Executive Function Capacities, Negative Driving Behavior and Crashes in Young Drivers
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Executive Function
3. Methods
4. Results
4.1. Working Memory
4.2. Inhibition
4.3. Set-Shifting
4.4. Attention
5. Discussion
5.1. Methodological Issues
5.2. Future Directions
6. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Leading Causes of Death Reports, National and Regional, 1999–2014. Available online: http://webappa.cdc.gov/sasweb/ncipc/leadcaus10_us.html (accessed on 27 July 2017).
- World Health Organization. Global Status Report on Road Safety 2015. Available online: http://www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention/road_safety_status/2015/en/ (accessed on 27 July 2017).
- Transportation Canada. Canadian Motor Vehicle Traffic Collision Statistics (NCDB), 2013. Available online: https://www.tc.gc.ca/media/documents/roadsafety/cmvtcs2013_eng.pdf (accessed on 27 July 2017).
- Insurance Institute for Highway Safety (IIHS). Fatality Facts: Teenagers 2014. Available online: http://www.iihs.org/iihs/topics/t/teenagers/fatalityfacts/teenagers/2014 (accessed on 27 July 2017).
- National Center for Statistics and Analysis. Young Drivers: 2014 Data (Traffic Safety Facts, Report No. DOT HS 812 278). Available online: https://crashstats.nhtsa.dot.gov/Api/Public/ViewPublication/812278 (accessed on 27 July 2017).
- Curry, A.E.; Hafetz, J.; Kallan, M.J.; Winston, F.K.; Durbin, D.R. Prevalence of teen driver errors leading to serious motor vehicle crashes. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2011, 43, 1285–1290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McDonald, C.C.; Curry, A.E.; Kandadai, V.; Sommers, M.S.; Winston, F.K. Comparison of teen and adult driver crash scenarios in a nationally representative sample of serious crashes. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2014, 72, 302–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huizinga, M.; Dolan, C.V.; van der Molen, M.W. Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 2017–2036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sturman, D.A.; Moghaddam, B. Striatum processes reward differently in adolescents versus adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 1719–1724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Romer, D.; Betancourt, L.M.; Giannetta, J.M.; Brodsky, N.L.; Farah, M.; Hurt, H. Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolescents. Neuropsychologia 2009, 47, 2916–2926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Romer, D.; Betancourt, L.M.; Brodsky, N.L.; Giannetta, J.M.; Yang, W.; Hurt, H. Does adolescent risk taking imply weak executive function? A prospective study of relations between working memory performance, impulsivity, and risk taking in early adolescence. Dev. Sci. 2011, 14, 1119–1133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Romer, D.; Reyna, V.F.; Satterthwaite, T.D. Beyond stereotypes of adolescent risk taking: Placing the adolescent brain in developmental context. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2017, 27, 19–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pope, C.N.; Bell, T.R.; Stavrinos, D. Mechanisms behind distracted driving behavior: The role of age and executive function in the engagement of distracted driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 98, 123–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Romer, D.; Lee, Y.-C.; McDonald, C.C.; Winston, F.K. Adolescence, attention allocation, and driving safety. J. Adolesc. Health 2014, 54, S6–S15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hayashi, Y.; Rivera, E.A.; Modico, J.G.; Foreman, A.M.; Wirth, O. Texting while driving, executive function, and impulsivity in college students. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 102, 72–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lambert, A.E.; Simons-Morton, B.G.; Cain, S.A.; Weisz, S.; Cox, D.J. Considerations of a dual-systems model of cognitive development and risky driving. J. Res. Adolesc. 2014, 24, 541–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beeli, G.; Koeneke, S.; Gasser, K.; Jancke, L. Brain stimulation modulates driving behavior. Behav. Brain Funct. 2008, 4, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Strayer, D.L.; Turrill, J.; Cooper, J.M.; Coleman, J.R.; Medeiros-Ward, N.; Biondi, F. Assessing cognitive distraction in the automobile. Hum. Factors 2015, 57, 1300–1324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Foy, H.J.; Runham, P.; Chapman, P. Prefrontal cortex activation and young driver behavior: A fNIRS study. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0156512:1–e0156512:18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Classen, S. (Ed.) Driving Simulation for Assessment, Intervention, and Training: A Guide for Occupational Therapy and Health Care Professionals; American Occupational Therapy Association Press: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2017; ISBN 978-1-56900-389-3. [Google Scholar]
- Curry, A.E.; Metzger, K.B.; Pfeiffer, M.R.; Elliott, M.R.; Winston, F.K.; Power, T.J. Motor vehicle crash risk among adolescents and young adults with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. JAMA Pediatr. 2017, 164, 942–948. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Classen, S.; Monahan, M. Indicators of simulated driving skills in adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Open J. Occup. Ther. 2013, 1, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reimer, B.; Fried, R.; Mehler, B.; Joshi, G.; Bolfek, A.; Godfrey, K.M.; Zhao, N.; Goldin, R.; Biederman, J. Brief report: Examining driving behavior in young adults with high functioning autism spectrum disorders: A pilot study using a driving simulation paradigm. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2013, 43, 2211–2217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rosenbloom, T.; Wultz, B. Thirty-day self-reported risky driving behaviors of ADHD and non-ADHD drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2011, 43, 128–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cox, S.M.; Cox, D.J.; Kofler, M.J.; Moncrief, M.A.; Johnson, R.J.; Lambert, A.E.; Cain, S.A.; Reeve, R.E. Driving simulator performance in novice drivers with Autism Spectrum Disorder: The role of executive functions and basic motor skills. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016, 46, 1379–1391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Garner, A.A.; Gentry, A.; Welburn, S.C.; Fine, P.R.; Franklin, C.A.; Stavrinos, D. Symptom dimensions of disruptive behavior disorders in adolescent drivers. J. Atten. Disord. 2014, 18, 496–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chee, D.Y.; Lee, H.C.; Patomella, A.H.; Falkmer, T. Driving behavior profile of drivers with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2017, 47, 2658–2670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lindsay, S. Systematic review of factors affecting driving and motor vehicle transportation among people with autism spectrum disorder. Disabil. Rehabil. 2017, 39, 837–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cox, D.J.; Brown, T.; Ross, V.; Moncrief, M.; Schmitt, R.; Gaffney, G.; Reeve, R. Can youth with autism spectrum disorder use virtual reality driving simulation training to evaluate and improve driving performance? An exploratory study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2017, 47, 2544–2555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pope, C.N.; Ross, L.A.; Stavrinos, D. Association between executive function and problematic adolescent driving. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2016, 37, 702–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Salthouse, T.A. Relations between cognitive abilities and measures of executive functioning. Neuropsychology 2005, 19, 532–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baddeley, A. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2012, 63, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McCabe, D.P.; Roediger, H.L.; McDaniel, M.A.; Balota, D.A.; Hambrick, D.Z. The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: Evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology 2010, 24, 222–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brydges, C.R.; Anderson, M.; Reid, C.L.; Fox, A.M. Maturation of cognitive control: Delineating response inhibition and interference suppression. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e69826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- O’Brien, F.; Gormley, M. The contribution of inhibitory deficits to dangerous driving among young people. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2013, 51, 238–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ross, V.; Jongen, E.M.M.; Wang, W.; Brijs, T.; Brijs, K.; Ruiter, R.A.C.; Wets, G. Investigating the influence of working memory capacity when driving behavior is combined with cognitive load: An LCT study of young novice drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2014, 62, 377–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Starkey, N.J.; Isler, R.B. The role of executive function, personality and attitudes to risks in explaining self-reported driving behavior in adolescent and adult male drivers. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2016, 38, 127–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dumontheil, I. Adolescent brain development. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2016, 10, 39–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luna, B.; Garver, K.E.; Urban, T.A.; Lazar, N.A.; Sweeney, J.A. Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 1357–1372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luna, B. Developmental changes in cognitive control through adolescence. Adv. Child Dev. Behav. 2009, 37, 233–278. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Jongen, E.M.M.; Brijs, K.; Komlos, M.; Brijs, T.; Wets, G. Inhibitory control and reward predict risky driving in young novice drivers—A simulator study. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2011, 20, 604–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Satterthwaite, T.D.; Elliott, M.A.; Ruparel, K.; Loughead, J.; Prabhakaran, K.; Calkins, M.E.; Hopson, R.; Jackson, C.; Keefe, J.; Riley, M.; et al. Neuroimaging of the Philadelphia neurodevelopmental cohort. NeuroImage 2014, 86, 544–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morris, L.J.; Dawson, S.J. Relationships between age, executive function and driving behavior. Available online: http://acrs.org.au/files/arsrpe/RS080004.pdf (accessed on 25 October 2017).
- Roca, J.; Lupiáñez, J.; López-Ramón, M.F.; Castro, C. Are drivers’ attentional lapses associated with the functioning of the neurocognitive attentional networks and with cognitive failure in everyday life? Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2013, 17, 98–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ross, V.; Jongen, E.; Brijs, T.; Ruiter, R.; Brijs, K.; Wets, G. The relation between cognitive control and risky driving in young novice drivers. Appl. Neuropsychol. Adult 2015, 22, 61–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Guinosso, S.A.; Johnson, S.B.; Schultheis, M.T.; Graefe, A.C.; Bishai, D.M. Neurocognitive correlates of young drivers’ performance in a driving simulator. J. Adolesc. Health 2016, 58, 467–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mäntylä, T.; Karlsson, M.J.; Marklund, M. Executive control functions in simulated driving. Appl. Neuropsychol. 2009, 16, 11–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hatfield, J.; Williamson, A.; Kehoe, E.J.; Prabhakharan, P. An examination of the relationship between measures of impulsivity and risky simulated driving amongst young drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 103, 37–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ross, V.; Jongen, E.M.M.; Brijs, K.; Brijs, T.; Wets, G. Investigating risky, distracting, and protective peer passenger effects in a dual process framework. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2016, 93, 217–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cascio, C.N.; Carp, J.; O'Donnell, M.B.; Tinney, F.J., Jr.; Bingham, C.R.; Shope, J.T.; Ouimet, M.C.; Pradhan, A.K.; Simons-Morton, B.G.; Falk, E.B. Buffering social influence: Neural correlates of response inhibition predict driving safety in the presence of a peer. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015, 27, 83–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Graefe, A.C.; Schultheis, M.T. Examining Neurocognitive Correlates of Risky Driving Behavior in Young Adults Using a Simulated Driving Environment. Proceedings of International Conference on Virtual Rehabilitation, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 26–29 August 2013; IEEEXplore: Hudson Hoboken, NJ, USA, November 2013; pp. 235–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klauer, S.G.; Ehsani, J.P.; McGehee, D.V.; Manser, M. The effect of secondary task engagement on adolescents’ driving performance and crash risk. J. Adolesc. Health 2015, 57, S36–S43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Winter, J.C.F.; Dodou, D. The Driver Behavior Questionnaire as a predictor of accidents: A meta-analysis. J. Saf. Res. 2010, 41, 463–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bowman, C.; Cutmore, T.; Shum, D. The development of prospective memory across adolescence: An event-related potential analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swedler, D.I.; Bowman, S.M.; Baker, S.P. Gender and age differences among teen drivers in fatal crashes. Ann. Adv. Automot. Med. 2012, 56, 97–106. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Wang, W.; Xi, J.; Chen, H. Modeling and recognizing driver behavior based on driving data: A survey. Math. Probl. Eng. 2014, 2014, e245641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salvucci, D.D. Modeling driver behavior in a cognitive architecture. Hum. Factors 2006, 48, 362–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sagberg, F.; Selpi; Bianchi Piccinini, G.F.; Engström, J. A review of research on driving styles and road safety. Hum. Factors 2015, 57, 1248–1275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McCartt, A.T.; Teoh, E.R. Tracking progress in teenage driver crash risk in the United States since the advent of graduated driver licensing programs. J. Saf. Res. 2015, 53, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Masten, S.V.; Foss, R.D.; Marshall, S.W. Graduated Driver Licensing and fatal crashes involving 16- to 19-year-old drivers. JAMA 2011, 306, 1098–1103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Watson, J.M.; Strayer, D.L. Supertaskers: Profiles in extraordinary multitasking ability. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2010, 17, 479–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Asimakopulos, J.; Boychuck, Z.; Sondergaard, D.; Poulin, V.; Ménard, I.; Korner-Bitensky, N. Assessing executive function in relation to fitness to drive: A review of tools and their ability to predict safe driving. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 2012, 59, 402–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Classen, S. Driving Simulation for Assessment, Intervention, and Training: A Guide for Occupational Therapy and Health Care Professionals; AOTA Press: Bethesda, MD, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Moisala, M.; Salmela, V.; Hietajärvi, L.; Carlson, S.; Vuontela, V.; Lonka, K.; Hakkarainen, K.; Salmela-Aro, K.; Alho, K. Gaming is related to enhanced working memory performance and task-related cortical activity. Brain Res. 2017, 1655, 204–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diamond, A.; Ling, D.S. Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 18, 34–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cassavaugh, N.D.; Kramer, A.F. Transfer of computer-based training to simulated driving in older adults. Appl. Ergon. 2009, 40, 943–952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cuenen, A.; Jongen, E. M.; Brijs, T.; Brijs, K.; Houben, K.; Wets, G. Effect of a working memory training on aspects of cognitive ability and driving ability of older drivers: merits of an adaptive training over a non-adaptive training. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2016, 42, 15–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tigadi, A.; Gujanatti, R.; Gonchi, A. Advanced Driver Assistance Systems. Int. J. Eng. Res. Gen. Sci. 2016, 4, 151–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- IIHS. Status Report: August 23, 2017. Available online: http://www.iihs.org/externaldata/srdata/docs/sr5206.pdf (accessed on 23 August 2017).
- Delgado, M.K.; Wanner, K.J.; McDonald, C. Adolescent cellphone use while driving: An overview of the literature and promising future directions for prevention. Media Commun. 2016, 4, 79–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Author | Sample | EF measure | Driving Outcome (and Metrics) | Summary of Main Finding(s) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Morris et al., 2008 [45] | i) n = 92, age: 17–25 ii) n = 244, age: 18–58 | Self-Report Global EF: BRIEF-A | Self-Report DBQ (lapses, errors and violations) | Lower scores on all EF subscales related to more negative driving outcomes. Poor global EF explained 27% and 17% of the variance in negative driving behavior for each respective group. In addition, EF partially mediated the effects of age on negative driving behavior. |
Mäntylä et al., 2009 [49] | n = 50, age: 15–19 | Performance Working Memory: N-back task Matrix monitoring Inhibition: Stroop task (Interference) Stop Signal task (Response) Set-shifting: Plus 3/Minus 3 task; Trail-Making Test | Performance Driving Simulator Lane change task (lane position and deviation) | Individual differences in EF were related to lane position and variability, but only poor working memory (not set-shifting or inhibition) significantly predicted greater variability. This effect was also mediated by computer gaming skills. |
Jongen et al., 2011 [43] | i) n = 31, age: 17–18 ii) n = 22, age: 22–24 | Performance Response Inhibition: Stop Signal task | Performance Driving Simulator Task1 of normal driving and Task2 of risk-reward driving (lane position, speeding, running red lights, crashes) | Inhibitory control increased with age (suggesting continuing development), and lower inhibitory control was related to more variability in lane position, but not with risky driving behavior (speeding and red-light running). |
Roca et al., 2013 [46] | n = 104, Mage: 21 | Performance Cognitive Failures: Cognitive Failures Questionnaire Attention & Vigilance: ANTI-V (tonic alertness/vigilance, executive control, orienting and phasic alertness indices) | Self-Report DBQ (lapses, errors, violations and aggressive driving) | The more cognitive failures reported, the higher the aberrant driving scores on the DBQ. More lapses while driving were related to more cognitive failures and poorer vigilance. No other driving behavior factors correlated with the ANTI-V. Driving errors and violations were also highly correlated with cognitive failures. |
O’Brien et al., 2013 [37] | i) n = 30, age: 17–21 (Speeding offenders) ii) n = 40, age: 17–21 (Controls) | Performance Response Inhibition: Go/No-Go task; Stop Signal task | Performance History of speeding offense | Police-reported speeding offenders had poorer inhibitory control on one performance task only (the Go/No-Go), compared to a non-offender control group. |
Graefe et al., 2013 [53] | n = 49, Mage: 20.25 | Performance Interference Inhibition: Stroop task Set-shifting: Wisconsin Card Sorting task; WM, Scanning, Processing Speed: Symbol Digit Modalities Test Attention: ANT (alerting, orienting, and executive attention) Risk propensity: BART | Performance Driving Simulator baseline task and risky driving task with time pressure and both rewards and punishments for performance (speed, lane position, stopping behavior, reaction times to hazards situations, risky overtakes) | Executive function performances did not significantly predict driving performance on the risky driving task. |
Ross et al., 2014 [38] | n = 46, age: 17–25 | Performance Working Memory: Visuospatial span; Verbal Letter span WM Load Task: Verbal N-back task | Performance Driving Simulator lane change task at baseline and with a secondary WM task (correct lane changes, lane change initiation and path deviation) | Driving performance deteriorated overall with increasing verbal WM load on the secondary task, but drivers with better verbal WM capacity at baseline had better lane change initiation and percentage of correct lane changes. These variables were not vulnerable to the secondary task load. |
Cascio et al., 2014 [52] | n = 42, age: 16–17 (all male) | Performance Response Inhibition: Go/No-Go task | Performance Driving Simulator with either a risk promoting or non-risk promoting peer passenger (red-light running) | Higher inhibitory control related to less red-light running, but only in the presence of a cautious peer passenger. |
Ross et al., 2015 [47] | n = 38, age: 17–25 (Mage: 19.03) | Performance Working Memory: Digit span; Visuospatial span Response Inhibition: Stop Signal task; Go/No-Go task | Performance Driving Simulator (lane position, speeding, responses to red and yellow traffic lights, responses to road hazards, and following distance to slow vehicles) | Poor verbal working memory and inhibitory control (on the Stop Signal task alone) predicted more variability in lane position. However, poor inhibitory control alone predicted more collisions and poorer hazard detection and response. Higher visuospatial working memory performance predicted more red and yellow light running. |
Guinosso et al., 2016 [48] | n = 74, age: 16–24 (Mage: 19.8) | Performance Interference Inhibition: Stroop task Set-shifting: Wisconsin Card Sort Test-64 Attention: Attention Network Task (ANT: alerting, orienting, and executive attention) WM Load Task: Verbal WM task | Performance Driving Simulator task at baseline and with a secondary WM task (velocity, accelerator position, lane position, steering wheel position) | Better Stroop inhibition and alerting predicted more consistent driving at baseline, and greater inhibitory control also predicted less variability in driving during distraction (WM load task). Flexibility, orienting, and conflict executive control were not associated with performance in either driving condition. |
Starkey et al., 2016 [39] | i) n = 46, age: 16–18 ii) n = 32, age: 25+ | Performance Working Memory: Digits Forwards/ Backwards Interference Inhibition: Color Word Interference Test Forward Planning: Tower Test Attention: Letter Cancellation Information Processing: Trail-Making Test | Self-Report (a) Driving history questionnaire (b) Driver Risk Taking questionnaire (c) Driver Attitude Questionnaire | Adolescent drivers had poorer EF and were more accepting of risk. Working memory and attitudes to risk explained self-reported driving behavior, with better working memory related to more self-reported risky driving behavior and acceptance of risk. Safer driving correlated with better forward planning and less acceptance of risk. |
Ross et al., 2016 [51] | i) n = 30, age: 17–18 ii) n = 20, age: 22–24 | Performance Response Inhibition: Stop Signal task | Performance Driving Simulator Task1 of normal driving and Task2 of driving with peer presence (lane position, speeding, running red and amber lights, braking and deceleration for hazards, and crashes) | Drivers with low inhibitory control showed increased speeding in the presence of peer passengers. Inhibitory control did not relate to lane position, running traffic lights, braking, or deceleration for road hazards and collisions. |
Pope, et al., 2016 [31] | n = 46, age: 16–19 | Self-Report Global EF: BRIEF-SR Performance Working Memory: Backwards Digit Span Set-shifting: Trail-Making Test | Self-Report Problematic driving outcomes (crashes, citations, being pulled over) | Poor self-reported planning and organization correlated with more reports of prior crashes, with poor self-reported inhibitory control associated with prior traffic citations. Multiple BRIEF subscales had a negative correlation with being pulled over. However, there was no relationship between performance based EF measures and driving outcomes. |
Pope, et al., 2017 [14] | i) n = 13, age: 19–20 ii) n = 21, age: 36–54 iii) n = 25, age: 65–92 | Self-Report Global EF: BRIEF-A | Self-Report Distracted driving behavior questionnaire | Younger and middle aged adults engaged in more distracted driving than older adults. Lower EF scores was a unique predictor of more self-reported engagement in distracted driving in all age groups. |
Hayashi et al., 2017 [16] | i) n = 20, Mage: 19 (Texters) ii) n = 20, Mage: 18.7 (Controls) | Self-Report Global EF: Executive Function Index | Self-Report Texting while driving questionnaire | The levels of EF on all subscales were higher in the “non-texter” (while driving) group, where “texters” had lower scores on EF subscales of strategic planning and impulse control, and lower total EF scores. |
Hatfield et al., 2017 [50] | n = 71, Mage: 18.96 | Performance Response Inhibition: Go/No-Go task Stroop task | Performance (a) Driving simulator (percentage of distance speeding and lane position) (b) Reward Saccade Task (c) Hazard Perception Task | Poor inhibitory control on the Go/No-Go alone positively correlated with total “unsafe” driving (defined unsafe responses to events), speeding in the slow zone, and overall speeding (with a large effect size). |
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Walshe, E.A.; Ward McIntosh, C.; Romer, D.; Winston, F.K. Executive Function Capacities, Negative Driving Behavior and Crashes in Young Drivers. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 1314. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14111314
Walshe EA, Ward McIntosh C, Romer D, Winston FK. Executive Function Capacities, Negative Driving Behavior and Crashes in Young Drivers. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2017; 14(11):1314. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14111314
Chicago/Turabian StyleWalshe, Elizabeth A., Chelsea Ward McIntosh, Daniel Romer, and Flaura K. Winston. 2017. "Executive Function Capacities, Negative Driving Behavior and Crashes in Young Drivers" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 14, no. 11: 1314. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14111314
APA StyleWalshe, E. A., Ward McIntosh, C., Romer, D., & Winston, F. K. (2017). Executive Function Capacities, Negative Driving Behavior and Crashes in Young Drivers. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(11), 1314. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14111314