations in perfumery. Part VI. Volatility of the essential oils. Am. Perf. Cosmet. 1968, 83, 37–47. [Google Scholar]
  8. Sturm, W.; Mansfeld, G. Tenacity and fixing of aromatic chemicals. Perf. Flav. 1976, 1, 6–16. [Google Scholar]
  9. Rudolfi, T.A.; Shchedrina, M.M.; Mindlin, L.O. Determination of the evaporation rate of essential oils and perfumery compositions using gas chromatography. Chromatographia 1988, 25, 520–522. [Google Scholar]
  10. Perring, K.D. Volatility and Substantivity. In The Chemistry of Fragrances; Pybus, D.H., Sell, C.S., Eds.; RSC Publishing: London, UK, 1999; pp. 188–201. [Google Scholar]
  11. Zarzo, M. Relevant psychological dimensions in the perceptual space of perfumery odors. Food Qual. Prefer. 2008, 19, 315–322. [Google Scholar]
  12. Boelens, H.; Haring, H.G. Molecular Structure and Olfactive Quality; Naarden International (internal report): Bussum, The Netherlands, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  13. Chastrette, M.; de Saint Laumer, J.Y.; Sauvegrain, P. Analysis of a system of description of odors by means of four different multivariate statistical methods. Chem. Senses 1991, 16, 81–93. [Google Scholar]
  14. The Rosace of Odors (Firmenich, 1972). Available online: http://membres.multimania.fr/sensonet/langa/rosace-firmenich.html (accessed on 1 July 2012).
  15. Jaubert, J.N.; Gordon, G.; Doré, J.C. Une organisation du champ des odeurs. II. Modéle descriptif de l'organisation de l'espace odorant. Parf. Cosmet. Ar. 1987, 78, 71–82. [Google Scholar]
  16. Brechbill, G.O. The Scent Pyramid; Fragrance Books Inc.: NJ. Available online: http://www.perfumerbook.com/ Scent%20Pyramid.pdf (accessed on 11 December 2012).
  17. Glöss, W. (Ed.) H&R Fragrance Guide: Feminine Notes, Masculine Notes, 2nd ed.; Glöss Verlag: Hamburg, Germany, 1991.
  18. Poucher, W.A. Perfumes, Cosmetics and Soaps, Vol. 2: The Production, Manufacture and Application of Perfumes, 8th ed.; Chapman & Hall: London, UK, 1974; pp. 47–67. [Google Scholar]
  19. Richardson, A. Measurement of Fragrance Perception. In The Chemistry of Fragrances; Pybus, D.H., Sell, C.S., Eds.; RSC Publishing: London, UK, 1999; pp. 145–157. [Google Scholar]
  20. Brud, W.S. Word versus odors: How perfumers communicate. Perf. Flav. 1986, 11, 27–44. [Google Scholar]
  21. Abe, H.; Kanaya, S.; Komukai, T.; Takahashi, Y.; Sasaki, S. Systemization of semantic descriptions of odors. Anal. Chim. Acta 1990, 239, 73–85. [Google Scholar]
  22. Chastrette, M.; Elmouaffek, A.; Sauvegrain, P. A multidimensional statistical study of similarities between 74 notes used in perfumery. Chem. Senses 1988, 13, 295–305. [Google Scholar]
  23. Aftelier. Natural Perfume Wheel; Aftelier Perfumes Inc.: Berkeley, CA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  24. Müller, J. The H&R Book of Perfume: Understanding Fragrance; Origins, History, Development; Guide to Fragrance Ingredients, 1st ed.; Glöss Verlag: Hamburg, Germany, 1992; pp. 66–74. [Google Scholar]
  25. Poucher, W.A. Perfumes, Cosmetics and Soaps, Vol. 1: The Raw Materials of Perfumery, 7th ed.; Chapman & Hall: London, UK, 1974; pp. 116–119. [Google Scholar]
  26. Wise, P.M.; Olsson, M.J.; Cain, W.S. Quantification of odor quality. Chem. Senses 2000, 25, 429–443. [Google Scholar]
  27. Dravnieks, A. Atlas of Odor Character Profiles, Data Series DS 61; American Society for Testing and Materials: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  28. Teixeira, M.A.; Rodríguez, O.; Rodrigues, A.E. Perfumery radar: A predictive tool for perfume family classification. Ind. Eng. Chem. Res. 2010, 49, 11764–11777. [Google Scholar]
  29. Jellinek, J.S. Perfume Classification: A New Approach. In Fragrance: The Psychology and Biology of Perfume; Van Toller, S., Dodd, G.H., Eds.; Chapman & Hall: London, UK, 1992; pp. 229–242. [Google Scholar]
  30. Société française des parfumeurs. Classification des Parfums et Terminologie; Comité Français du Parfum: Paris, 1998. Available online: http://www-crnl.univ-lyon1.fr/documentation/olfaction/classification_des_parfums/sommaire.htm (accessed on 1 July 2012).
  31. Zarzo, M. Hedonic judgments of chemical compounds are correlated with molecular size. Sensors 2011, 11, 3667–3686. [Google Scholar]
  32. Yokoyama, K.; Ebisawa, F. Detection and evaluation of fragrances by human reactions using a chemical sensor based on adsorbate detection. Anal. Chem. 1993, 65, 673–677. [Google Scholar]
  33. Stevenson, R.J.; Boakes, R.A. A mnemonic theory of odor perception. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 110, 340–364. [Google Scholar]
  34. Wilson, D.A.; Stevenson, R.J. Learning to Smell: Olfactory Perception from Neurobiology to Behavior, 1st ed.; Johns Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  35. Ayabe-Kanamura, S.; Schicker, I.; Laska, M.; Hudson, R.; Distel, H; Kobayakawa, T.; Saito, S. Differences in perception of everyday odors: A Japanese-German cross-cultural study. Chem. Senses 1998, 23, 31–38. [Google Scholar]
  36. Chrea, C.; Valentin, D.; Sulmont-Rossé, C.; Mai, H.L.; Nguyen, D.H.; Abdi, H. Culture and odor categorization: agreement between cultures depends upon the odors. Food Qual. Prefer. 2004, 15, 669–679. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kirk-Smith, M.D.; Booth, D.A. Chemoreception in human behavior: Experimental analysis of the social effects of fragrances. Chem. Senses 1987, 12, 159–166. [Google Scholar]
  38. Schaal, B.; Marlier, L.; Soussignan, R. Olfactory function in the human fetus: Evidence from selective neonatal responsiveness to the odor of amniotic fluid. Behav. Neurosci. 1998, 112, 1438–1449. [Google Scholar]
  39. Mennella, J.A.; Jagnow, C.P.; Beauchamp, G.K. Prenatal and postnatal flavor learning by human infants. Pediatrics 2001, 107, e88. [Google Scholar]
  40. Wallace, K.J.; Rosen, J.B. Predator odor as an unconditioned fear stimulus in rats: Elicitation of freezing by trimethylthiazoline, a component of fox feces. Behav. Neurosci. 2000, 114, 912–922. [Google Scholar]
  41. Rossiter, K.J. Structure-odor relationships. Chem. Rev. 1996, 96, 3201–3240. [Google Scholar]
  42. Ferdenzi, C.; Schirmer, A.; Roberts, S.C.; Delplanque, S.; Porcherot, C.; Cayeux, I.; Velazco, M.I.; Sander, D.; Scherer, K.R.; Grandjean, D. Affective dimensions of odor perception: A comparison between Swiss, British, and Singaporean populations. Emotion 2011, 11, 1168–1181. [Google Scholar]
  43. Kobayakawa, K.; Kobayakawa, R.; Matsumoto, H.; Oka, Y.; Imai, T.; Ikawa, M.; Okabe, M.; Ikeda, T.; Itohara, S.; Kikusui, T.; Mori, K.; Sakano, H. Innate versus learned odor processing in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature 2007, 450, 503–508. [Google Scholar]
  44. Collins Dictionary & Thesaurus of the English Language, 5th ed.; HarperCollins Publishers: Glasgow, UK, 2011.
  45. Chrea, C.; Grandjean, D.; Delplanque, S.; Cayeux, I.; Le Calvé, B.; Aymard, L.; Velazco, M.I.; Sander, D.; Scherer, K.R. Mapping the semantic space for the subjective experience of emotional responses to odors. Chem. Senses 2009, 34, 49–62. [Google Scholar]
  46. Tisserand, R. Essential Oils as Psychotherapeutic Agents. In Perfumery: The Psychology and Biology of Fragrance; Van Toller, S., Dodd, G.H., Eds.; Chapman & Hall: London, UK, 1988; pp. 167–181. [Google Scholar]
  47. Bain, A. The Senses and the Intellect; Parker: London, UK, 1855. [Google Scholar]
  48. Prost, C.; Le Guen, S.; Courcoux, P.; Demaimay, M. Similarities among 40 pure odorant compounds evaluated by consumers. J. Sens. Stud. 2001, 16, 551–565. [Google Scholar]
  49. Cambridge Learner's Dictionary, 2nd ed.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2004.
  50. Schiffman, S.; Robinson, D.E.; Erickson, R.P. Multidimensional scaling of odorants: Examination of psychological and physicochemical dimensions. Chem. Senses 1977, 2, 375–390. [Google Scholar]
  51. Dictionnaire de l'Académie Française, 9th ed.; Académie Française: Paris, France, 1992.
  52. Diccionario de la Lengua Espanola, 21st ed.; Real Academia Espanola: Madrid, Spain, 1992.
  53. Harper, R. Some chemicals representing particular odour qualities. Chem. Sens. Flav. 1975, 1, 353–357. [Google Scholar]
  54. Stoddart, D.M. The role of olfaction in the evolution of human sexual biology: An hypothesis. Man 1986, 21, 514–520. [Google Scholar]
  55. Blumenthal, P. Geruchsbezeichnungen in der französischen werbesprache. Vox Romanica 1979, 38, 155–173. [Google Scholar]
  56. Jellinek, J.S. A Consumer Oriented Classification of Perfumes; Dragoco: Totowa, NJ, USA, 1990; Volume 37, pp. 16–29. [Google Scholar]
  57. Ou, L.C.; Luo, M.R.; Sun, P.L.; Hu, N.C.; Chen, H.S.; Guan, S.S.; Woodcock, A.; Caivano, J.L.; Huertas, R.; Treméau, A.; Billger, M.; Izadan, H.; Richter, K. A cross-cultural comparison of colour emotion for two-colour combinations. Color Res. Appl. 2012, 37, 23–43. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Loading plot (p2vs. p1) of the database obtained by Boelens & Haring [12] (white triangles). Data were mean-centered and scaled to unit variance prior to the PCA. Thiboud's fragrance map [1] is superimposed (filled diamonds; labels in italics) after being properly scaled and rotated (DIM1 and DIM2 stand for dimension 1 and 2, respectively, in the original publication). Dotted lines group the floral descriptors. Equivalent or related odor attributes located close to each other are joined with dashed lines.
Figure 1. Loading plot (p2vs. p1) of the database obtained by Boelens & Haring [12] (white triangles). Data were mean-centered and scaled to unit variance prior to the PCA. Thiboud's fragrance map [1] is superimposed (filled diamonds; labels in italics) after being properly scaled and rotated (DIM1 and DIM2 stand for dimension 1 and 2, respectively, in the original publication). Dotted lines group the floral descriptors. Equivalent or related odor attributes located close to each other are joined with dashed lines.
Sensors 13 00463f1
Figure 2. Plot of fitted regression analysis of p[1] (projections on the first factorial axis) from the Firmenich database analyzed by Chastrette et al.[13]vs. p1 of the B-H database. Empty diamonds correspond to pairs of similar descriptors (e.g., ‘acidic—sourish’ are assumed to be equivalent attributes in the Firmenich and the B-H databases, respectively).
Figure 2. Plot of fitted regression analysis of p[1] (projections on the first factorial axis) from the Firmenich database analyzed by Chastrette et al.[13]vs. p1 of the B-H database. Empty diamonds correspond to pairs of similar descriptors (e.g., ‘acidic—sourish’ are assumed to be equivalent attributes in the Firmenich and the B-H databases, respectively).
Sensors 13 00463f2
Figure 3. (left) Olfactory representation adapted from the Rosace of Firmenich [14]. (right) Projections of dots over the solid line (Sfreshness) are compared with p1 loadings of equivalent attributes in the B-H database.
Figure 3. (left) Olfactory representation adapted from the Rosace of Firmenich [14]. (right) Projections of dots over the solid line (Sfreshness) are compared with p1 loadings of equivalent attributes in the B-H database.
Sensors 13 00463f3
Figure 4. Plot of fitted regression analysis of αJaubert (angular coordinate of odor classes in the semicircular Field of Odors [15], measured in degrees) vs. p1 loadings of equivalent descriptors in the B-H database.
Figure 4. Plot of fitted regression analysis of αJaubert (angular coordinate of odor classes in the semicircular Field of Odors [15], measured in degrees) vs. p1 loadings of equivalent descriptors in the B-H database.
Sensors 13 00463f4
Figure 5. Relationship between fresh odor character and substantivity of reference materials used by Boelens and Haring [12]: scatterplot of p1 (loadings in the formation of PC1) vs. antierogenic index (AI) and substantivity parameters: SI, SGS and EI (values in Table 2). AI, SGS and EI were conveniently transformed as indicated in the legend to range approximately on a 0–100 scale. The fitted regression line (r = −0.81) was obtained after discarding 8 outliers (filled points, highlighted in bold in Table 2). Dashed lines: prediction limits with a confidence level of 95%.
Figure 5. Relationship between fresh odor character and substantivity of reference materials used by Boelens and Haring [12]: scatterplot of p1 (loadings in the formation of PC1) vs. antierogenic index (AI) and substantivity parameters: SI, SGS and EI (values in Table 2). AI, SGS and EI were conveniently transformed as indicated in the legend to range approximately on a 0–100 scale. The fitted regression line (r = −0.81) was obtained after discarding 8 outliers (filled points, highlighted in bold in Table 2). Dashed lines: prediction limits with a confidence level of 95%.
Sensors 13 00463f5
Table 1. Correspondence between attributes/reference materials in the B-H database and descriptors in the H&R Fragrance Guide [17]. A substantivity index (SI) is calculated according to Equation (1) based on the frequency of occurrence of odor descriptors used to describe the top (NT), middle (NM) and base note (NB) of 820 commercial perfumes.
Table 1. Correspondence between attributes/reference materials in the B-H database and descriptors in the H&R Fragrance Guide [17]. A substantivity index (SI) is calculated according to Equation (1) based on the frequency of occurrence of odor descriptors used to describe the top (NT), middle (NM) and base note (NB) of 820 commercial perfumes.
Attribute ap1H&R Fragrance GuideAttribute ap1H&R Fragrance Guide


DescriptorNTNMNBSIDescriptorNTNMNBSI
bergamot oil0.341bergamot722000floral−0.010floral866814748
fresh0.341fresh5766386honey−0.064honey3503869
green0.279green2403517anisic−0.081anise501109
watery0.260watery010cedarwood oil−0.124cedarwood015936485
lemon oil0.189lemon401100woody−0.124woody316630582
citrusy0.189citrusy57000civet absolute−0.142civet00199100
galbanum res.0.180galbanum113703animal−0.142castoreum0083100
tart (dry)0.180dry1963038smoky−0.144leathery01104100
aldehyde0.177aldehydic140000amber.+costuse−0.161ambery00137100
bay oil0.165bay511034erogenic−0.161sensual0070100
lavender oil0.128lavender2042606eugenol−0.182clove f026050
clary sage oil0.126clary sage8362021spicy−0.182spicy163244130
vegetable0.126herbaceous165723patchouli oil−0.197patchouli013625383
fruity0.085fruity1512438vanillin−0.221vanilla00301100
fruity0.085peach b149702olibanum res.−0.249olibanum0311799
fir needle oil0.061fir c0244182balsamic−0.249balsamic0076100
coniferous0.061pine c187049sweet−0.280sweet05015788
peppermint oil0.045peppermint d29203sweet−0.280tonka g01287100
oakmoss res.−0.008oakmoss00139100musk+coumarin h−0.323musk00698100
earthy−0.008mossy00252100powdery−0.323powdery00376100
jasmine absolute−0.010jasmine0671050powdery−0.323warm00137100
aReference materials (in italics) and descriptors are listed by decreasing order of p1 (loadings in the formation of the first principal component). The term ‘res.’ stands for resinoid. The correspondence between descriptors and references used by Boelens & Haring [12] is indicated in Table 2.
bPeach is the fruity descriptor most frequently encountered in the H&R guide.
cPine and fir trees are conifers and their essential oil smells alike.
dPeppermint or spearmint.
eMixture of ambergris and costus oil.
fClove oil contains >85% of eugenol [25].
gTonka is the second attribute (after vanilla) most frequently associated to sweet in the H&R guide.
hMixture of musk ketone and coumarin.
Table 2. Substantivity (SI: substantivity index from Table 1; SGS: substantivity value from www.thegoodscentscompany.com; EI: evaporation index [7]) and fresh odor character (p1: loadings in the formation of PC1; AI: antierogenic index [4]) of reference materials used by Boelens & Haring [12]. Values in bold appear as outliers in Figure 5.
Table 2. Substantivity (SI: substantivity index from Table 1; SGS: substantivity value from www.thegoodscentscompany.com; EI: evaporation index [7]) and fresh odor character (p1: loadings in the formation of PC1; AI: antierogenic index [4]) of reference materials used by Boelens & Haring [12]. Values in bold appear as outliers in Figure 5.
Reference materialAttributeFreshnessSubstantivity


p1AISISGSEI
Bergamot oilFresh0.341230450
Methyl 2-octynoateGreen0.2791007240
Styrallyl acetateSourish0.2771008
Cyclamen aldehydeWatery0.26010072
Lemon oilCitrusy0.1898304800 d
Galbanum resinoidTart (dry)0.180100345
Aldehyde C-10Aldehyde0.17750036
10-undecen-1-olFatty0.16660152
Bay oilMetallic0.165−2034364140
Lavender oilLavender0.12850612410
Clary sage oilVegetable0.12672136180
HexadecanalFruity0.085−1008
Fir needle oilConiferous0.0611008224580
Peppermint oilMinty0.045100 a316300
Oakmoss resinoidEarthy−0.008−35100400
Jasmine absoluteFloral−0.010−1005028050
Methyl salicylateMedicinal−0.052−158
Ethyl phenylacetateHoney−0.06469312
Fennel oilAnisic−0.081924150
Cedarwood oilWoody−0.124108538830
Civet absoluteAnimal−0.142−100100400
Cade oilSmoky−0.144−80 b10040010
Ambergris + costus oilErogenic−0.161−100100316c
EugenolSpicy−0.182−100505220 e
Patchouli oilDusty−0.197−100834004
VanillinAromatic−0.221−100100400
Olibanum resinoidBalsamic−0.249−10099284
HeliotropinSweet−0.280−2388212
Musk ketone+coumarinPowdery−0.323−60100400
aValue of spearmint (both plants are botanically related).
bValue of birch tar oil (both smell smoky and are obtained by destructive distillation of wood).
cValue of costus oil (SGS of ambergris is not available).
dValue of lime oil.
eValue of clove oil.
Table 3. Substantivity parameters and fresh odor character of the 44 natural materials contained in the semantic odor profile database compiled by Thiboud [1].
Table 3. Substantivity parameters and fresh odor character of the 44 natural materials contained in the semantic odor profile database compiled by Thiboud [1].
Material aSubstantivityOdor Character Descriptors fFreshness



CS bSI cSGSdEI eIfreshIcitrusIagrestIfloralIbalsamIorientalSfreshgAI h
Petitgrain bergamot oil34281702211006100
Bergamot oil60450221000523
Jonquil absolute242012005−100
Lime oil2020800220000480
Mandarin oil2482200004−27
Coriander oil31382002020004−5
Grapefruit oil62642200004
Thyme oil7391722202020004100
Lemon oil804220000483
Orange Florida oil111140970220000410
Mimosa absolute144000022004−30
Mugwort oil94161020003
Chamomile oil, Roman101125301020003−100
Wormwood oil (absinthe)102125000021003
Violet leaf absolute18234002010020030
Cumin oil470020002−40
Myrtle oil44000022202
Rose oil (Bulgarian)850168101001002−80
Galbanum oil113725401010002100
Carrot seed oil1196800020002−60
Elemi oil13207202110202100

Clary sage oil20213618000200027
Geranium Bourbon oil2950281201001002−70
Tuberose absolute43503040002002−100
Cardamom oil3016320002001175
Rose absolute (French)43501680001001−90
Cedarwood oil Virginia88538830001000110
Narcissus absolute11494000002101
Nutmeg oil11525500010010−40
Ginger729215000000000
Copaiba64000002200
Clove bud oil2250188200001010−100
Cinnamon leaf oil2256304300010010

Vetiver oil Bourbon100864000000000100
Patchouli oil1008340040010010−100
Cistus oil (labdanum)1001004000020200−80
Styrax100984000002200−100
Pepper (black) oil10048400000001−1
Opoponax9010040020010021−210
Allspice (pimento berry)10040050000020−2−40
Sandalwood oil10088400000021−3−63
Peru100400000021−3−100
Benjoin100100400000021−3−100
Tolu100100400000021−3−100
aSorted by decreasing value of Sfresh.
bCoefficient of substantivity [18].
cSubstantivity index deduced from the H&R Fragrance Guide according to Equation (1).
dSubstantivity (hours) according to www.thegoodscentscompany.com.
eEvaporation index [7].
fIndicator variables corresponding to 6 relevant descriptors. The value 2 indicates a main descriptor in the semantic odor profile, and the value 1 indicates an ancillary descriptor.
gScores of freshness calculated as: Ifresh + Icitrus + Iagrestic + Ifloral − Ibalsamic − Ioriental.
hAntierogenic index [4].
Table 4. Substantivity parameters and antierogenic index (AI) of materials not included in Table 3 (sorted approximately by increasing order of substantivity).
Table 4. Substantivity parameters and antierogenic index (AI) of materials not included in Table 3 (sorted approximately by increasing order of substantivity).
MaterialSubstantivity aAIMaterialSubstantivity aAI


CSSISGSEICSSISGSEI
Lavender oil461241050Cascarilla oil292482000
Neroli oil5411621017Orange flower absolute3115400−50
Laurel leaf oil9750Cinnamon bark oil2456372120−100
Peppermint oil8316300100Lily of the valley; orchid50
Rosewood oil21612250−8Carnation absolute50−30
Hyacinth absolute111640Ylang-ylang oil325014090−80
Basil oil146225−50Ambrette seed oil3012120−100
Palmarosa oil1460500Orris resinoid40 b50−60
Juniper berry oil18700Jasmine absolute435028050−100
Gardenia14Heliotrope70
Rosemary oil2154820100Civet absolute79100400−100
Marjoram oil1812600Tonka resinoid100100400−55
Verbena resinoid190Castoreum absolute100100400−100
aCS: coefficient of subst.; SI: subst. index; SGS: subst. from www.thegoodscentscompany.com; EI: evaporation index.
bAverage CS of orris concrete, orris absolute and orris oleo-resin.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zarzo, M. What is a Fresh Scent in Perfumery? Perceptual Freshness is Correlated with Substantivity. Sensors 2013, 13, 463-483. https://doi.org/10.3390/s130100463

AMA Style

Zarzo M. What is a Fresh Scent in Perfumery? Perceptual Freshness is Correlated with Substantivity. Sensors. 2013; 13(1):463-483. https://doi.org/10.3390/s130100463

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zarzo, Manuel. 2013. "What is a Fresh Scent in Perfumery? Perceptual Freshness is Correlated with Substantivity" Sensors 13, no. 1: 463-483. https://doi.org/10.3390/s130100463

APA Style

Zarzo, M. (2013). What is a Fresh Scent in Perfumery? Perceptual Freshness is Correlated with Substantivity. Sensors, 13(1), 463-483. https://doi.org/10.3390/s130100463

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop