Next Article in Journal
Outcomes in COVID-19 Patients with Acute Cholangitis: A Single-Center Retrospective Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Atypical Plasma Cell Leukemia Mistaken for Acute Leukemia: A Case Report
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Cognitive Outcomes in Young Adults with Primary Arterial Hypertension: The Role of Cardiovascular Risk Factors and Hypertension-Mediated Organ Damage

by
Kristijonas Puteikis
1,2,
Karolis Ažukaitis
1,2,
Danguolė Dadurkevičienė
2,
Dovilė Mikučionytė
2,
Kazys Simanauskas
1,2,
Vaida Šileikienė
2,
Rytis Masiliūnas
1,2,
Augustina Jankauskienė
1,2 and
Rūta Mameniškienė
1,2,*
1
Faculty of Medicine, Vilnius University, 03101 Vilnius, Lithuania
2
Vilnius University Hospital Santaros Klinikos, 08406 Vilnius, Lithuania
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Medicina 2024, 60(8), 1353; https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina60081353
Submission received: 23 July 2024 / Revised: 14 August 2024 / Accepted: 17 August 2024 / Published: 20 August 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Neurology)

Abstract

:
Background and Objectives: We aimed to explore the association between cognitive performance and markers of hypertension-mediated organ damage (HMOD) in young adults with hypertension. Materials and Methods: A group of adults aged 16 to 45 years with primary arterial hypertension completed a battery of paper-pencil as well as computer-based neuropsychological tests across all major cognitive domains. They also underwent office and ambulatory 24 h blood pressure, intima-media thickness measurements, heart ultrasound, and laboratory analysis of their lipid profile, blood uric acid concentration, and urine albumin-creatinine ratio. Associations between cognitive test results and markers of HMOD were explored through correlation analysis and age-, sex-, and body mass index-adjusted linear regression modeling. Results: Seventy-six individuals (62, 81.6% male) aged 36.5 years (interquartile range 18.4 to 42.0 years) were enrolled. After adjusting for age, sex, and body mass index in linear regression models, worse categorical fluency was associated with higher left ventricular mass (βst = −0.264, p = 0.043) and worse performance in a task of sustained attention—with higher left ventricular mass index (βst = −0. 304, p = 0.016). Worse phonemic fluency was related to higher pulse pressure (βst = −0.241, p = 0.049) in the respective model. Better strategy use in the task of spatial working memory was linked to higher daytime mean diastolic blood pressure (βst = −0.343 p = 0.017). Conclusions: Performance among young adults with hypertension across selected cognitive domains was inversely associated with pulse pressure, markers of left ventricular damage, and directly associated with daytime diastolic blood pressure. Our study suggests that the previously reported relationship between cognitive and cardiovascular markers in hypertension exists earlier than in middle or late adulthood.

1. Introduction

Hypertension is a widespread and burdensome condition that greatly increases cardiovascular morbidity and mortality, making it one of the global health priorities [1]. While the more serious sequelae of poorly managed hypertension are numerous and include potentially lethal events, such as heart attacks or stroke, the risk of such outcomes can be mitigated by addressing the impact of hypertension on the cardiovascular and renal systems through early detection of subclinical hypertension-mediated organ damage (HMOD). The 2023 European Society of Hypertension (ESH) Guidelines for the management of arterial hypertension suggest specific clinical criteria for defining hypertension-mediated damage to the heart, peripheral arteries, eyes, and kidneys [2].
While the brain is acknowledged as another important target organ that is susceptible to damage induced by hypertension, no robust biomarkers have yet been suggested to define the presence of such injury. The Mini Mental State Examination and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment are proposed as routine screening methods in this context, but they are not specific to HMOD and, due to their low capability to detect subtle alterations, are applicable only in older populations [2,3]. Despite the known societal and economic importance of preserving brain health throughout the lifespan [4,5], cognitive outcomes in relation to hypertension have been understudied and have not undergone rigorous harmonization procedures characteristic of laboratory or imaging methods used to assess HMOD in the cardiovascular or renal systems. This is especially true in younger populations that are not yet at risk of dementia but have nevertheless been posited to experience negative effects of early hypertension on cognitive health in middle adulthood—the most robust evidence supporting this notion stems from the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study [6,7] as well as the Young Finns Study [8].
Even considering the studies that suggest negative alterations in cognitive functioning among young adults with hypertension, the mechanisms of the observed changes have not been fully elucidated. It is well-known that primary hypertension is a condition extending well beyond the isolated elevation of blood pressure (BP). The accumulation of different cardiovascular risk factors (CVRF) may exert independent effects on neurocognitive outcomes. In addition, prior studies have shown an overlap of HMOD patterns in patients with hypertension, including structural and functional measures of cardiac and arterial injuries. Whether cognitive outcomes are associated with well-known HMOD markers and, thus, may share common pathways has not been extensively studied, particularly in younger adults with hypertension, where multimorbidity is less pronounced. Using a neuropsychological battery combining paper-pencil and computerized testing, we attempted to address the existing gap in the literature by exploring the association between CVRF, markers of HMOD, and cognitive performance in a group of young adults with hypertension.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Setting and Participants

We performed a cross-sectional study at Vilnius University Hospital Santaros Klinikos between 1 June 2021, and 30 June 2023. A convenience sample of young adults with primary arterial hypertension was formed from patients who had arrived for a routine medical consultation at the Centre for Family Medicine or the pediatric cardiology outpatient clinic.
Participant inclusion criteria were: (i) young adults (age 16–45 years), (ii) Lithuanian speakers, (iii) primary arterial hypertension (treated or non-treated) as defined by the European Society of Cardiology (ESC) and the European Society of Hypertension (ESH) guidelines of 2018 and 2016 (for those <18 years of age) [9,10]. Exclusion criteria were: (i) significant deficits (sensory or motor) preventing completion of cognitive evaluation, (ii) cardiovascular comorbidities besides primary arterial hypertension (e.g., congenital heart disease related to hypoxia or hypoperfusion, heart rhythm or conduction disorders with hemodynamic significance), (iii) diabetes mellitus, and (iv) glomerular filtration rate < 60 mL/min/1.73 m2 upon enrollment.

2.2. Cognitive Evaluation

The neuropsychological test battery consisted of paper-pencil and computerized (Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) by Cambridge Cognition, Ltd., Cambridge, UK) tests.
The testing battery included the following:
  • Categorical verbal fluency (count variable of different animals listed in 60 s).
  • Phonemic verbal fluency (number of nouns starting with “P” listed in 60 s).
  • Verbal-logical story recall (VLS, verbal memory). Participants listened to a short verbal story and were asked to recall it immediately, after 30 min, and after 24 h. The story was scored on a 24-point scale for each item recited.
  • Intelligence quotient (IQ) measured by the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-II.
  • Nonverbal CANTAB tests (described in detail in Supplementary Table S1):
    (1)
    Match to Sample Visual Search (MTS): attention and processing speed.
    (2)
    Paired Associates Learning (PAL): visual memory and new learning.
    (3)
    Reaction Time Task (RTI): motor and mental response speed, movement time, reaction time, response accuracy, and impulsivity.
    (4)
    Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVP): sustained attention.
    (5)
    Stockings of Cambridge (SOC): spatial planning, working memory.
    (6)
    Spatial Span (SSP): working memory.
    (7)
    Spatial Working Memory (SWM): working memory and strategy use.
All tasks were performed by a licensed clinical psychologist in a single session conducted before ambulatory blood pressure monitoring (ABPM) measurement, except for IQ testing and 24-h VLS testing, which were performed upon taking off the ABPM cuff the second day. CANTAB tests were performed in accordance with the guidelines supplied by Cambridge Cognition, Ltd. The combined time required for neuropsychological evaluation was around 2 h for each participant. Situational factors, such as stress and fatigue, were mitigated through patient education and the use of an optimized examination protocol.

2.3. Cardiovascular Evaluation

Participants were measured for weight and height and underwent office BP (oBP) and ABPM measurements using validated oscillometric devices (ABPM: Mobile-O-Graph monitor, IEM, Aachen, Germany (18+ years), OnTrak Intertek 315762 device, Spacelabs Healthcare, Snoqualmie, Washington, United States (16–17 years)) in accordance with ESC/ESH guidelines, including recommendations for measurement conditions and posture [9,10]. The mean systolic (SBP) and diastolic (DBP) office BP values were calculated as the mean of the second and third measurements. Echocardiography was performed by an experienced cardiologist for all patients (Vivid 7 Ultrasound System, GE Healthcare, Chicago, Illinois, United States (18+ years), EPIQ diagnostic ultrasound system USN20B0120, Philips, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (16–17 years)). The left ventricular mass index (LVMI) was calculated as LVM/height (g/m2.7) according to ESH guidelines [2]. Intima-media thickness (IMT) was measured for all patients on both sides of the common carotid artery (the values were then averaged to yield a single measure) according to the Mannheim protocol (MyLab™X7, ESAOTE, Genoa, Italy (18+ years), EPIQ diagnostic ultrasound system USN20B0120 automated edge detection, Philips, Amsterdam, the Netherlands (16–17 years)) [11]. Patients provided a fasting blood sample to test for creatinine, uric acid, total cholesterol, high-density lipoprotein (HDL), low-density lipoprotein (LDL), and triglyceride concentrations. A morning urine sample was collected to measure the urine albumin-creatinine ratio (uACR).
LVH was defined as LVMI ≥ 50 g/m2.7 and ≥47 g/m2.7 for males and females, respectively, while LV concentric remodeling was defined as RWT ≥ 0.43 [2]. Mean IMT values > 0.9 mm were used to define the presence of atherosclerosis. Urine ACR values > 30 mg/g (>3.39 mg/mmol) were used for albuminuria, according to the ESH guidelines [2]. BMI values equal to or higher than 25 or 30 g/m2 were used to classify patients as overweight and obese, respectively.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

The normality of continuous variables was assessed using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Correlation coefficients (Spearman’s rho) were calculated to determine the association between the cognitive and cardiovascular measures. All cardiovascular variables having statistically significant correlations with a single cognitive measure (the dependent variable) were then included as independent variables in a stepwise regression model, alongside age, sex, and body mass index (BMI). Cardiovascular variable(s) found to be statistically significant in the stepwise model were then entered into a simple regression model adjusted for age, sex, and BMI.
All statistical tests were two-tailed, and the level of significance was set at p < 0.05. All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS v26. A sample size of n = 77 was sought to yield a power of 0.8, α = 0.05 for a linear multiple regression model with three predictors and an effect size of f2 = 0.15 (G*Power 3.1.9.7).

2.5. Ethics

The study was approved by the Vilnius Regional Biomedical Research Ethics Committee (approval no. 2021/5-1348-821) and conducted in accordance with the principles of the World Medical Association Helsinki Declaration as well as national regulations. All participants (and parents or legal guardians, in the case of participants who were <18 years old) provided written informed consent upon enrollment.

3. Results

3.1. General Results

The study sample that completed the neuropsychological assessment consisted of 76 individuals (62, 81.6% male) aged 36.5 years (interquartile range 18.4 to 42 years). Forty-six (60.5%) of them had completed tertiary and 10 (13.2%) secondary education, while the remaining participants were either high school students or had lower levels of education. The results of neuropsychological testing are presented in Table 1.
Considering the participants’ cardiovascular profile, 50 (65.8%) were undergoing pharmacological treatment for hypertension: 28 (56.0%) with antihypertensive medication from one, 18 (36.0%)—from two, 3 (6.0%)—from three and 1 (2.0%)—from five different classes of drugs. There were 31 (40.8%) and 28 (36.8%) overweight and obese participants, respectively. Five (6.6%) patients had LV hypertrophy (LVH), seventeen (22.4%) had LV concentric remodeling, six (7.9%)—carotid atherosclerosis, and one (1.3%)—albuminuria. Most patients had an LDL-cholesterol blood concentration of >1.8 mmol/L (18, 23.7%) or >2.6 mmol/L (53, 69.7%). Fourteen (18.4%) participants were smokers. Anthropometric, laboratory, and BP measurement data of the participants are presented in Table 2.

3.2. Correlation Data

A correlation heatmap between cognitive and cardiovascular measures is presented in Supplementary Figure S1.

3.3. Regression Analyses Based on Results from Verbal Fluency, Verbal Memory and Intelligence Tests

In a stepwise regression model, worse categorical verbal fluency was associated with higher LVM (βst = −0.306, p = 0.009), while age, sex, BMI, LVMI, and RWT were excluded from the model (R2 = 0.081). In a simple model adjusted for age, sex, and BMI, LVM remained a statistically significant variable (βst = −0.264, p = 0.043; R2 = 0.061).
In a stepwise model (R2 = 0.192), worse phonemic fluency was associated with higher pulse pressure (PP, βst = −0.362, p = 0.003) and uric acid concentration (βst = −0.276, p = 0.020). Age (βst = 0.277, p = 0.039) and PP (βst = −0.241, p = 0.049, Figure 1A) remained significant variables in a simple regression model adjusted for age, sex, and BMI (R2 = 0.192).
While the 30 min verbal story recall was related to nighttime mean arterial pressure (βst = 0.262, p = 0.028, R2 = 0.055) in a stepwise model, this relationship was no longer present after adjustment.
Immediate story recall (βst = 0.361, p = 0.002, R2 = 0.117), verbal intelligence (βst = 0.328, p = 0.013, R2 = 0.092), and general intelligence were associated only with age (βst = 0.429, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.172).

3.4. Regression Analyses Based on Results from Computerized Cognitive Assessment

Despite the detected correlations between tests of attention and processing speed (MTS), visual memory and new learning (PAL), and response speed (RTI) with IMT (Supplementary Figure S1), such relationships were no longer statistically significant (p > 0.05) after adjustment for sex, age, and BMI in regression modeling.
A measure of sustained attention (RVPA) was inversely correlated with LVMI in a stepwise model (βst = −0.335, p = 0.008, R2 = 0.097, p = 0.008), Figure 1B. This result persisted in a simple regression adjusted for sex, age, and BMI (βst = −0.304, p = 0.016, R2 = 0.089). Finally, a higher daytime mean DBP was related to better performance (higher strategy use) in a spatial working memory task (SWMS, βst = −0.343 p = 0.017, R2 = 0.081), Figure 1C.

4. Discussion

The aim of the current study was to explore how cognitive performance is related to cardiovascular profile and HMOD markers among young adults with primary arterial hypertension. We found statistically significant inverse associations between two cognitive domains—categorical verbal fluency and sustained attention—and LV parameters. Previous research has already shown that individuals with LV dysfunction tend to be at risk of white matter lesions, worse attention, executive performance, and global cognitive impairment, suggesting long-term hypertension exposure as a probable underlying factor for these findings [12,13,14]. On the other hand, recent reports revealed an independent relationship between brain imaging markers and LV parameters, thus questioning the role of hypertension alone as a causative factor of both cardiac and brain damage—a broader dysfunction of hormonal or stress responses (e.g., of the renin-aldosterone-angiotensin system) may be even more relevant [15,16]. It is essential to consider that the neuropsychological and imaging outcomes in earlier studies were evaluated in older populations, most often aged 65 years or more. While this helps to establish the potential relationship between left ventricle parameters and the risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia later in life [17], it remains possible that the link between markers of subtle heart and brain damage could be detected even in young adults.
Evidence from the Bogalusa Heart Study revealed that, in individuals aged 48.4 ± 5.1 years, a fifth of the relationship between systolic blood pressure burden and cognition can be explained by a higher LVMI [18]. The authors suggested that LVMI may, therefore, be a central factor mediating the negative influence of hypertension on cognitive outcomes later in life. The notion of an independent association between cognitive functioning and cardiac damage is further supported by studies with longitudinal follow-up in middle-aged individuals [13,19,20]. Our findings extend previous research by providing evidence of an inverse association between categorical verbal fluency, sustained attention, and LV mass parameters in very young adults with hypertension. In accordance with the studies mentioned above, this relationship appears to be independent of blood pressure or other cardiovascular risk factors. However, LVM or LVMI explained only 6.7–8.9% of the variance in the results of the cognitive tests used. Therefore, this relationship, despite being statistically significant, can be expected to reflect only a part of the complex factors underlying variability in the two cognitive domains.
We also identified an association between low phonemic fluency and high PP, with PP accounting for 19.2% of the variance in this cognitive task. Similar to cardiac damage estimated through LV function, higher PP has been reported to be related to worsening in a broad spectrum of cognitive domains after a follow-up of middle-aged adults of several years [21]. In older individuals, higher PP is also associated with worse cognitive functioning (including mild cognitive impairment) [22], the level of Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers [23], white matter lesions [24], and reduced white matter neuronal fiber integrity [25]. The pathways of PP-associated damage to nervous tissue are thought to include disruption in cerebral blood flow autoregulation, resulting in microhemorrhages as well as endothelial dysfunction, oxidative stress, and inflammation [26,27,28]. While the negative influence of high PP has been emphasized in mid- and later life, our results suggest that it may be related to worse verbal fluency even in younger adults.
Another finding of our study was that high strategy use in a task, which is centered on finding hidden tokens, correlated with higher mean daytime DBP. As both direct [29,30] and inverse associations [31,32] between DBP and brain health have been identified in the literature, the role of DBP in neurocognition is now considered to be highly dependent on contextual factors, such as age, sex, and PP [29]. For example, better cognitive outcomes may be associated with higher DBP only if SBP is not elevated, and thus PP is low [32]. While it remains difficult to interpret why daytime DBP could be higher in those displaying better strategy use (possible explanations could include that higher DBP reflects lower arterial stiffness or better perfusion to eloquent brain regions), it should be noted that no other BP measures were statistically significant predictors of the tested cognitive outcomes in the regression models.
The results presented should be interpreted in light of the various limitations of our study. First, because of the time-consuming and extensive neuropsychological battery used, the sample size was small, providing limited statistical power to detect subtle associations between cognitive and cardiovascular variables or to create robust regression models containing more than several independent variables. It, therefore, remains unknown whether there are any additional associations between markers of HMOD or cardiovascular risk and cognitive functioning that remained undetected in the current study. On the other hand, the exploratory nature of the study and the inclusion of a comprehensive neuropsychological battery inflated the risk of Type I error across the spectrum of our analyses. The fact that most individuals included in the study were already treated for hypertension should also be considered, as this factor may lead to a blunted effect size when exploring the association between cognitive and BP variables. While the sample was predominantly male, this is most likely representative of population trends, with young men being significantly more affected by primary hypertension [33]. A more diverse patient group, from the perspective of ethnicity and socioeconomic background, is necessary to increase the generalizability of our findings. Furthermore, the characterization of our study sample was not extensive enough to account for societal, psychological, lifestyle, or environmental confounders that may have important associations with cognitive performance in early adulthood. We believe this notion was also reflected by the rather small R-squared values in the presented regression models, indicating that a more comprehensive evaluation of cognitive profiles should be performed by accounting for a broader physiological, demographic, and socioeconomic context. We also did not investigate the association between cognitive function and HMOD in other organ systems (beyond renal and cardiovascular dysfunction) that are also affected by arterial hypertension. For instance, retinal changes have been shown to be among the earliest markers of hypertensive damage to the microvasculature [34] and may, therefore, have a more evident correlation with cognitive outcomes among youth.

5. Conclusions

Our study extends previous research in middle-to-late adulthood by showing that certain cognitive domains, such as verbal fluency, sustained attention, and strategy use, are associated with left ventricle parameters, pulse pressure, and diastolic blood pressure, even in very young adults with hypertension. Further investigation of how these relationships are mediated could help guide clinical decisions aimed at brain health preservation and promotion several decades before the onset of mild cognitive impairment or dementia. Given the potential for a larger time window to address the impact of hypertension on cognitive health in clinical practice, we believe that our results should be replicated in larger young adult studies focused on the association between left ventricular function, blood pressure characteristics, and cognitive performance. Moreover, future studies should consider the inclusion of a more diverse patient population as well as a longitudinal study design to provide generalizable data about the causal interaction between hypertension burden and cognitive functioning among young individuals. Finally, additional data on the pathophysiological interaction between hypertension-induced microvascular changes and brain function are needed from fundamental and translational studies.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/medicina60081353/s1: Figure S1. A heatmap representing correlations (Spearman’s correlation coefficients) between age, cognitive test, and cardiovascular variables. Table S1: Description of computerized cognitive CANTAB (Cambridge Cognition, Ltd.) measures used in the current study.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, K.P., K.A., A.J. and R.M. (Rūta Mameniškienė); methodology, K.P., K.A., A.J. and R.M. (Rūta Mameniškienė); formal analysis, K.P.; investigation, K.A., D.M., A.J., R.M. (Rūta Mameniškienė), D.D., K.S., V.Š. and R.M. (Rytis Masiliūnas); resources, A.J. and R.M. (Rūta Mameniškienė); data curation, K.P.; writing—original draft preparation, K.P., K.A., A.J. and R.M. (Rūta Mameniškienė); writing—review and editing, K.P., K.A., A.J. amd R.M. (Rūta Mameniškienė); visualization, K.P.; supervision, A.J. and R.M. (Rūta Mameniškienė); project administration, A.J. and R.M. (Rūta Mameniškienė); funding acquisition, K.P., K.A., A.J. and R.M. (Rūta Mameniškienė). All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project received funding from the Research Council of Lithuania (LMTLT), agreement No P-MIP-21-127.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by the Vilnius Regional Biomedical Research Ethics Committee (approval no. 2021/5-1348-821 issued on the 25th of May 2021) and conducted in accordance with the principles of the World Medical Association Helsinki Declaration as well as national regulations. All participants (and parents or legal guardians, in the case of participants who were <18 years old) provided written informed consent upon enrollment.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Pseudonymized data supporting the findings will be made available upon reasonable request, supported by a data analysis plan and approval by the Vilnius Regional Biomedical Research Ethics Committee.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study, in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data, in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. World Health Organisation. Global Report on Hypertension. The Race against a Silent Killer; World Health Organisation: Geneva, Switzerland, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  2. Mancia, G.; Kreutz, R.; Brunström, M.; Burnier, M.; Grassi, G.; Januszewicz, A.; Muiesan, M.L.; Tsioufis, K.; Agabiti-Rosei, E.; Algharably, E.A.E.; et al. 2023 ESH Guidelines for the management of arterial hypertension the Task Force for the management of arterial hypertension of the European Society of Hypertension. J. Hypertens. 2023, 41, 1874–2071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Scuteri, A.; Benetos, A.; Sierra, C.; Coca, A.; Chicherio, C.; Frisoni, G.B.; Gasecki, D.; Hering, D.; Lovic, D.; Manios, E.; et al. Routine assessment of cognitive function in older patients with hypertension seen by primary care physicians: Why and how—A decision-making support from the working group on ‘hypertension and the brain’ of the European Society of Hypertension and from the European Geriatric Medicine Society. J. Hypertens. 2021, 39, 90–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bassetti, C.L.A.; Endres, M.; Sander, A.; Crean, M.; Subramaniam, S.; Carvalho, V.; Di Liberto, G.; Franco, O.H.; Pijnenburg, Y.; Leonardi, M.; et al. The European Academy of Neurology Brain Health Strategy: One brain, one life, one approach. Eur. J. Neurol. 2022, 29, 2559–2566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. World Health Organisation. Optimizing Brain Health across the Life Course: WHO Position Paper; World Health Organisation: Geneva, Switzerland, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  6. Yaffe, K.; Vittinghoff, E.; Pletcher, M.J.; Hoang, T.D.; Launer, L.J.; Whitmer, R.A.; Coker, L.H.; Sidney, S. early adult to midlife cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive function. Circulation 2014, 129, 1560–1567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Reis, J.P.; Loria, C.M.; Launer, L.J.; Sidney, S.; Liu, K.; Jacobs, D.R.; Zhu, N.; Lloyd-Jones, D.M.; He, K.; Yaffe, K. Cardiovascular health through young adulthood and cognitive functioning in midlife. Ann. Neurol. 2013, 73, 170–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Hakala, J.O.; Pahkala, K.; Juonala, M.; Salo, P.; Kähönen, M.; Hutri-Kähönen, N.; Lehtimäki, T.; Laitinen, T.P.; Jokinen, E.; Taittonen, L.; et al. Cardiovascular Risk Factor Trajectories since Childhood and Cognitive Performance in Midlife: The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. Circulation 2021, 143, 1949–1961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Williams, B.; Mancia, G.; Spiering, W.; Agabiti Rosei, E.; Azizi, M.; Burnier, M.; Clement, D.L.; Coca, A.; de Simone, G.; Dominiczak, A.; et al. 2018 ESC/ESH Guidelines for the management of arterial hypertension. Eur. Heart J. 2018, 39, 3021–3104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Lurbe, E.; Agabiti-Rosei, E.; Cruickshank, J.K.; Dominiczak, A.; Erdine, S.; Hirth, A.; Invitti, C.; Litwin, M.; Mancia, G.; Pall, D.; et al. 2016 European Society of Hypertension guidelines for the management of high blood pressure in children and adolescents. J. Hypertens. 2016, 34, 1887–1920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Touboul, P.-J.; Hennerici, M.G.; Meairs, S.; Adams, H.; Amarenco, P.; Bornstein, N.; Csiba, L.; Desvarieux, M.; Ebrahim, S.; Hernandez, R.H.; et al. Mannheim carotid intima-media thickness and plaque consensus (2004–2006–2011). Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2012, 34, 290–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Arangalage, D.; Ederhy, S.; Dufour, L.; Joffre, J.; Van der Vynckt, C.; Lang, S.; Tzourio, C.; Cohen, A. Relationship between cognitive impairment and echocardiographic parameters: A review. J. Am. Soc. Echocardiogr. 2015, 28, 264–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Haring, B.; Omidpanah, A.; Suchy-Dicey, A.M.; Best, L.G.; Verney, S.P.; Shibata, D.K.; Cole, S.A.; Ali, T.; Howard, B.V.; Buchwald, D.; et al. Left Ventricular Mass, Brain Magnetic Resonance Imaging, and Cognitive Performance: Results from the Strong Heart Study. Hypertension 2017, 70, 964–971. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Moore, E.E.; Khan, O.A.; Shashikumar, N.; Pechman, K.R.; Liu, D.; Bell, S.P.; Nair, S.; Terry, J.G.; Gifford, K.A.; Anderson, A.W.; et al. Axonal Injury Partially Mediates Associations between Increased Left Ventricular Mass Index and White Matter Damage. Stroke 2022, 29, 808–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Frenzel, S.; Wittfeld, K.; Bülow, R.; Völzke, H.; Friedrich, N.; Habes, M.; Felix, S.B.; Dörr, M.; Grabe, H.J.; Bahls, M. Cardiac hypertrophy is associated with advanced brain aging in the general population. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2021, 10, e020994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Frenzel, S.; Bülow, R.; Dörr, M.; Felix, S.B.; Friedrich, N.; Völzke, H.; Wittfeld, K.; Grabe, H.J.; Bahls, M. Left ventricular hypertrophy as a risk factor for accelerated brain aging: Results from the Study of Health in Pomerania. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2024, 45, e26567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Elias, M.F.; Torres, R.V.; Davey, A. Parameters of Left Ventricular Mass and Dementia Moving the Literature Forward. Hypertension 2018, 71, 411–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Razavi, A.C.; Fernandez, C.; He, J.; Kelly, T.N.; Krousel-Wood, M.; Whelton, S.P.; Carmichael, O.T.; Bazzano, L. Left Ventricular Mass Index Is Associated with Cognitive Function in Middle-Age: Bogalusa Heart Study. Circ. Cardiovasc. Imaging 2020, 13, e010335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Uiterwijk, R.; Staals, J.; Huijts, M.; van Kuijk, S.M.J.; de Leeuw, P.W.; Kroon, A.A.; van Oostenbrugge, R.J. Hypertensive organ damage predicts future cognitive performance: A 9-year follow-up study in patients with hypertension. J. Clin. Hypertens. 2018, 20, 1458–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Rouch, L.; Hoang, T.; Xia, F.; Sidney, S.; Lima, J.A.; Yaffe, K. Twenty-five-year change in cardiac structure and function and midlife cognition. Neurology 2022, 98, E1040–E1049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Waldstein, S.R.; Rice, S.C.; Thayer, J.F.; Najjar, S.S.; Scuteri, A.; Zonderman, A.B. Pulse pressure and pulse wave velocity are related to cognitive decline in the baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Hypertension 2008, 51, 99–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Riba-Llena, I.; Nafría, C.; Filomena, J.; Tovar, J.L.; Vinyoles, E.; Mundet, X.; Jarca, C.I.; Vilar-Bergua, A.; Montaner, J.; Delgado, P. High daytime and nighttime ambulatory pulse pressure predict poor cognitive function and mild cognitive impairment in hypertensive individuals. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 2016, 36, 253–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Nation, D.A.; Edland, S.D.; Bondi, M.W.; Salmon, D.P.; Delano-Wood, L.; Peskind, E.R.; Quinn, J.F.; Galasko, D.R. Pulse pressure is associated with Alzheimer biomarkers in cognitively normal older adults. Neurology 2013, 81, 2024–2027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Zang, J.; Shi, J.; Liang, J.; Zhang, X.; Wei, W.; Yao, C.; Zhuang, X.; Wu, G. Pulse Pressure, Cognition, and White Matter Lesions: A Mediation Analysis. Front. Cardiovasc. Med. 2021, 8, 654522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Tarumi, T.; Thomas, B.P.; Wang, C.; Zhang, L.; Liu, J.; Turner, M.; Riley, J.; Tangella, N.; Womack, K.B.; Kerwin, D.R.; et al. Ambulatory pulse pressure, brain neuronal fiber integrity, and cerebral blood flow in older adults. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 2019, 39, 926–936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Thorin-Trescases, N.; de Montgolfier, O.; Pinçon, A.; Raignault, A.; Caland, L.; Labbé, P.; Thorin, E. Impact of pulse pressure on cerebrovascular events leading to age-related cognitive decline. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circ. Physiol. 2018, 314, H1214–H1224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Levin, R.A.; Carnegie, M.H.; Celermajer, D.S. Pulse Pressure: An Emerging Therapeutic Target for Dementia. Front. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. de Montgolfier, O.; Thorin-Trescases, N.; Thorin, E. Pathological Continuum From the Rise in Pulse Pressure to Impaired Neurovascular Coupling and Cognitive Decline. Am. J. Hypertens. 2020, 33, 375–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Hestad, K.; Engedal, K.; Schirmer, H.; Strand, B.H. The Effect of Blood Pressure on Cognitive Performance. An 8-Year Follow-Up of the Tromso Study, Comprising People Aged 45–74 Years. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Mahinrad, S.; Kurian, S.; Garner, C.R.; Sedaghat, S.; Nemeth, A.J.; Moscufo, N.; Higgins, J.P.; Jacobs, D.R.; Hausdorff, J.M.; Lloyd-Jones, D.M.; et al. Cumulative Blood Pressure Exposure during Young Adulthood and Mobility and Cognitive Function in Midlife. Circulation 2020, 141, 712–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Li, C.; Zhu, Y.; Ma, Y.; Hua, R.; Zhong, B.; Xie, W. Association of Cumulative Blood Pressure with Cognitive Decline, Dementia, and Mortality. J. Am. Coll. Ircardiol. 2022, 79, 1321–1335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Mathew, A.; Mesa, R.; Nahodyl, L.; Tremblay, J.; Rundek, T.; Al Hazzouri, A.Z.; Elfassy, T. Diastolic Blood Pressure and Cognitive Functioning: Differences by Systolic Blood Pressure among US Adults. Am. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. Other Dementias® 2023, 38, 15333175231172283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ostchega, Y.; Fryar, C.D.; Nwankwo, T.; Nguyen, D.T. Hypertension Prevalence among Adults Aged 18 and over: United States, 2017–2018; Key Findings Data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey; National Center for Health Statistics: Hyattsville, MD, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  34. Ramoshaba, N.E.; Huisman, H.W.; Lammertyn, L.; Kotliar, K.E.; Schutte, A.E.; Smith, W. Retinal microvasculature and masked hypertension in young adults: The African-PREDICT study. Hypertens. Res. 2020, 43, 1231–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Scatterplots showing the relationship between the selected cardiovascular and cognitive variables, including a line approximating the best fit of the linear interaction. (correlations between pulse pressure and phonemic fluency (A), left ventricular mass index (LVMI) and results of the Rapid Visual Information Processing A’ prime test (RVPA, higher is better) that measures sustained attention (B), mean daytime diastolic blood pressure (DBP) and Spatial Working Memory test (SWMS, lower is better) results measuring working memory and strategy use (C)).
Figure 1. Scatterplots showing the relationship between the selected cardiovascular and cognitive variables, including a line approximating the best fit of the linear interaction. (correlations between pulse pressure and phonemic fluency (A), left ventricular mass index (LVMI) and results of the Rapid Visual Information Processing A’ prime test (RVPA, higher is better) that measures sustained attention (B), mean daytime diastolic blood pressure (DBP) and Spatial Working Memory test (SWMS, lower is better) results measuring working memory and strategy use (C)).
Medicina 60 01353 g001
Table 1. Results of participant performance across different neuropsychological tests. For detailed descriptions of separate variables, please refer to the section “Cognitive evaluation” and Supplementary Table S1.
Table 1. Results of participant performance across different neuropsychological tests. For detailed descriptions of separate variables, please refer to the section “Cognitive evaluation” and Supplementary Table S1.
Cognitive TestNMean (SD) or Median (IQR)
Paper-pencil assessment
Categorical verbal fluency, points h7622.3 ± 7.0
Phonemic verbal fluency, points h7611.2 ± 5.4
VLS immediate recall, points h7615.5 (13.1–17.4)
VLS 30 min recall, points h7613.2 ± 3.9
VLS 24 h recall, points h7212.8 ± 4.1
Verbal IQ, points h72112.5 (100.3–120.0)
Nonverbal IQ, points h74112 (99.8–118.3)
IQ, points h68115 (105–123)
Computerized assessment
Match to Sample Visual Search
MTSPS82, ms c762131.6 (1738.5–2583.3)
MTSRCAMD, ms l762174.8 (1892.8–2623.4)
Paired Associates Learning
PALFAMS28, points h7614.2 ± 3.8
PALTEA28, points l769 (3.0–14.8)
Reaction Time Task
RTIFMDMT, ms l76282.8 ± 66.1
RTIFMDRT, ms l76357.5 (338.0–386.9)
RTISMDMT, ms l76242.5 (199.0–290.9)
RTISMDRT, ms l76321.3 (305.6–344.4)
Rapid Visual Information
Processing
RVPA, arb. unit h760.9 ± 0.1
RVPMDL, ms l76441.3 (409.0–506.5)
RVPPFA, arb.unit l760 (0–0)
Stockings of Cambridge
SOCITMD5, ms l7610,283.3 (7170.0–15,765.8)
SOCMNM5, points l765.6 (5.0–6.5)
SOCPSMMT, points h769.5 (9.0–11)
SOCSTMD5, ms l760 (0–410.6)
Spatial Span
SSPFSL, points h767 (6–8)
Spatial Working Memory
SWMBE4, points l760 (0–0)
SWMBE468, points l763 (0–11)
SWMBE6, points l760 (0–2)
SWMBE8, points l761 (0–9)
SWMS, arb. unit l766 (4–8)
c—complex interpretation, l—lower is better, h—higher is better, IQ—intelligence quotient, MTS—Match to Sample Visual Search, PAL—Paired Associates Learning, RTI—Reaction Time Task, RVP—Rapid Visual Information Processing, SOC—Stockings of Cambridge, SSP—Spatial Span, SWM—Spatial Working Memory, VLS—verbal-logical story task.
Table 2. Anthropometric, laboratory and blood pressure measurement data of the study participants.
Table 2. Anthropometric, laboratory and blood pressure measurement data of the study participants.
CharacteristicMean (SD) or Median (IQR)
Age, years36.5 (18.4–42.0)
BMI, kg/m228.8 ± 5.0
SBP, mmHg134 ± 16
DBP, mmHg80 (75–89)
PP, mmHg50 (45–58)
Creatinine, µmol/L79.3 ± 11.4
Uric acid, µmol/L384.2 ± 78.9
uACR, mg/mmol0.5 (0.3–0.8)
Cholesterol, mmol/l5.2 ± 1.2
LDL, mmol/L3.3 ± 1.0
HDL, mmol/L1.2 ± 0.3
Triglycerides, mmol/L1.4 (0.9–2.0)
LVM, g162.6 ± 43.4
LVMI, g/m2.733.1 ± 8.9
RWT0.38 ± 0.07
EF, %67 ± 9
24-h SBP, mmHg128 ± 10
24-h DBP, mmHg78 ± 9
24-h MAP, mmHg99 ± 10
Daytime SBP, mmHg131 ± 10
Daytime DBP, mmHg80 ± 10
Daytime MAP, mmHg102 ± 10
Nighttime SBP, mmHg117 ± 10
Nighttime DBP, mmHg68 (62.8–72.0)
Nighttime MAP, mmHg90 ± 9
SBP dipping, %10.4 ± 5.6
DBP dipping, %14.5 ± 6.0
IMT (left), mm0.58 (0.48–0.69)
IMT (right), mm0.59 ± 0.14
IMT (median), mm0.60 ± 0.14
BMI—body mass index, DBP—diastolic blood pressure, EF—ejection fraction, HDL—high-density lipoprotein cholesterol, IMT—intima-media thickness, LDL—low-density lipoprotein cholesterol, LVM—left ventricular mass, LVMI—left ventricular mass index, MAP—mean arterial pressure, PP—pulse pressure, RWT—relative wall thickness, SBP—systolic blood pressure, uACR—urine albumin-creatinine ratio.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Puteikis, K.; Ažukaitis, K.; Dadurkevičienė, D.; Mikučionytė, D.; Simanauskas, K.; Šileikienė, V.; Masiliūnas, R.; Jankauskienė, A.; Mameniškienė, R. Cognitive Outcomes in Young Adults with Primary Arterial Hypertension: The Role of Cardiovascular Risk Factors and Hypertension-Mediated Organ Damage. Medicina 2024, 60, 1353. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina60081353

AMA Style

Puteikis K, Ažukaitis K, Dadurkevičienė D, Mikučionytė D, Simanauskas K, Šileikienė V, Masiliūnas R, Jankauskienė A, Mameniškienė R. Cognitive Outcomes in Young Adults with Primary Arterial Hypertension: The Role of Cardiovascular Risk Factors and Hypertension-Mediated Organ Damage. Medicina. 2024; 60(8):1353. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina60081353

Chicago/Turabian Style

Puteikis, Kristijonas, Karolis Ažukaitis, Danguolė Dadurkevičienė, Dovilė Mikučionytė, Kazys Simanauskas, Vaida Šileikienė, Rytis Masiliūnas, Augustina Jankauskienė, and Rūta Mameniškienė. 2024. "Cognitive Outcomes in Young Adults with Primary Arterial Hypertension: The Role of Cardiovascular Risk Factors and Hypertension-Mediated Organ Damage" Medicina 60, no. 8: 1353. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina60081353

APA Style

Puteikis, K., Ažukaitis, K., Dadurkevičienė, D., Mikučionytė, D., Simanauskas, K., Šileikienė, V., Masiliūnas, R., Jankauskienė, A., & Mameniškienė, R. (2024). Cognitive Outcomes in Young Adults with Primary Arterial Hypertension: The Role of Cardiovascular Risk Factors and Hypertension-Mediated Organ Damage. Medicina, 60(8), 1353. https://doi.org/10.3390/medicina60081353

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop