Next Article in Journal
Coital Frequency and the Probability of Pregnancy in Couples Trying to Conceive Their First Child: A Prospective Cohort Study in Japan
Previous Article in Journal
Perceived Needs Among Asylum Seekers in Sweden: A Mixed Methods Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Countering Vaccine Hesitancy among Pregnant Women in England: The Case of Boostrix-IPV

Department of Behavioural Science and Health, University College London, London WC1E 6BT, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(14), 4984; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17144984
Submission received: 27 May 2020 / Revised: 23 June 2020 / Accepted: 7 July 2020 / Published: 10 July 2020

Abstract

:
This study explored the effects of message framing on vaccine hesitancy for the antenatal whooping cough vaccine. The study also assessed whether the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) constructs had any explanatory utility for vaccine intentions and behaviours in pregnant women. A between-subjects, cross-sectional design was employed. Participants (n = 282) were women who were pregnant (mean = 28 weeks, SD = 7.0), living in England and between 18 and 44 years of age. A self-report web-based survey was used to collect data. Participants were randomly assigned to read either (i) disease risk, (ii) myth busting, or (iii) control information before answering questions based on the TPB. No significant effects of message framing were found. Attitudes (Beta = 0.699; p < 0.001) and subjective norms (Beta = 0.262, p < 0.001) significantly predicted intention to vaccinate but perceived behavioural control did not. The TPB constructs accounted for 86% and 36% of the variance in vaccine intention and vaccine history respectively. Disease risk information did not influence vaccine acceptability in this sample of English pregnant women. The study offered preliminary evidence that interventions targeting constructs from the TPB may promote vaccine acceptability among pregnant women.

1. Introduction

Pertussis (whooping cough) is one of the leading causes of vaccine-preventable deaths worldwide [1]. Infants less than two months of age are the most vulnerable age group for pertussis-related morbidity and mortality [2,3]. In 2012, a national outbreak of whooping cough was declared in the UK. Over 9300 cases were reported, and 14 infants less than 12 months old died [4], resulting in the highest pertussis-related mortality rate since 1982 in the UK. In response to this epidemic, a pertussis-containing vaccine for pregnant women was introduced in October 2012 and was offered at routine antenatal appointments. The vaccine includes low dose diphtheria, tetanus, acellular pertussis, and inactivated poliomyelitis (Boostrix-IPV or DTaP/IPV). This vaccine is now routinely recommended in several countries (e.g., the US, Brazil, Israel, Belgium) and is available free of charge for pregnant women in the UK from 16-weeks gestation [5]. Despite the strong body of evidence providing support for the efficacy and safety of Boostrix-IPV, immunisation coverage, defined as the proportion of eligible persons who receive the vaccine, is suboptimal. Although immunisation coverage has increased since the introduction of the vaccine [6], it remains below the World Health Organisation coverage target of 90% worldwide [7], and the most recent annual report indicates that coverage in England is suboptimal and declining (69%) [8].
Vaccine hesitancy, defined as the ‘delay in acceptance or refusal of vaccination despite availability of vaccination services’ [9], is a central public health concern and pregnant women have been found to be more vaccine hesitant than the general population [10]. A systematic review addressing vaccine attitudes during pregnancy [11] reported that the majority of related studies have focused on the influenza vaccine in North America and the tetanus vaccine in Asia and Africa. The main concerns participants cited across all antenatal vaccines related to the safety and necessity of vaccines during pregnancy and low knowledge of the disease or vaccine. Only seven of 155 studies examined the whooping cough vaccine. Of those that did, most studies employed quantitative cross-sectional designs and examined a broad range of outcomes, from healthcare provider recommendations in the US [12] to immunisation coverage in Belgium [13] to vaccine intentions in Mexico [14].
Subsequent qualitative research which investigated attitudes toward Boostrix-IPV in the UK [15,16] reported similar findings to those cited across all antenatal vaccines in the review. The greatest self-reported barriers to uptake were safety concerns for the baby [16] and uncertainties relating to the risk and benefit of the whooping cough vaccine [15]. Despite studies which have identified consistent sources of vaccine hesitancy among pregnant women, there is a paucity of research which has developed and evaluated interventions to counter antenatal vaccine hesitancy for the whooping vaccine.
While it could be expected that any information to support the safety and benefit of vaccines would promote positive vaccine attitudes and increase immunisation coverage, research has highlighted that not all pro-vaccine messages are effective [17]. Nyhan and colleagues [17], for example, found that vaccine messages which directly addressed vaccine misconceptions and corrected vaccine myths reduced participants’ intentions to vaccinate. Other attempts to dispel vaccine safety misconceptions have also been ineffective and only served as a reminder of incorrect information to respondents [18].
The manner in which vaccine information is framed has previously been shown to significantly impact vaccine acceptability and uptake [19]. Many health researchers have drawn on Prospect Theory [20] to account for such findings, which maintains that presenting logically equivalent information about risk in different ways alters people’s preferences and actions. Several studies which employed loss-framed vaccine messages, highlighting the risk of not engaging in a protective behaviour [20], reported significant findings for increasing vaccine intentions over other strategies [21,22,23]. Horne and colleagues [23], for example, noted the success of messages which highlighted the consequences of failing to vaccinate. Hence, rather than attempting to refute common myths associated with the measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine, an approach which has previously been shown to be ineffective, they emphasised the risks of not vaccinating. They found that presenting this ‘disease risk information’ was significantly more effective in increasing vaccine acceptability (attitudes and intentions) than ‘myth busting information’; educational messages dispelling vaccine myths.
Additional theoretical frameworks which have been applied to understand and predict vaccine intentions and behaviours are constructs from the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) [24]. The TPB posits that attitudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioural control predict an individual’s intention to engage in a behaviour, which in turn, is the single best predictor of performing a behaviour [25,26]. Attitudes are defined as the degree to which a person has a favourable or unfavourable evaluation of an action. Subjective norms relate to normative influences and the perceived social pressure to perform or not perform an act. Perceived behavioural control (PBC) is the perceived ease or difficulty of performing the behaviour of interest, and PBC has been found to affect behaviour both directly and indirectly, through its effects on intentions [27]. The TPB has been criticised for its ability to better predict intentions over behaviour [28], its unsubstantiated assumption about the causal relationship between intention and behaviour [29], and disregard for other likely unconscious determinants of behaviour, such as emotions [30]. Nevertheless, the TPB has outperformed other theories in accounting for variance in vaccine uptake [31] and has been identified as a suitable theoretical model for predicting uptake for several vaccines [32,33].
The present study adopted a similar experimental design to Horne and colleagues [23] to explore the impact of framed messages on vaccine acceptability for Boostrix-IPV among a sample of pregnant women. The primary aim of the study was to assess whether disease risk information resulted in greater vaccine acceptability than myth busting information. Vaccine acceptability was measured using constructs from the TPB; attitudes, subjective norms, PBC and intentions. The secondary aim of the study was to assess whether constructs from the TPB were significantly associated with antenatal vaccine intentions and behaviours for Boostrix-IPV. Finally, the association between framing effects and background factors including, education level, number of children and vaccine history, was evaluated.

2. Materials and Methods

A cross-sectional between-subjects design was employed whereby participants were randomly assigned to one of three experimental groups. Participation was voluntary, but respondents were offered an incentive of entering a draw to win a £20 online Amazon.co.uk voucher. A self-report web-based questionnaire via SurveyMonkey.com was used to collect data. Two attention checks were included in the survey to ensure thoughtful answers were provided.
All participants were recruited via online social media platforms, namely Facebook.com and Netmums.co.uk. Participants had to identify as female, be at least 16 weeks pregnant, live in England and be between 18 and 50 years of age. The rationale for solely including participants who were 16 weeks pregnant was to ensure that all participants were eligible for the vaccine. A combination of targeted paid advertising and free informal advertising within online pregnancy groups was used to recruit participants. A total sample size of 279 was required by G* Power (Version 3.1. 9.2), University of Kiel, Germany, 2014 [34] to detect a small to medium effect size (f =0.1876), assuming a power of 0.80 and a two-sided significance of 0.05.
Ethical approval was granted by University College London Research Ethics Committee (reference: 10353/001). All data were collected between May and July 2017 inclusive. Respondents were briefed about the nature of the research via an information sheet before they were invited to participate in the study. After consent was obtained, participants were randomly assigned to one of three experimental conditions: (a) disease risk, (b) myth busting or (c) control. In each of the conditions, participants were asked to read brief information (less than one A4 page). In the disease risk condition, participants read a short story about a baby who contracted whooping cough, which highlighted the health risks posed by failing to vaccinate. In the myth busting condition, participants read that some pregnant women have safety concerns about the vaccine. They were then presented with a summary of three studies which refuted the link between vaccinating while pregnant and adverse outcomes for the mother and baby. In the control condition, participants read a vignette on an unrelated scientific topic (bird feeding). The content for all conditions was adapted from Horne and colleagues’ study [23]. Prior to data collection, the reading ability required for each condition was examined using three validated and widely used measures—the SMOG [35], the Gunning Fog [36] and the Flesch–Kincaid reading ease score [37]. This was to ensure the language was accessible and consistent across experimental conditions (see (Appendix A) for the wording of each condition).
After reading this information, participants’ vaccination attitudes were assessed based on constructs from the TPB [24]—PBC, attitudes, subjective norms, and intentions. Demographic information including age, ethnicity, education level, employment status, relationship status, gestational age, and number of children was also collected. Finally, participants were asked if they were aware of the vaccine prior to the study and whether they had already received Boostrix-IPV during their pregnancy (see Appendix B). To account for any potentially harmful effects of the experimental conditions, all participants were informed about the importance of the antenatal whooping cough vaccine in the debriefing sheet and directed to the National Health Service website for further information about the vaccine.
All analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 24; IBM Corp Armonk, NY: 2016. The online survey was designed so that almost all questions demanded a response from participants. Data were missing for 4% of participants for one variable; missing cases were excluded from analyses involving that variable.
Negatively phrased items were reversed, and summed scores were created for each TPB component, with higher scores indicating more positive attitudes, subjective norms, PBC and intentions towards vaccination. Cronbach’s alpha for each scale were acceptable (attitudes: α = 0.93; subjective norms: α = 0.82; PBC: α = 0.71; intentions: α = 0.98). The data violated assumptions of normality, even if transformed, so Kruskal–Wallis non-parametric analysis of variance was used to explore TBC scores across the three experimental groups. Data were also stratified to test for between-group differences, including education level (degree vs. no degree), previous children (yes vs. no), and already received whooping cough vaccine (yes vs. no).
Among unvaccinated participants, multiple regression analyses were used to determine whether attitudes, PBC and subjective norms predicted intentions to receive the vaccine. Finally, logistic regressions were conducted to assess whether the four TPB constructs; PBC, subjective norms, attitudes, and intentions were significant predictors of vaccine history across all participants. The p-value for all analyses was set at a significance level of <0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

In total, 414 participants responded to the study invitation and 292 met all eligibility criteria. Participants who failed the attention checks (n = 10) were excluded from further analyses, resulting in a final sample size of 282 participants. Ninety-seven participants were randomly assigned to the disease risk condition, 98 to the myth busting condition and 87 to the control condition. The participants’ mean age was 31 years (SD = 5.13, range: 18–44 years). The majority self-identified as White British (82%), were educated to a university degree level or higher (72%), were employed (80%), married (67%), and had no children (63%). The participants’ mean gestational age was 28 weeks (SD = 7.08, range: 16–42 weeks). Almost all participants were aware of the whooping cough vaccine prior to the study (97%) but a smaller proportion had received the vaccine (55%). Please see Table 1 for all sample characteristics. Most participants reported positive attitudes and PBC to vaccination. Participants were more likely to report that they are influenced (a great deal/ a lot) by what their doctor thinks they should do (56%), as opposed to their parents (14%) or their female friends (13%). Summary descriptives for the TPB variables are presented in Table 2.

3.2. Main Effects of Message Framing

It was hypothesised that individuals in the disease risk condition would report significantly higher scores for each of the TPB scales than participants in the myth busting or control conditions, indicating more positive attitudes, subjective norms, PBC, and intentions. A series of Kruskal–Wallis H tests were conducted to test this hypothesis. Vaccinated participants’ intentions scores were excluded from this analysis. No statistically significant differences were found across the three conditions in attitudes (χ2 (2) = 3.57, p = 0.17), PBC (χ2 (2) = 0.11, p = 0.95), subjective norms (χ2 (2) = 0.36, p = 0.83), or intentions (χ2 (2) = 1.16, p = 0.56; Table 3). Data were stratified to test for between-group differences, including education level (degree vs. no degree), previous children (yes vs. no), and already vaccinated (yes vs. no). All between group differences remained non-significant. Mean intention scores were very high across all three conditions, with more than 80% of participants reporting that they were ‘very likely’ to receive the vaccine—Agree strongly/Agree moderately/Agree a little.

3.3. Determinants of Intention to Receive the Whooping Cough Vaccine

A multiple regression was calculated to determine the influence of PBC, attitudes and subjective norms on intention to receive the whooping cough vaccine among participants who reported they had not already received the vaccine (n = 123). A significant regression equation was found; F (3119) = 235.98, p < 0.001, with an R2 of 0.856. Attitudes (Beta = 0.699, p < 0.001) and subjective norms (Beta = 0.262, p < 0.001) significantly predicted intention to vaccinate but PBC did not (Beta = 0.014, p > 0.05).

3.4. Determinants of Being Vaccinated against Whooping Cough

A multivariate logistic regression was conducted to assess whether the TPB constructs were significant predictors of having been vaccinated against whooping cough (n = 159) versus not being vaccinated (n = 123). The model which included PBC, attitudes, intentions and subjective norms was significant (χ2 (4) = 87.45, p < 0.001) and a good fit for the data (Hosmer and Lemeshow test; χ2 (8) = 4.96, p = 0.76). It correctly classified 72% of cases and explained approximately 36% of the variance (Nagelkerke R2 = 0.357). Both intention (odds ratio (OR) = 1.67, confidence interval (CI) = 1.17–2.39) and PBC (OR = 1.25, CI = 1.06–1.47) were significantly associated with being vaccinated. However, in contrast to determinants of intention to receive the vaccine, attitudes (OR = 0.93, CI = 0.80–1.08) and subjective norms (OR = 0.96, CI = 0.90–1.04) were not predictive of being vaccinated against whooping cough.

4. Discussion

This study assessed the effects of message framing on vaccine hesitancy for the antenatal whooping cough vaccine. The primary aim of the study was to assess whether disease risk information better predicted vaccine acceptability than ‘myth busting’ information. The study also explored whether the TPB constructs had any explanatory utility for vaccine intentions and behaviour among a population of pregnant women.
Contrary to this study’s hypothesis, and previous findings [21,22,23], no significant effects of message framing on vaccine acceptability were found across the three experimental conditions. These findings may be explained by factors unique to the current study. Previous studies have reported found that framing effects may depend on pre-existing characteristics of the participants [19,38]. Almost all participants were aware of the vaccine prior to the study. Given that all respondents were at least 16 weeks pregnant, this may be as a result of a recent health care consultation or otherwise. Health care consultations have previously been found to have a significant effect on vaccine acceptability [11,39,40,41,42]. Thus, it is possible that brief framing messages, such as those examined in the current study, are ineffective in altering vaccine attitudes among informed message recipients. These findings raise important questions for future research to address, given that the majority of message framing studies have not assessed participants’ prior vaccine-related knowledge [19].
Second, the effects of framing may be moderated by whether the advocated health behaviour is vaccination of the message recipient (i.e., mother) or vaccination of an individual for whom the recipient is responsible (i.e., baby/child) [43]. The antenatal whooping cough vaccine presents a unique case, in which the message recipient is both receiving the vaccine and, by proxy, choosing for someone else to be vaccinated. The effects of loss-framed messages have also been found to differ across vaccine type [19].
Results remained non-significant after adjusting for education level (degree vs. no degree), offspring status (children vs. no children) and vaccine history (yes vs. no). These results were consistent with previous research, which found no effect of offspring status [21] or vaccine history [44] on the efficacy of loss-framed vaccine messages. There is a dearth of research available with which to compare the findings of this study concerning the effect of education level on the efficacy of framed vaccine messages. However, a systematic review reported that the effects of education level on vaccine uptake, attitudes, and knowledge differed across and within countries, and thus, the authors concluded that education level cannot solely determine vaccine attitudes and behaviours [45].
The secondary aim of the study was to assess whether the TPB had any explanatory utility for vaccine intention and behaviour among pregnant women. The TPB was a good fit for the data and accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in both intention and behaviour. The TPB constructs predicted 86% of the variance in vaccine intentions. Subjective norms and attitudes emerged as significant predictors of vaccine intention, but PBC did not. Participants with more positive vaccine attitudes and those who perceived higher levels of approval from close family and friends showed stronger intentions to receive the antenatal vaccine. Consistent with previous meta-analyses [27,46,47] and other TPB vaccine research [32,33], the results suggest that the TPB presents an appropriate theoretical framework for interventions aimed at increasing antenatal vaccine intentions via specific proximal psychological determinants; subjective norms and attitudes.
The four constructs of the TPB explained 36% of the overall variance in vaccine behaviour, with intention and PBC emerging as significant predictors of behaviour. This figure compares favourably with previous meta-analyses, which found that on average the TPB accounted for 27% of the variance in behaviour [27] and 19.3% of the variance in health-related behaviours [47]. These findings are encouraging; however, it must be highlighted that the TPB was better at predicting vaccine intentions than behaviour, the latter of which is of more interest for designing public health interventions. Furthermore, a considerable proportion of vaccine behaviour remains unaccounted for using the TPB constructs. Thus, although the current study offered preliminary evidence that the TPB constructs may be targeted to increase antenatal vaccine uptake, further research is needed to identify other contributing variables [28]. For example, vaccines are often considered an emotional decision for pregnant women [48], which the TPB does not account for.
Almost all of the participants (97%) were aware of the whooping cough vaccine prior to the study. The National Health Service has made this cohort a priority for public health action [49] and the responses in this study may reflect such public health campaign efforts. Nevertheless, awareness of the vaccine was markedly higher in this sample than relatively recent English study samples (e.g., 51.3%) [15], which were of similar biological and gestational age. The proportion of highly informed, educated, White British and married women was overrepresented in the current sample, reflecting a narrow range of demographics and hence, the responses may not be representative of the pregnant population in England. Previous research examining uptake for the MMR vaccine in the UK has found that parents who continue to reject vaccination for their child are from more advantaged backgrounds [50]. Thus, given that the sample in this study included pre-dominantly women from socio-economically advantaged backgrounds, who may be less susceptible to the effects of message framing, the efficacy of message framing on vaccine hesitancy should be explored in other populations.
Mean intention scores were very high across all three conditions, with more than 80% of participants reporting that they were ‘very likely’ to receive the vaccine. A considerably smaller proportion of participants reported having already received the vaccine (55%). Pregnant women may receive the vaccine from 16 weeks up until the time of delivery, but they are encouraged to do so between 20 and 32 weeks, usually after the foetal anomaly scan. Of those who had not yet received the vaccine (123/282 participants), more than half were at or had surpassed the 32-week mark (56%), thereby raising uncertainty of whether they will receive the vaccine.
This study was limited by several noteworthy factors which should be addressed in future research to test the robustness of the findings. First, although participants from the relevant target population were recruited, where the majority of studies in this field have recruited university students [19], the final sample represented a narrow range of demographics, limiting the generalisability of the findings. Participants were active online pregnancy support group members and other social media users who volunteered to partake in vaccine research after being offered only a small incentive to do so. The number of women who elected not to participate in the study could not be measured, and hence, both the response rate and differences between respondents and non-respondents could not be determined. Future studies may benefit from recruiting participants from a diverse range of online platforms or settings.
Second, this study was limited by the use of self-report measures rather than medical reports confirming vaccine receipt. The cross-sectional design of the study, used to reduce attrition, limited the ability of the study to determine the longer-term impact of framing messages. Participants who expressed intentions to receive the vaccine were not followed up to confirm receipt of vaccine. Cross-sectional studies are also not wholly appropriate designs to test the utility of the TPB as a model [51]. In the current study the TPB constructs were assessed for their ability to account for past rather than future behaviour. Finally, although an adequate overall sample size was achieved as per a priori power calculations, some participants had to be removed from the analyses because they had already been vaccinated. Hence, some of the results were underpowered. Whilst all participants were eligible at the time of participation to receive the vaccine (i.e., they were all at least 16 weeks pregnant), this resulted in many participants having already received the vaccine. Future researchers should determine participants’ vaccination status prior to recruitment to ensure that all analyses are sufficiently powered.

5. Conclusions

Although significant framing effects were not observed in this cohort, the study made several important contributions to the field. To the authors’ knowledge, this was the first study to examine the effects of message framing on acceptability for the whooping cough vaccine among pregnant women. The study found no evidence that disease risk information affects antenatal vaccine acceptability. The results provided preliminary evidence that constructs from the TPB may be suitable targets for interventions aimed at reducing antenatal vaccine hesitancy.
Further research is needed to determine effective strategies to increase informed uptake in pregnant women. This cohort is an accessible population and is regularly exposed to health care information through routine antenatal care [52]. Pregnant women are also candidates for an increasing number of vaccines [52]. Hence, effective strategies for increasing uptake of Boostrix-IPV may provide useful insights for other antenatal vaccines.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.R., L.A.V.M., and A.F.; methodology, M.R.; formal analysis, M.R. and L.A.V.M.; investigation, M.R.; data curation, M.R.; writing—original draft preparation, M.R.; writing—review and editing, M.R., L.A.V.M. and A.F.; visualization, M.R.; supervision, L.A.V.M. and A.F.; project administration, M.R.; funding acquisition, A.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Cancer Research UK, grant numbers: C49896/A17429 and C7492/A17219.

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank L.M. and A.F. for their supervision throughout this project.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A. Experimental Conditions

Table A1. Experimental group A: disease risk condition.
Table A1. Experimental group A: disease risk condition.
The Content for this Condition was Drawn and Adapted from an Online Blog, Published on www.parents.com [53].
Warning
You or your baby could catch whooping cough by being around someone who has this disease. Whooping cough spreads from person to person through the air.
The Boostrix-IPV vaccine for pregnant women can prevent against whooping cough. Most babies whose pregnant mothers get their Boostrix-IPV vaccine will not get this disease. Many more children will get whooping cough if we stop vaccinating.
Here is a true story that shows why vaccination is so important.
“Last July 4, when I was about to give birth, I came down with a cough. Five days later, our beautiful and healthy daughter, Caroline, was born. When Caroline was around 2 weeks old, she started developing a dry cough, especially following feedings. I called my paediatrician; Caroline tested positive for whooping cough. The doctors immediately asked if anyone around her had been sick with a cough. My heart sunk. I told them that I had. I had passed it on to Caroline. I felt guilty, knowing this had been preventable. There is nothing doctors can do for these babies, except be there to support them with oxygen. We spent a week hoping for the worst to pass-it’s a long road. Whooping cough is sometimes called the ‘100-day cough’.”
Thankfully, the baby recovered fully.
“After the worst was over, we were told that Caroline’s case was considered mild. That sounded crazy to me. Watching her heart rate drop and her face turn blue was beyond terrifying.”
Pregnant women should be vaccinated for whooping cough. This is a serious disease. Please take your time to read the description of whooping cough and carefully view the picture of a baby with whooping cough.
Whooping cough
Whooping cough is a highly infectious serious illness that causes uncontrolled, violent coughing fits. It can lead to weight loss, brain damage, pneumonia and some cases may result in death.
*picture of baby with whooping cough shown here
Table A2. Experimental group B: myth busting condition.
Table A2. Experimental group B: myth busting condition.
Please Take your Time to Read Some Information about Whooping Cough.
Pregnant women should be vaccinated for whooping cough. Boostrix-IPV, the vaccine for whooping cough is safe and effective.
Because some pregnant women worry about causing harm to themselves or their baby, they may worry about the safety of the vaccine. Vaccine safety experts, including experts at the Medicines and Healthcare products Regulatory Agency (MHRA) agree that the whooping cough vaccine is not related to risks in pregnancy or pregnancy outcomes.
Many scientific studies have found no link between vaccinating against whooping cough whilst pregnant and negative outcomes for the mother or baby. These studies include:
  • A 2014 study published in ‘The British Medical Journal’ examined the safety of the whooping cough vaccine in pregnant women in the UK. The study found no increased risk for women who were vaccinated in the third trimester for a list of adverse events, including still birth.
  • A 2007 paper published in ‘The Lancet’ journal reviewed the evidence for a maternal vaccine against whooping cough. The paper found no negative effects on the course of pregnancy, delivery, or the baby. No miscarriages as a result of the vaccine were reported.
  • An extensive review, including more than five years of reports of pregnant women to the ‘Vaccine Adverse Event Reporting System’ found no safety concerns for the maternal whooping cough vaccine.
Table A3. Experimental group C: Control group.
Table A3. Experimental group C: Control group.
The Content for This Condition was Drawn from
The Royal Society for the Protection of Birds [54].
You are eligible to take part in this study! Please take your time to read the information below.
Feeding birds in the garden is a popular activity–over half of adults in the UK feed birds in their garden. That’s a lot of extra help for the birds! It is a great way to bring the birds closer to you to admire their fascinating behaviour and wonderful colours. Feeding birds is also an ideal way to enthuse children about wildlife.
What food is best for birds?
Difference birds need different foods to sustain them. If you supply a variety of food you are more likely to attract many types of birds.
Household scraps such as pastry, cooked rice and breadcrumbs are enjoyed by many garden birds. Fruit, especially bruised apples and pears, will be popular with thrushes and blackbirds. When you are buying bird food, try to get a good mix of peanuts, seeds and live food such as mealworms and waxworms. Then you should have something to offer all the birds. Good ready-made mixes should contain sunflower seeds, broken peanuts, and flaked maize and smaller seeds such as millet.
Where is the best place to put a bird table in my garden?
Bird tables should be placed where the birds are safe and will be able to feed undisturbed. Avoid putting them near fences or thick hedges, where cats can easily get to them. If there is a small bush nearby, birds can use this as a look-out point to make sure it is safe.
And don’t forget to make sure it is visible from a window so you can enjoy watching the birds as they feed.

Appendix B

Table A4. TBC constructs, demographics questions, and background information.
Table A4. TBC constructs, demographics questions, and background information.
TBC Constructs, Demographics questions and Background Information.
Perceived Behavioural Control
For each of the below, please rate your response from “disagree strongly” to “agree strongly”, where (1 = disagree strongly; 2 = disagree moderately; 3 = disagree a little; 4 = neither disagree nor agree; 5 = agree a little; 6 = agree moderately; 7 = agree strongly)
If I wanted to, I could attend an appointment to get vaccinated against whooping cough
I feel confident in my ability to get vaccinated for whooping cough.
There are barriers in the way of me receiving the whooping cough vaccine.
Attitudes
For each of the below, please rate your response from “disagree strongly” to “agree strongly”, where (1 = disagree strongly; 2 = disagree moderately; 3 = disagree a little; 4 = neither disagree nor agree; 5 = agree a little; 6 = agree moderately; 7 = agree strongly)
Getting vaccinated for whooping cough will help protect my baby from whooping cough.
If I get vaccinated for whooping cough, I can reduce my baby’s risk of whooping cough.
Getting vaccinated for whooping cough will decrease my chances of getting whooping cough.
We just want to make sure you are paying attention. Select ‘disagree a little’ from the options provided.
Intentions
For each of the below, please rate your response from “very unlikely” to “very likely”, where (1 = very unlikely; 2 = somewhat unlikely; 3 = a little unlikely; 4 = neither unlikely nor likely; 5 = a little likely; 6 = somewhat likely; 7 = very likely)
During your pregnancy…
How likely is it that you’ll consider getting the whooping cough vaccine?
How likely is it that you’ll try to get the whooping cough vaccine?
How likely is it that you’ll actually get the whooping cough vaccine?
If a doctor offered you the whooping cough vaccine during your pregnancy, how likely is it that you would get vaccinated?
Subjective norms
For each of the below, please rate your response from “not at all” to “a great deal”, where (1 = not at all; 2 = a little; 3 = somewhat; 4 = a lot; 5 = a great deal)
How much do your parents think you should receive the whooping cough vaccine?
How much does your doctor think you should receive the whooping cough vaccine?
Are you paying attention? Please select ’somewhat’ from the options provided if you are.
How much does your best female friend think you should receive the whooping cough vaccine?
In general, I want to do what my parents think I should do.
In general, I want to do what my doctor thinks I should do.
In general, I want to do what my best female friend thinks I should do.
Finally, we have a couple of questions about you. Please select one answer.
What is your ethnicity?
White British
Other White
African
Caribbean
Other Black
Indian
Bengali
Pakistani
Other Asian
Mixed/multiple ethnic groups
Other ethnic group
What is the highest level of education that you have? (Please select). If your UK qualification is not listed, select the closest option. If you have qualifications from outside the UK, select the closest option.
No qualifications
GCSE, CSE, O Grade, Standard Grade, Access 3 Cluster, Intermediate 1 or 2, Senior Certificate or equivalent
A Level, AS Level, SCE Higher Grade, Higher, Advanced Higher, CSYS, Advanced Senior Certificate or equivalent
Other post-school but pre-Higher Education qualifications not already mentioned (including foreign qualifications), HNC, HND, SVQ level 4
Degree, Postgraduate qualifications, Masters, PhD, SVQ level 5 or equivalent; Professional qualifications (for example, teaching, nursing, accountancy); Other Higher Education qualifications not already mentioned (including foreign qualifications)
Are you employed? (Please select)
Yes
No
What is your current relationship status? (Please select)
Single
In a partnership
Married
Divorced
Widowed
Separated
How many weeks pregnant are you currently?
____________
How many children do you have? This does not include your current pregnancy.
________________
Before taking this survey, were you aware of the whooping cough vaccine available for pregnant women?
Yes
No
Have you already received the whooping cough vaccine during your pregnancy?
Yes
No
Questions in italics indicate attention checks.

References

  1. Zepp, F.; Heininger, U.; Mertsola, J.; Bernatowska, E.; Guiso, N.; Roord, J.; Tozzi, A.E.; Van Damme, P. Rationale for pertussis booster vaccination throughout life in Europe. Lancet Infect. Dis. 2011, 11, 557–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Billingsley, M. Pregnant women in UK are offered whooping cough vaccine to protect newborns. Br. Med. J. 2012, 345, e6594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Vizzotti, C.; Juarez, M.V.; Bergel, E.; Romanin, V.; Califano, G.; Sagradini, S.; Rancaño, C.; Aquino, A.; Libster, R.; Polack, F.P.; et al. Impact of a maternal immunization program against pertussis in a developing country. Vaccine 2016, 34, 6223–6228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. PHE. Research and Analysis. Laboratory Confirmed Cases of Pertussis Reported to the Enhanced Pertussis Surveillance Programme in England: Annual Report for 2013; Public Health England: London, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  5. PHE. Vaccination against Pertussis (Whooping Cough) for Pregnant Women. Information for Healthcare Professionals; Public Health England: London, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  6. PHE. Infection Report. Pertussis Vaccination Programme for Pregnant Women Update: Vaccine Coverage in England, October to December 2016; Public Health England: London, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  7. WHO. Global Vaccine Action Plan 2011–2020. 2013. Available online: https://www.who.int/immunization/global_vaccine_action_plan/GVAP_doc_2011_2020/en/ (accessed on 8 August 2017).
  8. PHE. Pertussis Vaccination Programme for Pregnant Women Update: Vaccine Coverage in England, January to March 2019 and 2018/19 Annual Coverage; Health Protection Report; Public Health England: London, UK, 2019; p. 4. [Google Scholar]
  9. Jarrett, C.; Wilson, R.; O’Leary, M.; Eckersberger, E.; Larson, H.J. Strategies for addressing vaccine hesitancy—A systematic review. Vaccine 2015, 33, 4180–4190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Harper, S.A.; Fukuda, K.; Uyeki, T.M.; Cox, N.J.; Bridges, C.B. Prevention and control of influenza. Recommendations of the Advisory Committee on Immunization Practices (ACIP). MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2005, 54, 1–40. [Google Scholar]
  11. Wilson, R.J.; Paterson, P.; Jarrett, C.; Larson, H.J. Understanding factors influencing vaccination acceptance during pregnancy globally: A literature review. Vaccine 2015, 33, 6420–6429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Bonville, C.A.; Cibula, D.A.; Domachowske, J.B.; Suryadevara, M. Vaccine attitudes and practices among obstetric providers in New York State following the recommendation for pertussis vaccination during pregnancy. Hum. Vaccin. Immunother. 2015, 11, 713–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Laenen, J.; Roelants, M.; Devlieger, R.; Vandermeulen, C. Influenza and pertussis vaccination coverage in pregnant women. Vaccine 2015, 33, 2125–2131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Varan, A.K.; Esteves-Jaramillo, A.; Richardson, V.; Esparza-Aguilar, M.; Cervantes-Powell, P.; Omer, S.B. Intention to accept Bordetella pertussis booster vaccine during pregnancy in Mexico City. Vaccine 2014, 32, 785–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Donaldson, B.; Jain, P.; Holder, B.S.; Lindsey, B.; Regan, L.; Kampmann, B. What determines uptake of pertussis vaccine in pregnancy? A cross sectional survey in an ethnically diverse population of pregnant women in London. Vaccine 2015, 33, 5822–5828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Winslade, C.G.; Heffernan, C.M.; Atchison, C.J. Experiences and perspectives of mothers of the pertussis vaccination programme in London. Public Health 2017, 146, 10–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Nyhan, B.; Reifler, J. Does correcting myths about the flu vaccine work? An experimental evaluation of the effects of corrective information. Vaccine 2015, 33, 459–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Lewandowsky, S.; Ecker, U.K.H.; Seifert, C.M.; Schwarz, N.; Cook, J. Misinformation and Its Correction:Continued Influence and Successful Debiasing. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 2012, 13, 106–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Penţa, M.A.; Băban, A. Message Framing in Vaccine Communication: A Systematic Review of Published Literature. Health Commun. 2018, 33, 299–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Kahneman, D.; Tversky, A. Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision Under Risk. Econometrica 1979, 47, 263–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Abhyankar, P.; O’Connor, D.B.; Lawton, R. The role of message framing in promoting MMR vaccination: Evidence of a loss-frame advantage. Psychol. Health Med. 2008, 13, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Gerend, M.A.; Shepherd, J.E.; Monday, K.A. Behavioral frequency moderates the effects of message framing on HPV vaccine acceptability. Ann. Behav. Med. 2008, 35, 221–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Horne, Z.; Powell, D.; Hummel, J.E.; Holyoak, K.J. Countering antivaccination attitudes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 10321–10324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Ajzen, I. From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In Action Control; Kuhl, J., Beckmann, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1985; pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
  25. Ajzen, I. Attitudes, Personality, and Behavior; Dorsey Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  26. Ajzen, I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Armitage, C.J.; Conner, M. Efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behaviour: A meta-analytic review. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2001, 40, 471–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Sheeran, P. Intention—Behavior Relations: A Conceptual and Empirical Review. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 2002, 12, 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Sniehotta, F.F.; Presseau, J.; Araújo-Soares, V. Time to retire the theory of planned behaviour. Health Psychol. Rev. 2014, 8, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Conner, M.; Godin, G.; Sheeran, P.; Germain, M. Some feelings are more important: Cognitive attitudes, affective attitudes, anticipated affect, and blood donation. Health Psychol. 2013, 32, 264–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Gerend, M.A.; Shepherd, J.E. Predicting human papillomavirus vaccine uptake in young adult women: Comparing the health belief model and theory of planned behavior. Ann. Behav. Med. 2012, 44, 171–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Juraskova, I.; O’Brien, M.; Mullan, B.; Bari, R.; Laidsaar-Powell, R.; McCaffery, K. HPV vaccination and the effect of information framing on intentions and behaviour: An application of the theory of planned behaviour and moral norm. Int. J. Behav. Med. 2012, 19, 518–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Liao, Q.; Cowling, B.J.; Lam, W.W.; Fielding, R. Factors affecting intention to receive and self-reported receipt of 2009 pandemic (H1N1) vaccine in Hong Kong: A longitudinal study. Plos One 2011, 6, e17713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. McLaughlin, G.H. SMOG grading: A new readability formula. J. Read. 1969, 12, 639–646. [Google Scholar]
  36. Gunning, R. Technique of Clear Writing; McGraw-Hill International Book Co.: New York, NY, USA, 1952. [Google Scholar]
  37. Flesch, R. A new readability yardstick. J. Appl. Psychol. 1948, 32, 221–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Frew, P.M.; Saint-Victor, D.S.; Owens, L.E.; Omer, S.B. Socioecological and message framing factors influencing maternal influenza immunization among minority women. Vaccine 2014, 32, 1736–1744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Strassberg, E.R.; Power, M.; Schulkin, J.; Stark, L.M.; Mackeen, A.D.; Murtough, K.L.; Paglia, M.J. Patient attitudes toward influenza and tetanus, diphtheria and acellular pertussis vaccination in pregnancy. Vaccine 2018, 36, 4548–4554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Psarris, A.; Sindos, M.; Theodora, M.; Antsaklis, P.; Pergialiotis, V.; Loutradis, D.; Daskalakis, G. Routine immunizations during pregnancy, doctors’ compliance and patient hesitancy: A two stage study on vaccination uptake. Eur. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Reprod. Biol. 2019, 243, 36–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Wong, V.W.Y.; Fong, D.Y.T.; Lok, K.Y.W.; Wong, J.Y.H.; Sing, C.; Choi, A.Y.-y.; Yuen, C.Y.S.; Tarrant, M. Brief education to promote maternal influenza vaccine uptake: A randomized controlled trial. Vaccine 2016, 34, 5243–5250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Hayles, E.H.; Cooper, S.C.; Wood, N.; Sinn, J.; Skinner, S.R. What predicts postpartum pertussis booster vaccination? A controlled intervention trial. Vaccine 2015, 33, 228–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. O’Keefe, D.J.; Jensen, J.D. The relative persuasiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages for encouraging disease prevention behaviors: A meta-analytic review. J. Health Commun. 2007, 12, 623–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Ferguson, E.; Gallagher, L. Message framing with respect to decisions about vaccination: The roles of frame valence, frame method and perceived risk. Br. J. Psychol. 2007, 98, 667–680. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Larson, H.J.; Jarrett, C.; Eckersberger, E.; Smith, D.M.D.; Paterson, P. Understanding vaccine hesitancy around vaccines and vaccination from a global perspective: A systematic review of published literature, 2007–2012. Vaccine 2014, 32, 2150–2159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Godin, G.; Kok, G. The theory of planned behavior: A review of its applications to health-related behaviors. Am. J. Health Promot. 1996, 11, 87–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. McEachan, R.R.C.; Conner, M.; Taylor, N.J.; Lawton, R.J. Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the Theory of Planned Behaviour: A meta-analysis. Health Psychol. Rev. 2011, 5, 97–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Tucker Edmonds, B.M.; Coleman, J.; Armstrong, K.; Shea, J.A. Risk perceptions, worry, or distrust: What drives pregnant women’s decisions to accept the H1N1 vaccine? Matern. Child Health J. 2011, 15, 1203–1209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Guidelines for the Public Health Management of Pertussis in England; Public Health England: London, UK, 2018. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/762766/Guidelines_for_the_Public_Health_management_of_Pertussis_in_England.pdf (accessed on 9 April 2020).
  50. Pearce, A.; Mindlin, M.; Cortina-Borja, M.; Bedford, H. Characteristics of 5-year-olds who catch-up with MMR: Findings from the UK Millennium Cohort Study. BMJ Open 2013, 3, e003152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Weinstein, N.D. Misleading tests of health behavior theories. Ann. Behav. Med. 2007, 33, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Chu, H.Y.; Englund, J.A. Maternal Immunization. Clin. Infect. Dis. 2014, 59, 560–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Brunch. My Wake-Up Call with Whooping Cough. One Mother Shares the Story of Her Baby’s Terrifying Bout with the Disease. Available online: http://www.parents.com/baby/health/cough/my-wake-up-call-with-whooping-cough/ (accessed on 8 August 2017).
  54. RSPB. Frequently Asked Questions about Feeding Birds. Available online: https://www.rspb.org.uk/birds-and-wildlife/advice/how-you-can-help-birds/feeding-birds/frequently-asked-questions/ (accessed on 8 August 2017).
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Disease Risk
(n = 97)
Myth Busting
(n = 98)
Control Condition
(n = 87)
Age (years)
18–2414611
25–34636551
35–44202725
Ethnicity
White British808270
Other White131010
Non White467
Education level
GCSE or below766
A Level or equivalent1996
College qualification5511
Degree or higher667864
Employment status
Employed748471
Unemployed231416
Relationship status
Single534
In a partnership322618
Married606965
Previous children
No536560
Yes443327
Gestational age (wks)
16–24294027
25–34413239
35+232219
Vaccine awareness
Yes949684
No323
Received vaccine
Yes595545
No384342
Table 2. Summary descriptives for each Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) variable.
Table 2. Summary descriptives for each Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) variable.
ItemsOverallCurrently VaccinatedCurrently Unvaccinated
n (%)n (%)n (%)
Perceived behavioural control a
If I wanted to, I could attend an appointment to get vaccinated against whooping cough.264 (93.6)158 (99.4)106 (86.2)
I feel confident in my ability to get vaccinated for whooping cough.259 (91.8)159 (100)100 (81.3)
There are (no) barriers in the way of me receiving the whooping cough vaccine.235 (83.3)142 (89.3)93 (75.6)
Attitudes a
Getting vaccinated for whooping cough will help protect my baby from whooping cough.245 (86.9)157 (98.7)88 (71.5)
If I get vaccinated for whooping cough, I can reduce my baby’s risk of whooping cough.249 (88.3) 158 (99.4)91 (74.0)
Getting vaccinated for whooping cough will decrease my chances of getting whooping cough.216 (76.6)137 (86.2)79 (64.2)
Subjective norms b
How much do your parents think you should receive the whooping cough vaccine?156 (55.3)100 (62.9)56 (45.5)
How much does your doctor think you should receive the whooping cough vaccine?232 (82.3)146 (91.8)86 (69.9)
How much does your best female friend think you should receive the whooping cough vaccine?173 (61.3)109 (68.6)64 (52.0)
In general, I want to do what my parents think I should do.39 (13.8)25 (15.7)14 (11.4)
In general, I want to do what my doctor thinks I should do.158 (56.0)106 (66.7)52 (42.3)
In general, I want to do what my best female friend thinks I should do.37 (13.1)22 (13.8)15 (12.2)
a Agree strongly/ Agree moderately/ Agree a little; b A great deal/A lot.
Table 3. Mean Scores for the Theory of Planned Behaviour variables across the three intervention conditions (n = 282).
Table 3. Mean Scores for the Theory of Planned Behaviour variables across the three intervention conditions (n = 282).
Disease Risk
(n = 97)
Myth Busting
(n = 98)
Control
(n = 87)
Kruskal-Wallis
RangeMeanSDMeanSDMeanSDp-value
Attitudes3–2118.415.0118.364.7217.555.360.17
Subjective norms6–3014.135.2713.984.8913.645.260.83
Perceived Behavioural Control3–2119.233.1819.083.3819.392.850.95
Intentions4–2820.4210.6722.149.6620.2410.670.56

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ryan, M.; Marlow, L.A.V.; Forster, A. Countering Vaccine Hesitancy among Pregnant Women in England: The Case of Boostrix-IPV. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4984. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17144984

AMA Style

Ryan M, Marlow LAV, Forster A. Countering Vaccine Hesitancy among Pregnant Women in England: The Case of Boostrix-IPV. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(14):4984. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17144984

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ryan, Mairead, Laura A V Marlow, and Alice Forster. 2020. "Countering Vaccine Hesitancy among Pregnant Women in England: The Case of Boostrix-IPV" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 14: 4984. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17144984

APA Style

Ryan, M., Marlow, L. A. V., & Forster, A. (2020). Countering Vaccine Hesitancy among Pregnant Women in England: The Case of Boostrix-IPV. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(14), 4984. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17144984

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop