RF EMF Risk Perception Revisited: Is the Focus on Concern Sufficient for Risk Perception Studies?
Abstract
:1. Background
1.1. Introduction
1.2. Research Aims
- The suggested grouping of RF EMF risk perceptions according to three components—concern, thematic relevance, and discursive relevance—results in a meaningful distribution of responses.
- People with high levels of concerns for whom RF EMF risk potentials are thematically or both thematically and discursively relevant, will evaluate RF EMF exposure situations differently in comparison people with high levels of concerns but with low levels of thematic and discursive relevance. They will express stronger negative feelings and moral concerns and view themselves as strongly exposed.
- The effect of a reduction in RF EMF exposure on the acceptance of base stations depends on the thematic and discursive relevance of a respondent’s RF EMF risk perception.
2. Methods
3. Results
3.1. Risk Perception Groups and Socio Demographic Patterns
3.2. Intuitive Exposure Assessments
3.3. Affective, Moral and Subjective Exposure Evaluations in Risk Perception Groups
3.4. Differential Effects of RF EMF Exposure Reduction on the Acceptance of Base Stations in Risk Perception Groups
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Acknowledgments
Author Contributions
Conflicts of Interest
Availability of Data and Materials
Abbreviations
ANOVA | Analysis of variance |
BS | base station |
End con | enduring concerned |
MP | mobile phones |
N/l con | not and less concerned |
Not end con | not enduringly concerned |
RF EMF | radio frequency electromagnetic field |
RP | risk perception |
S | Scenario |
SPSS® | Statistical Package for the Social Sciences |
References
- Tesanovic, M.; Conil, E.; De Domenico, A.; Aguero, R.; Freudenstein, F.; Correia, L.M.; Bories, S.; Martens, L.; Wiedemann, P.M.; Wiart, J. The Lexnet Project: Wireless Networks and EMF: Paving the Way for Low-EMF Networks of the Future. IEEE Veh. Technol. Mag. 2014, 9, 20–28. [Google Scholar]
- Espinosa, H.G.; Brindley, C.; Thiel, D.V. An assessment of simulation methodologies for the analysis of near-field radiation zones related to human exposure. In Proceedings of the IEEE iWAT International Workshop on Antenna Technology, Sydney, Australia, 4–6 March 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Gajšek, P.; Ravazzani, P.; Wiart, J.; Grellier, J.; Samaras, T.; Thuróczy, G. Electromagnetic field exposure assessment in Europe radiofrequency fields (10 MHz–6 GHz). J. Expos. Sci. Environ. Epidemiol. 2015, 25, 37–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sandman, P.M. Risk Communication: Facing Public Outrage. Manag. Commun. Q. 1987, 2, 235–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber, E.; Blais, A.; Betz, N.E. A domain specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perception and risk behaviors. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2002, 15, 263–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fischhoff, B.; Lichtenstein, S.; Slovic, P.; Derby, L.; Keeney, R.L. Acceptable Risk; Camridge University Press: Cambrigde, UK, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Weinstein, N.D.; Kwitel, A.; McCaul, K.D.; Magnan, R.E.; Gerrard, M.; Gibbons, F.X. Risk perceptions: Assessment and relationship to influenza vaccination. Health Psychol. 2007, 26, 146–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dillard, A.J.; Ferrer, R.A.; Ubel, P.A.; Fagerlin, A. Risk perception measures’ associations with behavior intentions, affect, and cognition following colon cancer screening messages. Health Psychol. 2012, 31, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sjöberg, L. Risk and worry. Risk Anal. 1998, 18, 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trumbo, C.W.; Peek, L.; Meyer, M.A.; Marlatt, H.L.; Gruntfest, E.; McNoldy, B.D.; Schubert, W.H.A. Cognitive-Affective Scale for Hurricane Risk Perception. Risk Anal. 2016, 36, 2233–2246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eurobarometer TNS Opinion Social. Eurobarometer 73.3. Electromagnetic Fields. 2010. Available online: http://ec.europa.eu/public_opinion/archives/ebs/ebs_347_en.pdf (accessed on 14 November 2016).
- Schwarz, N. Self-reports: How the questions shape the answers. Am. Psychol. 1999, 54, 93–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zwick, M. Risk as perceived by the German public: Pervasive risks and “switching” risks. J. Risk Res. 2005, 8, 481–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gaskell, G.; Hohl, K.; Gerber, M. Do closed survey questions overestimate public perceptions of food risks? J. Risk Res. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gray, E.; Jackson, J.; Farrall, S. Reassessing the Fear of Crime. Eur. J. Criminol. 2008, 5, 363–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schütz, A.; Luckmann, T. Strukturen der Lebenswelt; Suhrkamp: Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Schütz, A. Das Problem der Relevanz. Richard Zaner; Suhrkamp: Frankfurt am Main, Germany, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Hogarth, R.; Portell, M.; Cuxart, A. What Risks Do People Perceive in Everyday Life? A Perspective Gained from the Experience Sampling. (ESM). Risk Anal. 2007, 27, 1427–1439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Andreas, D.C. Risk Discourse. In The International Encyclopedia of Language and Social Interaction; Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2015; ISBN 978-1-118-61110-4. [Google Scholar]
- Hammond, K.R. Ecological Validity: Then and Now. 1998. Available online: http://www.brunswik.org/notes/essay2.html (accessed on 15 March 2017).
- Freudenstein, F.; Wiedemann, P.M.; Brown, T.W.C. Exposure perception as a key indicator of risk perception and acceptance of sources of radio frequency electromagnetic fields. J. Environ. Public Health 2015, 2015, 198272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wiedemann, P.M.; Boerner, F.; Claus, F. How far is how far enough? Safety perception and acceptance of extra-high-voltage power lines in Germany. J. Risk Res. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindell, M.K.; Earle, T.C. How close is close enough: Public perceptions of the risks of industrial facilities. Risk Anal. 1983, 3, 245–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- European Social Survey. ESS Round 6 Source Questionnaire; Centre for Comparative Social Surveys, City University London: London, UK, 2012; Available online: http://www.europeansocialsurvey.org (accessed on 14 November 2016).
- Freudenstein, F.; Wiedemann, P.M.; Varsier, N. Exposure knowledge and risk perception of RF EMF. Front. Public Health 2015, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rozin, P. Technological stigma: Some perspectives from the study of contagion. In Risk, Media, and Stigma: Understanding Public Challenges to Modern Science and Technology; Flynn, J., Slovic, P., Kunreuther, H., Eds.; Earthscan Publications: London, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Cousin, M.; Siegrist, M. Risk perception of mobile communication: A mental models approach. J. Risk Res. 2010, 13, 599–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Claassen, L.; Bostrom, A.; Timmermans, D.R. Focal points for improving communications about electromagnetic fields and health: A mental models approach. J. Risk Res. 2014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Claassen, L.; van Dongen, D.; Timmermans, D.R. Improving lay understanding of exposure to electromagnetic fields; the effect of information on perception of and responses to risk. J. Risk Res. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Question | Answer Option |
---|---|
Concerns | 5–point Likert scale from 1 = not at all concerned, 2 = not very concerned, 3 = neither nor, 4 = fairly concerned, 5 = very concerned |
“How concerned are you about the potential health effects of electromagnetic fields in general?” | |
Thematic relevance | 5–point Likert scale from 1 = never, 2 = not very often, 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, 5 = very often |
“How often in your daily life do you think about the topic “potential health effects of electromagnetic fields”?” | |
Discursive relevance | 5–point Likert scale, same as for above |
“How often in your daily life do you talk about potential health effects of EMF with other people (including conversation, via Facebook, twitter, chat, online forum or similar)?” |
Daily RF EMF Exposure | Number and % within Groups | Total | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Not and Lesser Concerned | Not Enduringly Concerned | Enduringly Concerned | ||
(1) Not at all | 29 (3.1%) | 2 (0.4%) | 0 (0.0%) | 31 (1.8%) |
(2) | 155 (16.7%) | 38 (6.9%) | 7 (3.1%) | 200 (11.7%) |
(3) | 326 (35.1%) | 156 (28.5%) | 45 (19.8%) | 527 (30.9%) |
(4) | 246 (26.5%) | 197 (35.9%) | 81 (35.7%) | 524 (30.8%) |
(5) To a very high degree | 173 (18.6%) | 155 (28.3%) | 94 (41.4%) | 422 24.8%) |
Total | 929 (100%) | 548 (100%) | 227 (100%) | 1704 (100%) |
Evaluation of | N/l Con | Not End Con | End Con | Total Mean | p | F (Welch) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mobile phone (MP) calls: | ||||||
Affective evaluation | 2.86 | 3.09 | 3.46 | 3.02 | <0.001 | 53.31 |
Moral evaluation | 2.37 | 3.08 | 3.80 | 2.79 | <0.001 | 154.67 |
Subjective exposure perception | 2.92 | 3.63 | 4.08 | 3.31 | <0.001 | 185.33 |
Base stations | ||||||
Affective evaluation | 3.41 | 3.69 | 4.05 | 3.59 | <0.001 | 31.22 |
Moral evaluation | 3.21 | 3.94 | 4.47 | 3.62 | <0.001 | 149.43 |
Subjective exposure perception | 3.42 | 4.20 | 4.63 | 3.85 | <0.001 | 131.98 |
Exposure Reduction Scenario | RP Groups | RP Groups | Mean Difference Distance (M) | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
0% | n/l con | not end con | 872 | <0.001 |
end con | 1471 | <0.001 | ||
not end con | n/l con | 872 | <0.001 | |
end con | 600 | 0.055 | ||
end con | n/l con | 1471 | <0.001 | |
not end con | 600 | 0.055 | ||
30% | n/l con | not end con | 684 | <0.001 |
end con | 1174 | <0.001 | ||
not end con | n/l con | 684 | <0.001 | |
end con | 490 | 0.072 | ||
end con | n/l con | 1174 | <0.001 | |
not end con | 490 | 0.072 | ||
50% | n/l con | not end con | 559 | <0.001 |
end con | 976 | <0.001 | ||
not end con | n/l con | 549 | <0.001 | |
end con | 427 | 0.080 | ||
end con | n/l con | 976 | <0.001 | |
not end con | 427 | 0.080 | ||
70% | n/l con | not end con | 429 | <0.001 |
end con | 853 | <0.001 | ||
not end con | n/l con | 429 | <0.001 | |
end con | 424 | 0.060 | ||
end con | n/l con | 853 | <0.001 | |
not end con | 424 | 0.060 |
© 2017 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wiedemann, P.M.; Freudenstein, F.; Böhmert, C.; Wiart, J.; Croft, R.J. RF EMF Risk Perception Revisited: Is the Focus on Concern Sufficient for Risk Perception Studies? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 620. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14060620
Wiedemann PM, Freudenstein F, Böhmert C, Wiart J, Croft RJ. RF EMF Risk Perception Revisited: Is the Focus on Concern Sufficient for Risk Perception Studies? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2017; 14(6):620. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14060620
Chicago/Turabian StyleWiedemann, Peter M., Frederik Freudenstein, Christoph Böhmert, Joe Wiart, and Rodney J. Croft. 2017. "RF EMF Risk Perception Revisited: Is the Focus on Concern Sufficient for Risk Perception Studies?" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 14, no. 6: 620. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14060620
APA StyleWiedemann, P. M., Freudenstein, F., Böhmert, C., Wiart, J., & Croft, R. J. (2017). RF EMF Risk Perception Revisited: Is the Focus on Concern Sufficient for Risk Perception Studies? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(6), 620. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14060620